CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY. Abstract

Similar documents
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Holistic processing does not require configural variability

PSYC20007 READINGS AND NOTES

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Selective attention to global and local levels

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

Range effects of an irrelevant dimension on classification

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

When the visual format of the color carrier word does and does not modulate the Stroop effect

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Working Memory Load and Stroop Interference Effect

Synaesthesia. Hao Ye

Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments

Verbal Working Memory. The left temporoparietal junction in verbal working memory: Storage or attention. Baddelely s Multiple-Component Model

Supplementary Materials

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

Project exam in Cognitive Psychology PSY1002. Autumn Course responsible: Kjellrun Englund

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching

Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

Identify these objects

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

Functional Fixedness: The Functional Significance of Delayed Disengagement Based on Attention Set

Item-specific control of automatic processes: Stroop process dissociations

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

STEPS Students Report

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments

Stroop interference is affected in inhibition of return

Auditory Dominance: Overshadowing or Response Competition?

Fundamentals of Cognitive Psychology, 3e by Ronald T. Kellogg Chapter 2. Multiple Choice

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer

Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet?

The role of parity, physical size, and magnitude in numerical cognition: The SNARC effect revisited

A Structured Language Approach to Teach Language and Literacy to Hearing and Visually Impaired Pupils with Autism

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Face Perception - An Overview Bozana Meinhardt-Injac Malte Persike

Modelling the Stroop Effect: Dynamics in Inhibition of Automatic Stimuli Processing

Working Memory Load and the Stroop Interference Effect

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Perceptual Load in Different Regions of the Visual Field and its Effect on Attentional Selectivity. Hadas Marciano Advisor: Yaffa Yeshurun

The relative attractiveness of distractors and targets affects the coming and going of item-specific control: Evidence from flanker tasks

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

The effects of perceptual load on semantic processing under inattention

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

Satiation in name and face recognition

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

The measure of an illusion or the illusion of a measure?

TEMPORAL ORDER JUDGEMENTS A SENSITIVE MEASURE FOR MEASURING PERCEPTUAL LATENCY?

Language Speech. Speech is the preferred modality for language.

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection

Running head: COLOR WORDS AND COLOR REPRESENTATIONS

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Context influences holistic processing of nonface objects in the composite task

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

On the role of stimulus response and stimulus stimulus compatibility in the Stroop effect

Semantic Contamination of Visual Similarity Judgments

Amodal Completion in Visual Search Preemption or Context Effects?

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

Attentional set for axis of symmetry in symmetry-defined visual search

Interaction among auditory dimensions: Timbre, pitch, and loudness

The effect of orientation on number word processing

The obligatory nature of holistic processing of faces in social judgments

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

New Reflections on Visual Search

Although most previous studies on categorization

birds were used. Every two weeks during treatment, all thirty-two items were probed. A final probe was collected six to seven weeks post-treatment.

ON THE ROLE OF WORKING MEMORY IN RESPONSE INTERFERENCE ' JOHN F. STINS, SJOERD VOSSE, DORRET I. BOOMSMA, ECO J. C. DE GEUS

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

Effect of irrelevant information on the processing of relevant information: Facilitation and/or interference? The influence of experimental design

Cross-modal interaction between vision and touch: the role of synesthetic correspondence

Introduction and Historical Background. August 22, 2007

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

Dissociating between cardinal and ordinal and between the value and size magnitudes of coins

Humans and nonhuman animals discriminate numbers in a

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS

Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations

Interaction Between Social Categories in the Composite Face Paradigm. Wenfeng Chen and Naixin Ren. Chinese Academy of Sciences. Andrew W.

One Mechanism or Two: A Commentary on Reading Normal and Degraded Words

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

Enhanced Performance for Recognition of Irrelevant Target-Aligned Auditory Stimuli: Unimodal and Cross-modal Considerations

The role of modality congruence in the presentation and recognition of taskirrelevant stimuli in dual task paradigms.

