Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Similar documents
Preventing Postcompletion Errors: How Much Cue Is Enough?

Cognitive Strategies and Eye Movements for Searching Hierarchical Displays

Robot Learning Letter of Intent

Working Memory Load Affects Device-Specific but Not Task-Specific Error Rates

Voluntary Product Accessibility Template (VPAT)

Designing A User Study

Cognitive Modeling Demonstrates How People Use Anticipated Location Knowledge of Menu Items

Experimental Research in HCI. Alma Leora Culén University of Oslo, Department of Informatics, Design

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Empirical Research Methods for Human-Computer Interaction. I. Scott MacKenzie Steven J. Castellucci

Evaluating Tactile Feedback in Graphical User Interfaces

Topics. Experiment Terminology (Part 1)

Does It Know I m Not Maintaining Good Posture? : An In-Home Play Study of Wii Fit

Getting the Design Right Daniel Luna, Mackenzie Miller, Saloni Parikh, Ben Tebbs

The knowledge and skills involved in clinical audit

Avaya IP Office R9.1 Avaya one-x Portal Call Assistant Voluntary Product Accessibility Template (VPAT)

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Unified Modeling of Proactive Interference and Memorization Effort: A new mathematical perspective within ACT-R theory

Supporting the Mobile Notification Process through Tactile Cues Selected using a Paired Comparison Task

History of SETMA s Preparation for ICD-10 By James L. Holly, MD October 1, 2015

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Assessing Modes of Interaction

Development of the Web Users Self Efficacy scale (WUSE)

CANTAB Test descriptions by function

SMS USA PHASE ONE SMS USA BULLETIN BOARD FOCUS GROUP: MODERATOR S GUIDE

Information Design. Information Design

CS 544 Human Abilities

Thermo-Message: Exploring the Potential of Heat as a Modality of Peripheral Expression

Chapter 5: Research Language. Published Examples of Research Concepts

The Rough Guide to AAC! Essential ideas for success. Suzanne Martin Speech & Language Therapist: AAC Consultant ACE Centre

Sperling conducted experiments on An experiment was conducted by Sperling in the field of visual sensory memory.

Human Abilities: Vision, Memory and Cognition. Oct 14, 2016

MCC Human Machine lnterface

UIC Solutions Suite Webinar Series Transcript for how-to webinar on Action Planning for Prevention & Recovery Recorded by Jessica A.

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Ask the Expert Educator Edition

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Research Landscape. Qualitative = Constructivist approach. Quantitative = Positivist/post-positivist approach Mixed methods = Pragmatist approach

Getting Started.

Common Sense Assistant for Writing Stories that Teach Social Skills

TICK-MARKS, AXES, AND LABELS: THE EFFECTS OF ADDING CONTEXT TO AUDITORY GRAPHS

Unintended Effects: Varying Icon Spacing Changes Users Visual Search Strategy

Scottish Parliament Region: North East Scotland. Case : Tayside NHS Board. Summary of Investigation

THE ATTENTIONAL COSTS OF INTERRUPTING TASK PERFORMANCE AT VARIOUS STAGES

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016

Comment on McLeod and Hume, Overlapping Mental Operations in Serial Performance with Preview: Typing

Hanwell Instruments Ltd. Instruction Manual

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

IPM 12/13 T1.2 Limitations of the human perceptual system

Cognitive Modeling Reveals Menu Search is Both Random and Systematic

Further Investigations into Post-completion Error: the Effects of Interruption Position and Duration

smk72+ Handbook Prof. Dr. Andreas Frey Dr. Lars Balzer Stephan Spuhler smk72+ Handbook Page 1

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, in press

IAASB Main Agenda (September 2005) Page Agenda Item. Analysis of ISA 330 and Mapping Document

User Manual Verizon Wireless. All Rights Reserved. verizonwireless.com OM2260VW

An Activation-Based Model of Routine Sequence Errors

DRUG USE OF FRIENDS: A COMPARISON OF RESERVATION AND NON-RESERVATION INDIAN YOUTH

Intro to HCI / Why is Design Hard?

Psy201 Module 3 Study and Assignment Guide. Using Excel to Calculate Descriptive and Inferential Statistics

Double Your Weight Loss By Using A Journal

mehealth for ADHD Parent Manual

Girl Scout Western Oklahoma Adult Awards & Recognitions

Human Information Processing. CS160: User Interfaces John Canny

RELEASED. Clearing your active insulin

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

Lecture 18: Controlled experiments. April 14

Fleishman s Taxonomy of Human Abilities

Evaluation: Scientific Studies. Title Text

Avaya B159 Conference Telephone Voluntary Product Accessibility Template (VPAT)

Involving people with autism: a guide for public authorities

Avaya one-x Communicator for Mac OS X R2.0 Voluntary Product Accessibility Template (VPAT)

Human Information Processing

Objectives. Quantifying the quality of hypothesis tests. Type I and II errors. Power of a test. Cautions about significance tests

P.I.E.C.E.S. Dementia Care Series Approach September, 2011

How do Robotic Agents Appearances Affect People s Interpretations of the Agents Attitudes?

Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm

CS 102 Human-Computer Interaction Lecture 2: Cognition (1)

Go4orienteering: a revolution in orienteering exercises

Intelligent Object Group Selection

Two Experiments on Co-located Mobile Groupware

With Marie-Claire Ross The SELLSAFE Communication Mentor

Five Mistakes and Omissions That Increase Your Risk of Workplace Violence

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

Memory 2/15/2017. The Three Systems Model of Memory. Process by which one encodes, stores, and retrieves information

6/16/2011. Making Eye Gaze Technology a Reality: The Road from Trials to Conversations. My Background. Eye Gaze Technology

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

Lightened Dream. Quick Start Guide Lightened Dream is a dream journal designed to wake you up in your dreams.

INTERNATIONAL STANDARD ON ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS 3000 ASSURANCE ENGAGEMENTS OTHER THAN AUDITS OR REVIEWS OF HISTORICAL FINANCIAL INFORMATION CONTENTS

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Top Ten Tips for Supporting Communication

VISUAL PERCEPTION & COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Avaya G450 Branch Gateway, Release 7.1 Voluntary Product Accessibility Template (VPAT)

Coach Morse - Morse Code Practice Unit

DTI C TheUniversityof Georgia

SIM 16/17 T1.2 Limitations of the human perceptual system

Konftel 300Mx. Voluntary Product Accessibility Template (VPAT)

Further investigations into post-completion error: the effects of interruption position and duration

Transcription:

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI? Franklin P. Tamborello, II Phillip H. Chung Michael D. Byrne Rice University Department of Psychology 61 S. Main, MS-25 Houston, TX 775 {tambo, pchung, byrne}@rice.edu Abstract Most research on how people represent procedures suggests that control labels are central. However, our data suggest that even moderately-experienced users do not rely on labels to locate interface elements. Keywords label usage, interface layout ACM Classification Keywords H5.m. Information interfaces and presentation (e.g., HCI): GUI, Screen Design Introduction Most of us take for granted the importance of labeling controls on an interface. How else could the user know what each control does? Yet when one thinks about how rapidly a skilled user can perform a familiar task using a familiar interface, it hardly seems likely that he/ she is taking the time to read the labels on his/her controls. Copyright is held by the author/owner(s). CHI 28, April 5 April 1, 28, Florence, Italy ACM 1-978-1-6558-12-8/8/4. Surely there are plenty of good reasons for including control labels in an interface. For example, novices need to know what each control does, and expert users still need to be reminded what a seldom-used control does. But anticipating who uses what control under what circumstances is extremely difficult, if not

impossible. Given the above anecdotal evidence that users often disregard labels, it seems plausible that interface labels and other text messages should not be relied upon solely for very important communication from the machine to the user. We have found experimental evidence suggesting that experienced users of an interface rely very little upon textual labels to perform their tasks. Given that, it seems likely that they are instead using some kind of spatial representation to find the objects they need to perform their actions. Method To test our hypothesis that skilled users do not rely on labels for commonly-used controls, we wanted to use a task that 1) incorporates a sufficient training component to ensure that users are skilled at the task when data is collected from them, and 2) incorporates some task components other than reading, such as clicking buttons. Meeting those two design criteria ensures that the task satisfies the two major assumptions of our hypothesis, namely that users are sufficiently familiar with the task and with the interface that they need not search for controls and that there is something for them to do besides reading things. Furthermore, we wanted a contrasting condition that instead varied some spatial aspect of the interface. To this end we used one of the interactive Star Trekthemed computer tasks, the Phaser[1 4]. The Phaser uses a single-screen GUI composed of radio buttons, check boxes, buttons, and a few other elements (Figure 1). Twelve subjects (mean age 25.1 years, age standard deviation 4 years, 5 female) participated in the label removal condition, 18 in the added buttons condition (mean age 19.5 years, age standard deviation 1.7 years, 11 female). Participants trained for by reading an instruction manual, then performing the necessary steps once with the manual present as reference, then three times more without the manual. Only trials in which the user committed no errors were counted toward training criteria. Each participant returned approximately one week later for the test session. The test session simply consisted of performing the task with no assistance from the manuals or the experimenter. Participants performed 14 trials of the Phaser task, with the first four considered practice and therefore not included in analyses. Instructions at the beginning of the test phase noted that the interface may or may not change during the experiment and that the user would be warned of any interface changes. Participants completed the first seven trials of the Phaser using the control version of that task s interface (see Figure 1A). Before the onset of the eighth trial, the experiment displayed instructions warning subjects that the Phaser interface was about to change, but the instructions did not specify how the interface would change. The eighth and all subsequent trials of the Phaser all used the changed interface (see Figure 1B). The changed interface was exactly the same as the control interface with the exception that labels for all controls were replaced with series of X s. For the contrasting, spatial, condition, the change instead consisted of adding additional, superfluous, buttons to each button cluster, below the used buttons.

