The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study

Similar documents
A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control

Parametric manipulation of conflict and response competition using rapid mixed-trial event-related fmri

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Chantal E. Stern,*, Adrian M. Owen, Irene Tracey,*, Rodney B. Look,* Bruce R. Rosen,* and Michael Petrides

Goal-directed human cognitive processes can be characterized

Reproducibility of fmri Results across Four Institutions Using a Spatial Working Memory Task

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe

Functional magnetic resonance imaging: basic principles of and

Supporting Information

The frontal cortex comprises a third of

Contribution of Working Memory to Transient Activation in Human Inferior Prefrontal Cortex during Performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval

performance of verbal working memory tasks

Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: who is at risk?

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human prefrontal cortex

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Differences in brain structure and function between the sexes has been a topic of

Supplementary Information

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Functional Anatomic Study of Episodic Retrieval

ORIGINAL ARTICLES Abnormal Prefrontal Cortex Function during Response Inhibition in Turner Syndrome: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Motivational influences on cognitive control: Behavior, brain activation, and individual differences

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Effects of Focal Frontal Lesions on Response Inhibition

Presupplementary Motor Area Activation during Sequence Learning Reflects Visuo-Motor Association

Functional specializations in lateral prefrontal cortex associated with the integration and segregation of information in working memory

Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

An f MRI study of facial emotion processing in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome.

Supplementary Online Content

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

Cingulate Hypoactivity in Cocaine Users During a GO NOGO Task as Revealed by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fmri

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Dissociation of Verbal Working Memory System Components Using a Delayed Serial Recall Task

The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: Effects of memory load and individual differences

Human memory has been regarded as an associative network

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Event-Related fmri of Tasks Involving Brief Motion

Behavioural Brain Research

Hemodynamic responses in visual, motor, and somatosensory cortices in schizophrenia

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1.

Visualization strategies for major white matter tracts identified by diffusion tensor imaging for intraoperative use

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the implicit association of concepts and attributes

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming

ARTICLE IN PRESS. St. Louis, MO 63130, USA d Department of Radiology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA

Differential Prefrontal Cortex Activation During Inhibitory Control in Adolescents With and Without Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Preparation for integration: The role of anterior prefrontal cortex in working memory

HUMAN SOCIAL INTERACTION RESEARCH PROPOSAL C8CSNR

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different

Dissociation of Mnemonic and Perceptual Processes During Spatial and Nonspatial Working Memory Using fmri

Flexible adaptation to changing environments is one of the

positron-emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes

Selective Tuning of the Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Response during Simple Target Detection Dissociates Human Frontoparietal Subregions

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

Errors of mathematical processing: The relationship of accuracy to neural regions associated with retrieval or representation of the problem state

Event-Related Activation in the Human Amygdala Associates with Later Memory for Individual Emotional Experience

Training-Induced Functional Activation Changes in Dual-Task Processing: An fmri Study

The Critical Relationship between the Timing of Stimulus Presentation and Data Acquisition in Blocked Designs with fmri

Contributions of the hippocampus and the striatum to simple association and frequency-based learning

Experimental design. Experimental design. Experimental design. Guido van Wingen Department of Psychiatry Academic Medical Center

T* 2 Dependence of Low Frequency Functional Connectivity

Data Analysis. Memory and Awareness in Fear Conditioning. Delay vs. Trace Conditioning. Discrimination and Reversal. Complex Discriminations

AN fmri EXAMINATION OF VISUAL INTEGRATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation

Prediction of Successful Memory Encoding from fmri Data

The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict

Supporting Online Material for

Working memory (WM) is a system for the temporary

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Transcription:

NeuroImage 16, 449 453 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1074, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fmri Study S. Durston,*, K. M. Thomas,* M. S. Worden,* Y. Yang, and B. J. Casey* *Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, New York; Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, The Netherlands; and Department of Psychiatry, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, New York Received May 22, 2001 In this study we combined event-related fmri with a parametric manipulation of the go nogo paradigm to examine the effect of preceding context on inhibitory processes. Nogo trials were preceded by either 1, 3, or 5 go trials and then compared to one another. Two distinct patterns of activation were associated with behavioral inhibition: First, the ventral prefrontal cortex, cingulate gyrus, and superior parietal regions showed a context effect with an increase in MR signal to nogo trials with increasing number of preceding go trials. Second, anterior regions in the supplementary and premotor cortex showed an increase in MR signal on the nogo condition after 5 preceding go trials, but not after only 1 or 3. A model using the BOLD response in our data was used to verify that the effect of context was not an artifact of the randomization scheme used in the design. 2002 Elsevier Science (USA) Key Words: functional MRI; event-related; inhibition; context. Ventral prefrontal cortex has been implicated in behavioral inhibition, based on animal, clinical, and neuroimaging studies. Studies in nonhuman primates have demonstrated that lesions in ventral prefrontal cortex result in deficits in behavioral inhibition (Butters et al., 1973; Iversen and Miskin, 1970), as have studies of patients with lesions in this region (Godefroy and Rousseaux 1996). Numerous imaging studies have shown that these areas are involved in response inhibition, using such paradigms as the go nogo and stop signal task (e.g., Casey et al., 1997; Garavan et al., 1999; Konishi et al., 1999; Liddle et al., 2001; Rubia et al., 2000). More evidence that ventral prefrontal cortex is directly related to response inhibition was provided by Casey et al. (1997), who demonstrated that the volume of activity in this region correlated with correct performance on a go nogo task. However, imaging studies have shown that other areas are also activated during response inhibition, such as dorsolateral frontal cortex and regions of the anterior cingulate gyrus (e.g., Casey et al., 1997; Garavan et al., 1999; Konishi et al., 1999; Liddle et al., 2001). It has been suggested that these additional regions may not be involved in response inhibition per se, but reflect cognitive processes, related to other aspects of the task (Garavan et al., 1999), such as maintaining other task relevant information, or monitoring conflict, respectively. In the current study, we examined the effect of preceding context on inhibition by using a parametric manipulation: We varied the number of response trials preceding an inhibition trial in a variation of the go nogo paradigm. Areas that are critical for response inhibition should be activated at all levels of our manipulation, whereas noncritical areas may not be. One possible hypothesis is that the manipulation of context results in increasing task or response demands that will simply lead to more activation in the regions involved in the inhibition of a response. Alternatively, other brain regions, not typically reported in studies of response inhibition but implicated in maintaining context could be involved (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal regions). An advantage of a parametric manipulation such as the one described is that it will allow the comparison of groups, across different levels of task difficulty, if behavioral performance is indeed influenced by context. Previous developmental and clinical studies using the go nogo task have not controlled for differences in behavioral performance across tasks, which may be related to the differences in the pattern of activation reported between groups. Parametric manipulations have most often been used when testing for individual or group differences in more traditional working memory tasks (e.g., the n-back task Braver et al., 1997; Thomas et al., 1999) and not in inhibitory tasks. In order to manipulate preceding context, we modified the go nogo task (e.g., Casey et al., 1997). We parametrically manipulated the number of targets preceding every nontarget, so that either 1, 3, or 5 targets always preceded a nogo trial and maintained an overall target frequency of 75% across the entire experiment. 449 1053-8119/02 $35.00 2002 Elsevier Science (USA) All rights reserved.

450 DURSTON ET AL. We used a rapid mixed trial event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) design to link MR signal change to each individual trial type. The use of an event-related design, combined with the parametric manipulation of preceding context allows for the comparison of nogo trials to one another, rather than just comparing nogo trials to go trials. This manipulation allows one to tease apart systems involved in the representation of when not to respond rather than in the behavioral process of making a response or not. As we anticipated that the 12- to 16-s interval traditionally used in event-related designs would be too long to produce the desired behavioral inhibition (and too boring to ultimately use with children), we used a rapid mixed trial design for this fmri study (Dale and Buckner, 1997). METHODS Paradigm. The subject s task was to press a button in response to visually presented stimuli, but to avoid responding to a rare nontarget. The task consisted of five runs, which lasted 3 min and 24 s each. Each run contained a total of 49 trials, with 75% go trials, resulting in a total of 60 nogo trials, 20 of each type (with 1, 3, or 5 preceding go trials). The order of presentation of the different types of nogo trials was pseudorandomized. In order to make this paradigm better suitable for use with children at a later date characters from the Pokemon cartoon series were used as stimuli. Stimulus duration was 500 ms. The interstimulus interval was 4s. 1 Subjects. Ten healthy right-handed adult subjects (mean age 28.0 years, 5 male) were run in two versions of the go nogo paradigm. 1 Subjects were screened for any contraindications for an MRI. We obtained written consent from all subjects prior to scanning. Scan acquisition. EPI BOLD images were acquired in 24 axial slices on a 1.5 T GE Signa scanner, covering most of the brain (TR 2000, TE 40, 64 64, 4-mm slice thickness, 3.125 3.125-mm in-plane resolution). 1 We performed a comparison of two versions of our task: (1) One with a fixed interstimulus interval (ISI) of 4 s; and (2) one with a jittered ISI, which varied between 3 and 5 s with a mean of 4 s. The results were very similar for both versions: Accuracy and reaction time were comparable, and both paradigms produced similar patterns of activation. However, activation was more robust for the fixed than the jittered design. Therefore, the results reported in this paper are those for the fixed design. Differences in robustness are hypothesized to be related to off-peak sampling in the jittered presentation, resulting in reduced power. Alternatively, the regular, predictable presentation rate associated with the fixed design (stimulus appears every 4 s) may result in a more compelling behavioral set, making inhibition harder to accomplish, and resulting in a more robust activation in the areas associated with performance of this task. Anatomical Spin Echo images were also collected (TR 500, TE min, 256 256, FOV 20, 4-mm slice thickness) in the same locations as the functional slices. Stimuli were presented using the integrated functional imaging system (IFIS) (PST, Pittsburgh), which uses a LCD video display in the bore of the MR scanner and a fiberoptic response collection device. Analysis. AIR (Automated Image Registration) version 3.08 (Woods et al., 1992) was used for motion correction, image smoothing (2 mm), and cross registration of data. A voxelwise, multifactorial ANOVA was performed pooling data into a 10 (subjects) 4 (trial type - go trials and nogo trials preceded by 1, 3, or 5 go trials) design, averaging across all 5 runs. Two scans were included in the ANOVA for each trial, at 4 and 6 s after stimulus presentation. Regions of three or more contiguous voxels (P 0.005) were identified (Forman et al., 1995). Only correct trials were analyzed. Images were warped into stereotaxic space using AFNI (Cox, 1996) to localize regions of activity, based on the coordinate system of the Talairach atlas (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988). RESULTS Behavioral. None of the subjects were explicitly aware that the nontarget always followed 1, 3, or 5 targets (i.e., never 2 or 4) based on self-report at the end of the study. Average accuracy on the nogo trials was 95.5%. Average reaction time on go trials was 559 ms. The number of errors on the nogo trials increased with number of preceding go trials (2.5%, 5%, 6%, P 0.05). FMRI. Three general patterns of change in MR signal were observed (see Table 1 and Fig. 1). Two of these patterns reflected the contextual manipulation. In general, the MR signal to nogo trials increased with number of preceding go trials. Areas in the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 9/44 and 44/46), cingulate gyrus (both anterior and posterior) (BA 32 and 23/31) and superior parietal cortex (BA 7) showed an increase in MR signal after three preceding go trials, whereas regions in the superior and middle frontal gyri (BA 6/8) did not show an increase in MR signal until after five preceding go trials. Primary motor cortex (BA 4) showed a decrease in activation for all three nogo conditions compared to go trials. MODELING The behavioral increase in the number of errors on nogo trials with increasing number of preceding go trials confirms our assumption that inhibition becomes more difficult as the demands on response inhibition increase (i.e., inhibiting after having made more responses consecutively). Several regions showed an in-

THE EFFECT OF PRECEDING CONTEXT ON INHIBITION 451 TABLE 1 Patterns of Activation for the Comparison of Go Trials to Nogo Trials, Preceded by 1, 3, or 5 Go Trials Pattern in figure 2 Area Brodmann Side Talairach Max F A Inferior frontal gyrus 9/44 Right (36, 23, 28) 7.51 44/46 Left ( 30, 25, 23) 13.37 Anterior cingulate gyrus 32 Right (7, 37, 29) 9.01 Posterior cingulate gyrus 23/31 Right (8, 39, 26) 10.07 23/31 Left ( 11, 44, 25) 7.30 Superior parietal lobule 7 Left ( 17, 67, 41) 7.02 B Superior frontal gyrus 6 Right (9, 6, 57) 8.78 8 Left ( 11, 37, 45) 8.18 Middle frontal gyrus 6/8 Left ( 42, 0, 44) 7.96 C Primary motor 4 Left ( 42, 28, 57) 12.29 Note. Patterns correspond to Fig. 1. crease in MR signal as a function of increasing number of preceding targets (i.e., the effect of context). These regions may be recruited more as interference in the task demands increase (i.e., withholding a response after having made several responses). We wanted to exclude the possibility that this increase in MR signal might be based on an artifactual summation of blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) responses due to our randomization scheme. In order to check this, we modeled our randomization scheme using the model of hemodynamic response function proposed by Boynton et al. (1996): h t t/ n 1 e t/ / n 1! Modeling methods. In order to fit the model to our data, we chose a total of 14 regions of interest (ROI) from our dataset that showed an increase in MR signal to nogo trials relative to go trials. We plotted the BOLD response for these regions to all three types of nogo trials (see Fig. 2). We fit the model to the various BOLD curves to empirically determine the values of n and for each BOLD curve separately. The model was then combined with the randomization scheme, using the empirically determined values of n and. This resulted in models for the average BOLD response to a nogo trial after 1, 3, or 5 preceding go trials in all 14 regions. As the results were comparable for all regions, the values FIG. 1. (A) Brain regions showing the context effect; (B) Brain regions showing an increase in activation only to nogo trials following 5 go trials. (C) Brain regions showing a response to the go trials. (D) Three patterns observed for the comparison of go trials to nogo trials after 1, 3, or 5 preceding go trials.

