Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures

Similar documents
International Trade and Finance Association. Cultural Materialism: Where East Meets West

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

Schwartz Values Clusters and Tourists Activities

A Short Form of Sweeney, Hausknecht and Soutar s Cognitive Dissonance Scale

National Culture Dimensions and Consumer Digital Piracy: A European Perspective

Deakin Research Online Deakin University s institutional research repository DDeakin Research Online Research Online This is the published version:

Validation of Intercultural Sensitivity Measure Individualism Collectivism

Culture and Survey Behavior

Validation of Intercultural Sensitivity Measure Individualism Collectivism

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Trust: The importance of distinguishing between different actors and dimensions

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

Chapter Two: Cross-Cultural Research Methods

The Self Group Distinction Scale: A new approach to measure individualism and collectivism in adolescents


Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications to cross-cultural comparisons of school culture

Emotion regulation and well-being among Puerto Ricans and European Americans

A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Perceived Fairness of Pay: The Importance of Task versus Maintenance Inputs in Japan, South Korea, and Hong Kong

Abstract. The current research examined differences in career decision-making profiles (CDMP)

Using Cultural Values as a Measure of Intercultural Sensitivity

SHORT NOTES I or we : Family socialization values in a national probability sample in Taiwan

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

LEADERSHIP ATTRIBUTES AND CULTURAL VALUES IN AUSTRALIA AND NEW ZEALAND COMPARED: AN INITIAL REPORT BASED ON GLOBE DATA

Considering residents level of emotional solidarity with visitors to a cultural festival

Decisions, Judgments, and Reasoning About Conflicts Between Friendship and Individualism in. Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood.

Measures of children s subjective well-being: Analysis of the potential for cross-cultural comparisons

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Professor Patrick Sturgis

Understanding Social Norms, Enjoyment, and the Moderating Effect of Gender on E-Commerce Adoption

Packianathan Chelladurai Troy University, Troy, Alabama, USA.

Modeling the Influential Factors of 8 th Grades Student s Mathematics Achievement in Malaysia by Using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)

Scope insensitivity in helping decisions: Is it a matter of culture and values?

College Student Self-Assessment Survey (CSSAS)

ASSESSING THE UNIDIMENSIONALITY, RELIABILITY, VALIDITY AND FITNESS OF INFLUENTIAL FACTORS OF 8 TH GRADES STUDENT S MATHEMATICS ACHIEVEMENT IN MALAYSIA

THE STRUCTURE AND MEASUREMENT OF SELF-CONSTRUALS: A CROSS- CULTURAL STUDY OF THE SELF-CONSTRUAL SCALE LILIA GABRIELA MIRAMONTES

CULTURE AND MENTAL REPRESENTATIONS OF POWER: IMPLICATIONS FOR CONSUMERS INFORMATION-PROCESSING STRATEGIES, JUDGMENTS, AND INFLUENCE ATTEMPTS

higher education in a changing world

Individualism, collectivism, and dissonance: a within-culture comparison

What is Multilevel Modelling Vs Fixed Effects. Will Cook Social Statistics

DISCREPANCY BETWEEN ACTUAL SELF AND VIRTUAL SELF: THEORETICAL EXTENSIONS, MEASUREMENT AND RELATION TO CONTRIBUTION IN VIRTUAL COMMUNITIES

Running Head: SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 1

The Effect of the Fulfillment of Hedonic and Aesthetic Information Needs of a Travel Magazine on Tourist Decision Making

Chapter 9. Youth Counseling Impact Scale (YCIS)

Assessing Measurement Invariance of the Teachers Perceptions of Grading Practices Scale across Cultures

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I)

DIFFERENTIAL PREDICTION OF LIFE SATISFACTION IN INDIVIDUALISTIC AND COLLECTIVISTIC CULTURES: TOWARDS INTEGRATION OF PERSONALITY AND CULTURAL MODELS

Problem with Cross-Cultural Comparison of User-Generated Ratings on Mechanical Turk

Self-Related and Other-Related Pathways to Subjective Well-Being in Japan and the United States

AN EXPLORATORY STUDY OF LEADER-MEMBER EXCHANGE IN CHINA, AND THE ROLE OF GUANXI IN THE LMX PROCESS

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Research on Software Continuous Usage Based on Expectation-confirmation Theory

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Group Environment Questionnaire With an Intercollegiate Sample

Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data

International Conference on Humanities and Social Science (HSS 2016)