University of Illinois Press

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Rachel Rees, University College London Cate Calder, Cued Speech Association UK

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

Transcription:

CONGRUENCE EFFECTS IN LETTERS VERSUS SHAPES: THE RULE OF LITERACY Thomas Lachmann *, Gunjan Khera * and Cees van Leeuwen # * Psychology II, University of Kaiserslautern, Kaiserslautern, Germany # Laboratory for perceptual Dynamics, Brain Science Institute, Riken, Wako-shi, Japan lachmann@rhrk.uni-kl.de Abstract In a series of experiments in which participants had to classify stimuli as letters versus shapes we found a differentiation between holistic non-letter processing and analytic letter processing in adults. In a recent study we investigated whether this distinction is also present in literates. We found that the differentiation is absent in illiterates; they uniformly showed analytic perception for both letters and non-letters. In a recent study (Lachmann, Khera, Srinivasan and van Leeuwen, 2012) we compared a group of adult skilled readers to a group who never learned to read, using the classification task in which a differentiation in processing between letters and non-letters was originally found (van Leeuwen & Lachmann, 2004). In this task, target letters and non-letters (shapes) were surrounded by a task-irrelevant shape. Shapes were classified faster if the target and its surrounding (non-target) were form-congruent as compared to when they differed in shape (when both were form-incongruent). This is an example of the well-known congruence effect (Bavelier, Deruelle & Proksch, 2000); and can be related to early and mid-level visual perception (Boenke, Ohl, Nikolaev, Lachmann & van Leeuwen, 2008). The congruence effect indicates (in an operationally specific sense) holistic perceptual grouping: The surrounding visual information is bound to the target, and is processed faster if both call for the same response. For letter targets, however, the opposite result was found: letters were categorized faster when surrounded by an incongruent non-target (when surrounding non-target was of different visual form than the target) than when the non-target was congruent: a negative congruence effect (Lachmann and van Leeuwen, 2008, 2008 b; van Leeuwen & Lachmann, 2004). The dissociation in processing strategy was considered a product of a specific analytic strategy optimized during learning to read in order to guarantee a rapid grapheme-phoneme mapping. Binding irrelevant visual information from the surrounding would disturb this fast mapping. The visual structure of the surrounding shape is therefore suppressed (as symmetry suppression for letters, cf. Pegado et al., 2011, Lachmann et al., 2009). Doing so is more difficult for congruent than for incongruent items, resulting in negative congruence effects (Bavelier et al., 2000; Briand, 1994; van Leeuwen & Bakker, 1995). Thus, we may conclude that in the early or intermediate stages of visual perception, skilled readers process letters using a unique analytic encoding strategy whereas for processing shapes a holistic processing strategy is applied. For illiterates who are unfamiliar with the alphabet, we did not expect such a dissociation. Since illiterates are not able to differentiate letters from non-letter shapes, they were expected to process both letters and non-letter shapes with one and the same strategy. The question in Lachmann et al. (2012) was: what strategy is it, a holistic or an analytic strategy? If holistic, we may conclude that the analytic strategy is a secondary adaptation; if analytic, we may conclude that both analytic and holistic processing are intrinsic, primary strategies of the visual system.

Experiment There were 32 Indian illiterates who participated in the study (Lachmann et al, 2012). All of them reported that they, while having normal vision and hearing are not able to read neither English nor Hindi, do not speak English, and are not familiar with Latin alphabets. Prior to the experiment their familiarity with the alphabet was determined by using a letter identification test. All participants from illiterate group included in the study performed within chance level. The control group consisted of 26 Indian adults who were able to read fluently and to write in English. As in van Leeuwen and Lachmann (2004, Exp. 4), we used 24 unique stimuli: the four capital letters A, H, L, C; and the four geometrical shapes square, triangle, rectangle, circle, each of which was shown either in isolation or surrounded by a congruent or incongruent nontarget geometrical shape. For instance, A in isolation, A surrounded by a triangle (congruent condition), A surrounded by a rectangle (incongruent condition). Response Category 1 Response Category 2 Selection 1 Selection 2 Selection 3 Selection 4 Selection 5 Selection 6 Fig. 1. Six selections of stimuli used in the experiment for individual participants. Further explanations in the text.