452 DURSTON ET AL. FIG. 2. Example of a BOLD response, plotted for a region of interest that responded to context. from the overall average BOLD curve are further reported here. Modeling results. Under the null hypothesis that there was no effect of context (i.e., the amplitude of the BOLD curve following a nogo was the same for all three levels of the nogo), the model predicted no artifactual effect of context for either design. DISCUSSION Overall, this study demonstrated an effect of preceding context on behavior and pattern of brain activity. The number of errors made by subjects to nogo trials increased as a function of the number of preceding go trials. Similarly, brain regions implicated in response inhibition showed increased activity as the number of preceding go trials increased. Brain regions traditionally linked to manipulations of context, memory load, or task demands, such as dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Braver et al., 1997) were not activated by the context manipulation, rather the brain regions included inferior frontal cortex and the anterior cingulate gyrus. Two distinct patterns of activation were observed for regions that responded to the context manipulation (see Table 1 and Fig. 2). The ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9/44 and 44/46), the cingulate gyrus (BA 32 and 23/31) and superior parietal cortex (BA 7) increased their activation to nogo trials as a function of the number of preceding go trials. These regions may be maintaining the task or response demands as interference from the go trials increases, in a similar manner to that suggested for dorsolateral prefrontal regions (Braver et al., 1997; Thomas et al., 1999). Activity in anterior supplementary and premotor cortex increased only to nogo trials that followed 5 go trials, but not fewer. At least one imaging study using a go nogo paradigm has reported activation of premotor areas to nogo trials. That event-related fmri study (using 18-s intervals) reported activity in anterior premotor areas to nogo trials (Humberstone et al., 1997). Single cell recording studies have also described cells in premotor cortex that respond to withholding a response (Watanbe, 1986). One possible explanation for activity in this region is that it reflects preparation for a motor response prior to stimulus onset. Perhaps after a certain number of responses or threshold, the system is primed to make a motor response as evidenced by the activity in these premotor regions that is then overridden when a nontarget appears. In this paradigm, subjects had 4 seconds between trials, allowing them plenty of time to prepare for the next event, particularly if the expectation for or probability of a target/ response was high. Similar patterns of activity and interpretations of premotor activity have been reported during working memory tasks, suggesting either preparation of a motor response or practice effect (Braver et al., 1997; Petersen et al., 1998; Thomas et al., 1999). Either of these interpretations is consistent with the current findings. Overall, our results show the feasibility of using context manipulations to vary task difficulty in an inhibitory task. As such, this task may be used when comparing groups with different levels of accuracy on the go nogo task as it will allow the comparison of subjects at the same level of performance, but different level of task demand. This manipulation will prove critical in studies comparing age groups and psychiatric conditions (e.g., ADHD). It should be noted that at least two other studies have manipulated target frequency in a go nogo task (DeZubicaray et al., 2000; Casey et al., 2001). However, these tasks used blocked designs in acquisition or analysis of the data. Both studies report a distributed network of regions, including regions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, that show linear increases as a function of number of targets and nontargets. Since these studies used block designs where the probability of go trials was manipulated per block, varying between 10 and 100%, the signal change associated with a single nogo trial could not be examined as blocked designs do not allow the isolation of the trial of interest (i.e., the nogo), but rather involve collapsing across trial types. Furthermore error trials cannot be easily excluded from these designs. In sum, we have demonstrated both a behavioral and biological effect of preceding context on inhibition using a version of a traditional inhibitory task. We replicated previous work demonstrating the importance of the ventral prefrontal cortex in performance of this task and implicated other regions as well (cingulate and parietal cortex). All of these regions were sensitive to preceding context, but more dorsolateral prefrontal regions often described as critical for maintaining context were not sensitive to this manipulation. This finding suggests that dorsal and ventral prefrontal cortex may both be sensitive to preceding context, but differ

THE EFFECT OF PRECEDING CONTEXT ON INHIBITION 453 according to the contextual information maintained. In the current study, the relevant behavioral response was perhaps more important than the stimulus information that is typically manipulated in tasks implicating more dorsal prefrontal regions. Finally, our behavioral task and experimental design manipulations have implications for future studies. We showed that manipulating the number of preceding targets affected task difficulty as indicated by overall accuracy. This finding suggests that parametric manipulations of preceding context may be a useful approach for titrating task difficulty when studying subjects of different ages or different clinical populations. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank D. Silbersweig and E. Stern for their consultation on designing the paradigm and B. van Berckel for his help in subject recruitment. This work was supported in part by an NIMH K01 award to B.J.C. and by awards from the Netherlands Royal Academy of the Arts and Sciences (KNAW), foundation De Drie Lichten and the Dutch Society for Neuropsychology to S.D. REFERENCES Boynton, G. M., Engel, S. A., Glover, G. H., and Heeger, D. J. 1996. Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J. Neurosci. 16(13): 4207 4221. Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., and Noll, D. C. 1997. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 5: 49 62. Butters, N., Butter, C., Rosen, J., and Stein, D. 1973. Behavioral effects of sequential and one-stage ablations of orbital prefrontal cortex in the monkey. Exp. Neurol. 39(2): 204 214. Casey, B. J., Forman, S. D., Franzen, P., Berkowitz, A., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Thomas, K. M., and Noll, D. C. 2001. Sensitivity of prefrontal cortex to changes in target probability: A functional MRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 13: 26 33. Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom, L. E., Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Haxby, J. V., Noll, D. C., Cohen, J. D., Forman, S. D., Dahl, R. E., and Rapoport, J. L. 1997. A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation during performance of a go-nogo task. J. Cogn. Neurol. 9(6): 835 847. Cox, R. W. 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance images. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29: 162 173. Dale, A. M. 1999. Optimal experimental design for event-related fmri. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8: 109 114. Dale, A. M., and Buckner, R. L. 1997. Selective averaging of rapidly presented individual trials using fmri. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5: 329 340. DeZubicaray, G. I., Andrew, C., Zelaya, F. O., Williams, F. C. R., and Dumanoir, C. 2000. Motor response suppression and the prepotent tendency to respond: A parametric fmri study. Neuropsychologia 38: 1280 1291. Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W. F., Mintun, M. A., and Noll, D. C. 1995. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri). Magn. Reson. Med. 33: 636 647. Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., and Stein, E. A. 1999. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96(14): 8301 8306. Godefroy O, and Rousseayx, M. 1996. Divided and focused attention in patients with lesions of the prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 30(2): 155 174. Humberstone, M., Sawle, G. V., Clare S., Hykin, J., Coxon, R., Bowtell, R., MacDonald, I. A., and Morris, P. G. 1997. Functional magnetric resonance imaging of single motor events reveals human presupplementary motor area. Ann. Neurol. 42(4): 632 637. Iversen, S. D., and Mishkin, M. 1970. Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective loss of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Exp. Brain Res. 11(4): 376 386. Kawashima, R., Satoh, K., Itoh, H., Ono, S., Furumoto, S., Gotoh, R., Koyama, M., Yoshioka, S., Takahashi, T., Takahashi, K., Yanagisawa, T., and Fukuda, H. 1996. Functional anatomy of GO/NO-GO discrimination and response selection A PET study in man. Brain Res. 728(1): 79 89. Klingberg, T., and Roland, P. E. 1997. Interference between two concurrent tasks is associated with activation of overlapping fields in the cortex. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 6(1): 1 8. Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Kikyo, H., Kameyama, M., and Miyashita, Y. 1999. Comon inhibitory mechanism in inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 122: 981 991. Liddle, P. F., Kiehl, K. A., and Smith, A. M. 2001. Event-related fmri study of response inhibition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 12(2): 100 109. Petersen, S. E., van Mier, H., Fiez, J. A., and Raichle, M. E. 1998. The effects of practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95(3): 853 860. Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C., Simmons, A., Andrew, C., and Bullmore, E. T. 2000. Functional frontalisation with age: Mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with age. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24(1): 13 19. Talairach, J., and Tournoux, P. 1988. Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Thieme, New York. Thomas, K. M., King, S. W., Franzen, P. L., Welsh, T. F., Berkowitz, A. L., Noll, D. C., Birmaher, V., and Casey, B. J. 1999. A developmental functional MRI study of spatial working memory. Neuroimage 10(3): 327 338. Watanabe. 1986. Prefrontal unit activity during delayed conditional Go/No-go discrimination in the monkey. II. Relation to Go and No-go responses. Brain Res. 382: 15 27. Woods, R. P., Cherry, S. R., and Mazziotta, J. C. 1992. Rapid automated algorithm for aligning and reslicing PET images. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 16: 620 633.