Motivations of Luxury Consumption in America vs. China

Defining Psychology Behaviorism: Social Psychology: Milgram s Obedience Studies Bystander Non-intervention Cognitive Psychology:

The measurement of media literacy in eating disorder risk factor research: psychometric properties of six measures

Culture & Survey Measurement. Timothy Johnson Survey Research Laboratory University of Illinois at Chicago

The Psychometric Properties of Dispositional Flow Scale-2 in Internet Gaming

Test Bank. Chapter 2: The Cultural Context. Multiple Choice

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

How consumption influences our self-image Effects of material values and loneliness on self-image stability. Bachelor thesis Stephanie Haen ANR

Deliberate Self-Indulgence vs. Involuntary Loss of Self-Control: Towards a Robust Cross- Cultural Consumer Impulsiveness Scale

Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs -- A Pilot Study

Cultural Accommodation: The Effect of Language on the. Responses of Bilingual Hong Kong Chinese Managers

Is self-enhancement related to modesty or to individualism-collectivism? A test with four Israeli groups

Understanding Emotions. How does this man feel in each of these photos?

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University.

Personal Style Inventory Item Revision: Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Effects of Cultural Adjustment on Academic Achievement of International Students

Is the power of weak ties universal? A crosscultural comparison of social interaction in Argentina and Canada

An Assessment of the Mathematics Information Processing Scale: A Potential Instrument for Extending Technology Education Research

The CSGU: A Measure of Controllability, Stability, Globality, and Universality Attributions

VALUES AND ENTREPRENEURIAL ATTITUDE AS PREDICTORS OF NASCENT ENTREPRENEUR INTENTIONS

Handout 5: Establishing the Validity of a Survey Instrument

Best on the Left or on the Right in a Likert Scale

Social Determinants and Consequences of Children s Non-Cognitive Skills: An Exploratory Analysis. Amy Hsin Yu Xie

The Mediating Role of Parental Expectations in Culture and Well-Being

Electronic Supplementary Material. for. Pride, Personality, and the Evolutionary Foundations of Human Social Status

Validating Measures of Self Control via Rasch Measurement. Jonathan Hasford Department of Marketing, University of Kentucky

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

MURDOCH RESEARCH REPOSITORY.

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

How face influences consumption

Running head: HEDONISM AND HAPPINESS 1. Individualism as the Moderator of the Relationship between Hedonism and Happiness: A. Study in 19 Nations

COMPARISON OF FAMILY ENVIRONMENTAL SCALE (FES) SUBSCALES BETWEEN MALAYSIAN SETTING WITH THE ORIGINAL DIMENSION OF FES

ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH

Personal Growth Interpretation of Goal Attainment as a New Construct Relevant to Well-being

Is the minimal group paradigm culture dependent? A cross-cultural multi-level metaanalysis

Test Validity. What is validity? Types of validity IOP 301-T. Content validity. Content-description Criterion-description Construct-identification

THE APPLICATION OF ORDINAL LOGISTIC HEIRARCHICAL LINEAR MODELING IN ITEM RESPONSE THEORY FOR THE PURPOSES OF DIFFERENTIAL ITEM FUNCTIONING DETECTION

Running head: CROSS-CULTURAL INDIVIDUALIST AND COLLECTIVIST NORMS. Evaluating Group Member Behaviour Under Individualist and Collectivist Norms:

Reliability and Validity of the Divided

Vitanya Vanno *, Wannee Kaemkate, Suwimon Wongwanich

Transcription:

Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures Ellen Garbarino, University of Sydney, ellen.garbarino@sydney.edu.au Julie A. Lee, University of Western Australia, julie.lee@uwa.edu.au Geoff N. Soutar, University of Western Australia, geoff.soutar@uwa.edu.au Abstract A common expectation of the materialism literature is that highly individualistic people are more materialistic due to a substitution of goods for social relationships as self-identifiers; although the evidence is mixed. Including the vertical-horizontal distinction into the individualist-collectivist framework is expected to provide more insight into the drivers of materialism. We test our hypotheses using hierarchical regression based on online survey data from young adults from seven countries with widely varying cultural orientations. We find the materialism-success dimension is positively correlated with VI and negatively correlated with HI and that both orientations are positively correlated with the materialism-happiness dimension. Also, the effect of individual-level VI is stronger when country-level VI is lower. Keywords: Materialism, cross-cultural consumer behaviour, vertical-horizontal individualism 1

Materialism and Cultural Orientation: The Role of Vertical/Horizontal Indi vidualism within and across Cultures Introduction and Literature Review Given the widespread association marketing has with materialism, materialism has received surprisingly little attention in the discipline. Materialism is typically viewed as a relatively stable value or trait that is more common in some cultures and people than in others (e.g., Ger and Belk, 1996, 1999; Clarke and Micken, 2002). Highly individualistic people are often expected to be more materialistic because they are assumed to be more likely to substitute goods for social relationships as self-identifiers (Clarke and Micken, 2002; Wong, 1997); although the evidence in support of this straightforward assertion is mixed (Ger and Belk, 1996, 1999; Eastman et al., 1997). We propose that the inclusion of the vertical-horizontal distinction, recently incorporated into the individualist-collectivist framework, should further our understanding of the drivers of materialism. Specifically, we suggest verticalindividualism, with its emphasis on improving individual status, is a critical driver of materialism-success, whereas horizontal-individualism, with its emphasis on equality in status and uniqueness, will decrease materialism-success. However, vertical-individualism and horizontal-individualism are both expected to be positively related to materialism-happiness. These relationships are explored using data collected in seven countries with wide range of cultural orientations. Materialism has been conceptualised as an enduring belief that it is important to a person s identity to own material possessions. The construct is frequently conceptualised as having three dimensions: centrality, happiness, and success (Richins and Dawson, 1992). Centrality captures the idea that materialist place possessions and their acquisition at the center of their lives (Richins and Dawson, 1992, p. 304) and is embodied in the tongue-in-cheek saying I shop, therefore I am. The happiness dimension captures a belief that having things is essential to happiness; materialists believe increasing consumption will increase their wellbeing in life. The third dimension assesses a tendency by materialists to judge their own and others success by the number and quality of the things they own, capturing the ability goods have to confer status and project a desired self-image (Richins and Dawson, 1992). Some of the earliest work on materialism in marketing recognises the importance of crosscultural differences (Ger and Belk, 1996, 1999). However, relatively little research has explored the link between cultural orientation and materialism. Existing research suggests materialism is positively correlated with individualism and negatively correlated with collectivism (Wong, 1997; Clarke and Micken, 2002). This link between materialism and a cultural orientation toward individualism is implicitly stated in Mukerji s (1983, p. 8) definition of materialism as a cultural system in which material interests are not made subservient to other social goals. However, past research has had mixed results. In one of the earliest efforts, Wong (1997) found US undergraduates individualism was unrelated to their materialism, although their collectivism was significantly negatively related to materialism. More recently, Clarke and Micken (2002) found undergraduate business students in highly individualist countries, (USA and Australia - ranked 1 and 2/50; Hofstede, 2001), had a significantly higher level of materialism than did students in a moderately individualistic country (Mexico - ranked 30/50); although France (ranked 10/50 on individualism) had the highest level of materialism. Finally, Wong, Rindfleisch and Burroughs (2003) found adults individualism (measured as vertical individualism) was positively related to their materialism in US, Thai, Japanese, Korean and Singaporean samples. 2

Recent advances in the conceptualisation of cultural orientations suggest that distinguishing the vertical and horizontal aspects of individualism will offer insight into their relationship with materialism. 1 Triandis and colleagues (e.g., Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis and Gelfand, 1998) have argued that individualist and collectivist cultures are better understood by incorporating a distinction that separates societies based on how hierarchical a society is. This added distinction breaks the traditional two-group model into four groups (Vertical Individualists, Vertical Collectivist, Horizontal Individualists, and Horizontal Collectivists). Shavitt et al. (2006, p. 326) offer concise descriptions of each orientation: Vertical Individualists (VI) are concerned with improving their individual status and distinguishing themselves from others via competition, achievement and power ; Vertical Collectivists (VC) focus on complying with authorities and on enhancing the cohesion and status of their ingroups, even when that entails sacrificing their own personal goals ; Horizontal Individualists (HI) prefer to view themselves as equal to others in status. Rather than standing out, the focus is on expressing one s uniqueness and establishing one s capability to be successfully self-reliant ; and Horizontal Collectivists (HC) focus on sociability and interdependence with others within an egalitarian framework. These orientations have been shown to have convergent and divergent validity (e.g., Triandis and Gelfand (1998)). We propose that incorporating the vertical-horizontal distinction and the dimensionality of materialism will provide greater insight into the link between materialism and cultural orientation. Specifically, we expect VI to be positively and HI to be negatively related to materialism-success. However, we expect both VI and HI to be positively related to materialism-happiness. People with a VI orientation believe society is strongly hierarchical and that standing is achieved through individual success, which should lead to a positive correlation with materialism-success. Conversely, people with a HI orientation have an aversion to conspicuously successful persons and to braggarts, emphasizing instead the virtues of modesty (Shavit et al., 2006, p. 326), which should lead to a negative correlation with materialism-success. These relationships are supported by the values literature which finds that VI is positively, and HI negatively, related to achievement and power values (Oishi et al, 1998). However, since both the VI and HI orientations share self-reliance, independence and hedonism, we expect both VI and HI to be positively related to materialism-happiness. Finally, materialism is expected to be unrelated to collectivist orientations. Although VCs are strongly hierarchical, the standing within a group is likely to be determined by pre-existing group memberships and success related to group relationship management rather than ownership. Similarly, HCs, with their strong focus on egalitarianism, are less likely to expend scarce resources on material goods, to demonstrate their position in the social order. The current study explores the role of cultural orientation at both the country and individual level. Our expectation is that the influence of the individual s personal level of VI on their level of materialism will be moderated by the level of VI in their cultural context, such that individual level of VI will be less influential in people from cultures that are inherently higher on VI. A strong VI culture level is likely led all members toward higher levels of materialism, regardless of personal orientation, due to the influence of social norms on materialism (Ahuvia and Wong, 2002). Whereas people who reside in a low VI culture, but still personally ascribe to a strong VI orientation, should have an even closer relationship between their personal VI leveland their materialism. Thus, we hypothesize: H1: A person s level of vertical individualism is a) positively related to their materialismsuccess and b) positively related to their materialism-happiness. 3

H2: A person s level of horizontal individualism is a) negatively related to their materialism-success and b) positively related to their materialism-happiness. H3: The level of cultural vertical individualism is positively related to materialism-success. H4: The effect of individual-level VI on materialism-success will be moderated by the country-level VI, such that the individual VI effect will be stronger when countrylevel VI is lower. The Present Study Data were collected as part of a larger study designed to develop a cross-culturally valid consumer decision-making model that was tested in the tourism context. The sample countries were chosen for their relevance to the larger tourism context. While there were some limitations in the countries that were chosen, at least one country was chosen that was higher on each of the four cultural dimensions (the USA (VI: n=249), the UK (VI: n=247), Australia (HI: n=463), Germany (VI: n=634), Brazil (HC: n=537), China (VC: n=244) and South Korea (VC: n=430)). Responses were collected over the Internet by a large online panel provider with members in each of the countries. Respondents were recruited by email and paid by the panel provider in points that are used for online purchases. The use of online panels allows for a cost-effective large-scale multi-country sample. The samples were matched, using quotas for age groups and gender within relevant young adult age groups (18-20, 21-23, 24-26, and 27-29). We used young adults because of the need for a more homogeneous sample to increase multi-country measurement convergence and their relevance to both the tourism and materialism topics. The initial questionnaire was developed in English and translated into Mandarin, German, Portuguese, and Korean by bilingual translators living in each of the target countries; then back translated into English by a second translator, following the translation-back-translation method (Brislin, 1970). The researchers and the final translator compared the English versions and resolved any discrepancies in meaning. The survey included questions about shopping habits, traits, values and socio-demographics, as part of a larger study. Materialism was measured by Richins and Dawson s (1992) 18-item three-dimensional scale on a strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7) scale. Reverse items were reworded, as suggested by Wong et al. (2003). The four cultural orientations were measured by Triandis and Gelfand s (1998) 16-item scale on a strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7) scale. The measurement properties of the materialism and cultural orientation dimensions were examined using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and Fornell and Larker s (1981), initially in the USA, and then in the other countries. One-factor congeneric models were examined for the materialism-subscales that did not fit the data well. The removal of one item from materialism-success produced a good fit (χ 2 5 = 10.11, p = 0.07; CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.06); the removal of two items from materialism-centrality produced a good fit (χ 2 2 = 2.55, p = 0.28; CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.03) and the removal of one item from materialism-happiness produced a good fit (χ 2 2 = 3.76, p =.15; CFI = 1; RMSEA = 0.06). The reliabilities were all high (>0.80) and the AVEs suggested convergent validity (>0.70 for success and happiness and 0.54 for centrality). However, the shared variance between success and centrality was greater than the lowest AVE score, which suggested discriminant validity could not be assumed between these two constructs. Consequently, the centrality dimension was dropped, as it was not focal to the current research. The subscales metric invariance was then assessed by examining the χ 2 diff between an unconstrained model and a model in which the 4

measurement weights were constrained equal across all of the countries. Metric invariance was achieved for the two sub-scales (Success: χ 2 diff3 = 11.33, p = 0.01, with remaining fit indices <0.01; Happiness: χ 2 diff3 = 5.07, p = 0.17). The one-factor congeneric models for the 4-item HC subscale had a good fit (χ 2 2 = 0.80, p = 0.67; CFI = 1; RMSEA < 0.01). The removal of one item from each of the other subscales led to a good fit, after fixing two of the error variances to be equal to provide the necessary degree of freedom (VI: χ 2 1 = 1.84, p = 0.17; CFI = 0.99; RMSEA = 0.06; HI: χ 2 1 = 0.30, p = 0.59; CFI = 1; RMSEA < 0.01; and VC: χ 2 1 = 0.02, p = 0.90; CFI = 1; RMSEA < 0.01). The reliabilities were all acceptable (ranging from 0.73 for VI to 0.87 for VC) and the AVEs suggested convergent validity (ranging from 0.49 for VI to 0.68 for VC). The shared variance between each of the dimensions was less than the lowest AVE score, suggesting discriminant validity. Partial metric invariance was achieved for each scale (HI: χ 2 diff3 = 5.33, p =0.15; VI: χ 2 diff3 = 1.93, p = 0.59; VC: χ 2 diff3 = 4.21, p = 0.24; HC: χ 2 diff6 = 6.50, p = 0.37). The following model specifications were used to examine the hypothesised relationships and the regressions were estimated using the SAS PROC MIXED procedure, following the approach suggested by Singer (1998): Level 1: MAT ij = 0j + 1j (VI ij - MEANVI j ) + 2j (HI ij - MEANHI j ) + r ij, 0j = 00 + u 0j, 1j = 10 + u 1j ; Level 2: 0j = 00 + 01 MEANVI j + u 0j, In this case, i indicates individuals; j indicates groups; MAT represents a person s materialism and VI and HI represent a person s vertical and horizontal individualism, respectively. MEANVI represents a country cohort s mean vertical individualism. Combining the 2 levels yields the hierarchical model: Multilevel: MAT ij = 00 + 01 MEANVI j + 10 (VI ij - MEANVI j ) + 20 (HI ij - MEANHI j ) + 11 MEANVI i (VI ij - MEANVI j ) + 0j + 1j (VI ij - MEANVI j ) + r ij. The fixed component of the equation includes 00 + 01 MEANVI j + 10 (VI ij - MEANVI j ) + 20 (HI ij - MEANHI j ) + 11 MEANVI i (VI ij - MEANVI j ), while the randomcomponent includes 1j (VI ij - MEANVI j ) + r ij. The level 1 error term(r ij ) is assumed to be normally distributed with a mean of 0 and a variance of 2. The random intercept ( 0j ) is assumed to be multivariate normally distributed over the groups, with an expected value of 0 and a variance of 00. The coefficient 0j was specified as random so as to allow the intercept to vary across groups. In our model, respondents are nested within groups, in this case the country cohorts. The Level 1 predictors were centred within country cohort and Level 2 predictors were centred by the grand-mean (Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002; Steenkamp, ter Hofstede and Wedel, 2006). Results Table 1 presents the parameter symbols and estimates of effects on materialism-success and materialism-happiness. The intercept estimated the average country cohort materialismsuccess score as 3.61 and the average materialism-happiness score as 4.22. Table 1: Effects on Materialism (unstandardised coefficients) Independent Variables Materialismsuccess Materialismhappiness Intercept ( 00 ) 3.61*** 4.22*** Main effects: Individual 5

VI ( 10 ).65***.48*** HI ( 20 ) -.05**.16*** Main effects: Country MEANVI ( 01 ).79**.61* Cross level interactions VI x MEANVI ( 11 ) -.12*.00 Explained variance (%) Individual-level 35% 23% Country-level 89% 72% * p <.05, ** p <.01, *** p <.001 H1 argues that the higher a person s VI, a) the higher the degree of materialism-success and b) the higher the degree of materialism-happiness. H1a and H1b were supported, materialismsuccess 10 = 0.65 (p < 0.001); materialism-happiness 20 = 0.48 (p < 0.001). H2 argues that the higher a person s HI, a) the lower the degree of materialism-success and b) the higher the degree of materialism-happiness. H2a and H2b were also supported: materialism-success 10 = -0.05 (p < 0.01); materialism-happiness 20 = 0.16 (p < 0.001). We also anticipated a main effect of the average vertical individualism in a country cohort (H3). Consistent with H3, the degree of vertical individualism of the country cohorts had a positive effect on materialismsuccess 01 = 0.79, and to a lesser extent on materialism-happiness 01 = 0.61. Finally, H4 argues for a cross-level interaction between the individual and country level materialismsuccess variables. H4 was also supported; the positive effect VI had on materialism-success was weaker in cultures in which higher vertical-individualism was the norm ( 11 = -0.12, p < 0.05). Table 1 also reports the explained variance, which indicates the percentage of explainable variation, explained at each level, following Singer (1998). At the country level, VI explains 89% of the explainable variation in country-level mean materialism-success and 72% of the explainable variation in country-level mean materialism-happiness. In addition, the variance component for the intercept (success = 0.04; p = 0.07: happiness = 0.04; p = 0.08) was not significant, suggesting there is very little additional variation in country-level materialism that is not explained by the model. At the individual level, VI and HI explain 35% of the within-country cohorts explainable variation in materialism-success and 23% of the explainable variation in materialism-happiness. Discussion The current study refines our understanding of the relationship between individualism and materialism, by illustrating the differential influences the vertical (emphasising hierarchy) and horizontal (emphasising equality) dimensions have on materialism-success, but not on materialism-happiness. It shows the importance of distinguishing between aspects of individualism that address differences between countries such Australia s (HI) and the USA s (VI) culture as these differences have important implications for cross-cultural researchers and for marketers trying to develop appropriate strategies for each country. 1 Interestingly, all of the countries mentioned in the prior paragraph were vertical countries. 6

References Ahuvia, A.C., Wong, N.Y.C., 2002. Personality and values based materialism: Their relationship and origins. Journal of Consumer Psychology 12 (4) 389-402. Brislin, R. W. 1970. Back-translation for cross-culture research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185 216. Clarke III, I.r, Micken, K.S., 2002. An exploratory cross-cultural analysis of the values of materialism. Journal of International Consumer Marketing 14 (4), 65-89. Eastman, J.K., Fredenberger, B., Campbell, D. Calvert, S., 1997. The relationship between status consumption and materialism: A cross-cultural comparison of Chinese, Mexican and American students. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, winter, 52-66. Fornell, C., Larcker, D. F., 1981. Evaluating structural equations models with unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research 18(1), 39-50. Ger, G., Belk, R., 1996. Cross-cultural differences in materialism. Journal of Economic Psychology 17, 55-77. Ger, G., Belk, R., 1999. Accounting for materialism in four cultures. Journal of Material Culture 4(2) 183-204. Hofstede, G., Culture s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2 nd ed), Thousand Oaks, CA, USA Sage 2001. Mukerji, C., 1983. From Graven Images: Patterns of Modern Materialism. New York: Columbia University Press. Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., Diener, E., Suh, E.M., 1998. The measurement of values and individualismcollectivism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24, 1177-1189. Raudenbush, S.W., Bryk, A.S., 2002, Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publication. Richins, M.L., Dawson, S. 1992. A consumer values orientation for materialism and its measurement: Sc ale development and validation. Journal of Consumer Research 19(3), 303-316. Shavitt, S., Lalwani, A.K., Zhang, J., Torelli, C.J., 2006. The horizontal/vertical distinction in crosscultural consumer research. Journal of Consumer Psychology 16 (4), 325-356. Singer, J.D., 1998. Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, hierarchical models, and individual growth models, Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 24, 323-355. Singelis, T.M., Triandis, H.C., Bhawuk, D.P.S., Gelfand, M.J., 1995. Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement, Cross- Cultural Research 29 (3), 240-275. St eenkamp, J-B, ter Hofstede, F., Wedel, M., 2006. A cross-national investigation into the individual and national cultural antecedents of consumer innovativeness, Journal of Marketing, 63, 55-69. Triandis, H.C., Gelfand, M.J., 1998. Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 (1), 118-128. Wong, N.Y.C. 1997. Suppose you own the world and no one knows? Conspicuous Consumption, Materialism and Self. Advances in Consumer Research 24, 197-203. Wong, N., Rindfleisch, A., & Burroughs, J.E. 2003. Do reverse-worded items confound measures in cross-cultural consumer research? The case of the Material Values Scale. Journal of Consumer Research, 30, 72-91. 7