As in the 2004 experiment, six subsets of stimuli were presented to the participants in a counterbalanced fashion (Fig. 2). For each individual, the stimuli were restricted to two letters and two shapes. Letters and shapes were pair-wise similar in visual form, e.g. an A and a triangle. They were assigned in a counterbalanced manner to two different response categories: For instance, Category 1: A or a Rectangle versus Category 2: L or a Triangle. Note that letters and shapes that are similar to each other in shape, such as the A and the triangle, were always assigned to different response categories. Thus, in order to solve the task, phonological coding of the letters is useful to distinguish between response categories, but is not necessary. In skilled readers this design was found to implicitly require a distinction between letters and shapes (see Table 1, results from van Leeuwen & Lachmann, 2004). Table 1: Results from van Leeuwen and Lachmann, Exp 4. (2004). Reaction Time (msec) Targets Context Congruence M SD Letters Isolated 464.0 120.0 Surrounded Congruent 475.0 123.1 Incongruent 468.5 119.1 Shapes Isolated 480.7 133.2 Surrounded Congruent 484.9 128.3 Incongruent 491.5 131.1 A speeded choice reaction task (4 targets x 3 conditions: isolated, congruent surrounding, incongruent surrounding, 60 repeated measures) was required according to the response categories displayed in Fig. 2, by pressing either the left or the right bottom marked on the keyboard of the computer lap top. The four stimuli were shown to the participant prior to the experiment and it was marked for what two stimuli, e.g. A and square, the left and for what, e.g., L or triangle, the right bottom press was required. It was emphasized that the surrounding, if it occurs, is not relevant for the task and should be ignored. Results and Discussion Results for both groups in Lachmann et al. (2012) are displayed in Fig. 2. Even though both groups in this study consisted of Indian adults, they are likely to differ in more than their ability to read; such as in general intelligence, education, language skills and in their familiarity with computers, which all may have influenced their performance. In particular, the latter factor may be responsible for the considerably higher over-all reaction times in illiterates, when compared to literates, both in the earlier and the present study. Such differences are probably inevitable in this kind of studies. However, the pattern of results, in particular that we found literates to show the same effects as literates in earlier studies whereas illiterates were found to show no differentiation in processing between letters and non-letter shapes suggests that congruence effects are influenced by literacy. Illiterates not only processed letters and shapes equally over all, i.e., there was no letter-superiority effect, they also showed for both materials the same preference for items presented in isolation versus in surrounding, as well as the same effects of irrelevant surrounding congruence. For both letters and non-letters, incongruent surroundings were preferred over congruent ones. In contrast, literates of the same ethnicity differentiated between letters and non-letters, just as skilled readers from Germany and Japan did (Lachmann and van Leeuwen, 2004; Jincho et al., 2008). They showed a letter superiority

effect: letters are processed faster than non-letters and produced opposite congruence effects for letters versus non-letters: a positive congruence effects for non-letters and a negative congruence effects for letters (see Fig. 2). Fig. 2. Results from Lachmann, Khera, Srinivasan and van Leeuwen (2012). The negative congruence effect in illiterates, i.e. their preference for incongruent surroundings, implies that an analytic perceptual strategy prevailed. Developmental studies might have led us to expect that before reading is automated, at least in an age up from six years on, a holistic strategy to predominate. Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2008) compared adults with different groups of children: beginning normal readers from Grade 3 and 4, and age matched developmental dyslexics. Most of these children showed positive congruence effects for both letters and non-letters, indicating holistic preference. One reason could be that certain brain functions related to reading, especially auditory processing (Banai & Ahissar, 2006) are still developing at this age. This may keep them from using an analytic letter processing strategy that would enable rapid grapheme to phoneme encoding. A subgroup of dyslexic children in Lachmann and van Leeuwen (2008), with particular difficulties in reading non-words, however, shows particularly strong negative congruence effect for letters. This suggests that at this stage in development the analytic strategy at least is present, even though, for object recognition and face recognition, the holistic strategy became already dominant (Schwarzer, 2002). The results in dyslexics, therefore, are not inconsistent with those of illiterates: The negative congruence effects in illiterates means that analytic processing is not a reading-specific, secondary differentiation in perceptual organization that accompanies the process of learning to read. Rather, it is a generic and primary perceptual processing strategy, on a par with the holistic strategy. Skilled reading recruits this general perceptual strategy for letter recognition, and uses it in a coordinated fashion along with other functions, including phonological, cognitive, motor and attentional ones, in meeting the specific demands of reading. What is specific to skilled reading is not the automatization of a letter-specific perceptual strategy, but the automated coordination of various functional components in their specific combination (Lachmann et al., 2012). In this process, letter processing becomes tied up with the analytic perceptual processing strategy. As a result, adult readers no longer show the ability to process simple

non-letter objects analytically. This result is in accordance with the pervasiveness of congruency effects in visual object perception (Boenke et al, 2009; Erikson & Schultz, 1979; Pomerantz, Pristach & Carson, 1989). In incongruent conditions, observers fail to ignore irrelevant information, even if this would facilitate processing. This effect is usually considered a result of attentional interference of the irrelevant flanking or surrounding information; this remains a puzzle if we consider that, in principle, focused attention could have been applied to the target (Miles & Proctor, 2010). The present study suggests that this is because analytic processing has preferentially become associated with reading. Despite this, having learned to read does not render entirely impossible the analytic processing of nonletter shapes. Evidence of analytic processing is not restricted to letters; negative congruence effects, although sparse, are found whenever active suppression of surrounding information is needed to distinguish a target (Bavelier et al., 2000, Briand, 1994; van Leeuwen & Bakker, 1995). The fact that these conditions are rare suggests that the differentiation that associates holistic processing with non-letters and analytic processing with letters is, by and large, effective. References Banai, K., and Ahissar, M. (2006). Auditory Processing Deficits in Dyslexia: Task or Stimulus Related? Cerebral Cortex, 16(12), 1718-1728. Bavelier, D., Deruelle, C., & Proksch, J. (2000). Positive and negative compatibility effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(1), 100-112. Boenke, L., Ohl, F., Nikolaev, A., Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2009). Different time courses of Stroop and Garner effects in perception - an Event-Related Potentials Study. NeuroImage, 45, 1272-1288. Briand, K. A. (1994). Selective attention to global or local-structure of objects alternative measures of nontarget processing. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 562-575. Eriksen, C.W., & Schultz, D.W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 249 263. Jincho, N., Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). Dissociating congruence effects in letters versus shapes: Kanji and kana. Acta Psychologica, 129, 138-146. Lachmann, T., Khera, G., Srinivasan, N., & van Leeuwen, C. (2012). Learning to read aligns visual analytical skills with grapheme-phoneme mapping: Evidence from illiterates. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 4:8. Doi: 103389/fnevo.2012. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). Different letter-processing strategies in diagnostic subgroups of developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 730-744. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008 b). Differentiation of holistic processing in the time course of letter recognition. Acta Psychologica, 129, 121-129. Miles, J. D., & Proctor, R. W. (2010). Attention is required for acquisition but not expression of new response biases. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 36, 1554-1560. Pegado, F., Nakamura, K., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2011). Breaking the symmetry: Mirror discrimination for single letters but not for pictures in the Visual Word Form Area. Neuroimage, 55, 742-74. Pomerantz, J.R., Pristach, E.A., & Carson, C.E. (1989). Attention and object perception. In: Shepp, B., Ballesteros, S. (Eds.), Object perception: structure and process (pp. 53-89). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

van Leeuwen, C., & Bakker, L. (1995). Stroop can occur without Garner interference: Strategic and mandatory influences in multidimensional stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 379-392. van Leeuwen, C., & Lachmann, T. (2004). Negative and positive congruence effects in letters and shapes. Perception & Psychophysics, 6, 908-925.