A concurrent working memory letter task was also introduced on the day of testing. As in previous studies [1 4], its function was to increase working memory load during task performance. Participants were presented with auditory stimuli in the form of randomly ordered letters spoken through the headphones at a rate of one letter every three seconds. A tone was presented randomly at intervals ranging from nine to 45 seconds, upon which the participants were directed to recall the last three letters in order and type them into the text box that appeared on the screen. The experiment gave additional motivation to the users to complete Phaser trials quickly and as best they could. Performance in both the Phaser and memory updating tasks were scored, and the three users who earned the top scores were awarded cash prizes. The two dependent variables were step completion times and error rates per step. However, some steps generated no meaningful completion times because of intervening events. For example, stop charging was clicked immediately after waiting for a thermometerstyle gauge to fill up to a point indicating the battery was charged. so the time for this step was not determined by the user s actions Results: Label Removal Removing labels had negligible impact on user performance of the Phaser task, as measured by error rate or click time, per step (see Figure 2); main effect (a) (b) figure 1: Phaser control (a) and labels removed (b) interfaces.

of interface change on error rate, F(1, 11) = 1.36, p =.27, interaction with step number, F(9, 99) = 1.67, p.11, click time main effect, F(1, 11) = 2.7, p =.13, interaction with step number, F(6, 66) = 1.78, p =.12. Results: Button Addition In contrast to removing labels, simply adding extraneous buttons did impact performance (Figure 3): change by step interaction on error, F (9, 153) = 3.99, p <.1. There was no similar effect on step completion times, indicating the effect on error rates was not simply a speed-accuracy trade-off. Discussion The lack of effect of label removal is not likely to simply be a matter of an ineffectual experimental paradigm, as the additional buttons condition illustrates. Nor does it (a) (b).25 35 3.2 25 Mean Error Frequency.15.1 Mean Step Completion Time (ms) 2 15 1.5 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 1 2 4 5 7 8 9 figure 2: Phaser error rates (a) and click times (b), labels crossed-out.

seem to be a lack of statistical power, given the results from the button addition condition. Why should adding objects to the interface impact performance, particularly when those objects are never used in the task? The reason(s) is not clear to us, but it seems plausible that the appearance of the extra objects could cause some kind of perceptual/motor interference. Furthermore, it seems striking that something so seemingly inconsequential as adding (a).25 buttons that do not even work would disrupt task performance more than removing all button labels. We therefore believe that users experienced in the use of a certain interface to perform a specific task generally do not read labels of control objects such as buttons,. We think this suggests a potential benefit that may be utilized: If experienced users do not read control labels, then substantial space within an interface may saved by simply leaving labels out or (b) 35 3.2 25 Mean Error Frequency.15.1 Mean Step Completion Time (ms) 2 15 1.5 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 1 2 4 5 7 8 9 figure 3: Phaser error rates (a) and click times (b), extraneous buttons added.

reducing their size when conditions allow it (obviously not for novice users). But if users do not read labels, how do they navigate an interface and know where each action needs to be performed? To answer that question, we are engaged in a program of research designed to map the spatial factors used in finding one s way around an interface. Preliminary data suggests that people may not simply memorize a global position of a control device within the entire display. They also seem to not rely upon landmarks within the interface that are not also controls themselves. We think experienced users of interfaces may be using some kind of hierarchical, relative representation of space that is closely coupled with their representations of action sequences. There seems to exist a real hole in user interface models like GOMS where factors like spatial representations come into play. GOMS, for example, makes predictions about how long it should take to read a label, and may make gross estimates of visual search latencies. But it says nothing about what to expect of human performance when you move buttons within a cluster and also move button clusters. Nor does it tell us how a human can navigate an interface without verbal labels. To this end, we are pursuing investigations designed to tell us something about how people represent space within an interface and what ramifications those representations may have for human performance, particularly of skilled tasks. In particular, we intend to take an integrated cognitive modeling approach so that we may say something substantive about the complex interaction of perceptual-motor, cognitive, task structure, and interface structure factors interacting when people perform some task like the Phaser. Acknowledgments This research was supported by the Office of Naval Research under grants #N14-3-1-94 and N14-6-1-56 to the third author. The views and conclusions expressed are those of the authors and should not be interpreted as representing the official policies or endorsements, either expressed or implied, of the ONR, the U.S. government, or any other organization. References [1] Byrne, M. D., & Bovair, S. (1997). A working memory model of a common procedural error. Cognitive Science, 21, 31-61. [2] Byrne, M. D., & Davis, E. M. (26). Task structure and postcompletion error in the execution of a routine procedure. Human Factors, 48, 627-638. [3] Chung, P. H. (26). Changing the Interface with Minimal Disruption: The Roles of Layout and Labels. Doctoral dissertation, Rice University, Houston, TX. [4] Chung, P. H., & Byrne, M. D. (in press). Cue effectiveness in mitigating postcompletion errors in a routine procedural task. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies.