Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Similar documents
BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED

Time Varying Comb Filters to Reduce Spectral and Temporal Masking in Sensorineural Hearing Impairment

Determination of filtering parameters for dichotic-listening binaural hearing aids

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

FPGA IMPLEMENTATION OF COMB FILTER FOR AUDIBILITY ENHANCEMENT OF HEARING IMPAIRED

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Pitch & Binaural listening

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

Effects of speaker's and listener's environments on speech intelligibili annoyance. Author(s)Kubo, Rieko; Morikawa, Daisuke; Akag

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Issues faced by people with a Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Tactile Communication of Speech

PERIPHERAL AND CENTRAL AUDITORY ASSESSMENT

Auditory fmri correlates of loudness perception for monaural and diotic stimulation

A. SEK, E. SKRODZKA, E. OZIMEK and A. WICHER

Hearing in Noise Test in Subjects With Conductive Hearing Loss

Development of a new loudness model in consideration of audio-visual interaction

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

1706 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113 (3), March /2003/113(3)/1706/12/$ Acoustical Society of America

Characterizing individual hearing loss using narrow-band loudness compensation

FREQUENCY COMPRESSION AND FREQUENCY SHIFTING FOR THE HEARING IMPAIRED

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

Lecture 3: Perception

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

Asynchronous glimpsing of speech: Spread of masking and task set-size

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

Sound localization psychophysics

Temporal offset judgments for concurrent vowels by young, middle-aged, and older adults

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

The basic hearing abilities of absolute pitch possessors

Enrique A. Lopez-Poveda Alan R. Palmer Ray Meddis Editors. The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Psychoacoustics of hearing impairment

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Charactering the individual ear by the Auditory Profile

Hearing II Perceptual Aspects

Computational Perception /785. Auditory Scene Analysis

Prediction of Loudness Growth in Subjects with Sensorineural Hearing Loss Using Auditory Steady State Response. Afaf A. Emara

Changes in the Role of Intensity as a Cue for Fricative Categorisation

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Perceptual Effects of Nasal Cue Modification

Masker-signal relationships and sound level

! Can hear whistle? ! Where are we on course map? ! What we did in lab last week. ! Psychoacoustics

Lateralized speech perception in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners and its relationship to temporal processing

Improving Audibility with Nonlinear Amplification for Listeners with High-Frequency Loss

2/25/2013. Context Effect on Suprasegmental Cues. Supresegmental Cues. Pitch Contour Identification (PCI) Context Effect with Cochlear Implants

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Spectral-peak selection in spectral-shape discrimination by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Relating binaural pitch perception to the individual listener s auditory profile

Audiogram+: GN Resound proprietary fitting rule

Effects of aural combination tones on the loudness of a pure tone masked by an inharmonic pure-tone

The Handbook of Hearing and the Effects of Noise

Hearing the Universal Language: Music and Cochlear Implants

Intelligibility of narrow-band speech and its relation to auditory functions in hearing-impaired listeners

64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014

Hearing Lectures. Acoustics of Speech and Hearing. Auditory Lighthouse. Facts about Timbre. Analysis of Complex Sounds

Relating binaural pitch perception to the individual listener's auditory profile

Lecture Outline. The GIN test and some clinical applications. Introduction. Temporal processing. Gap detection. Temporal resolution and discrimination

Comparison of Binaural Interaction Component (BIC) in Symmetrical and Asymmetrical Hearing Loss: Pilot Study

Brian D. Simpson Veridian, 5200 Springfield Pike, Suite 200, Dayton, Ohio 45431

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants

Why? Speech in Noise + Hearing Aids = Problems in Noise. Recall: Two things we must do for hearing loss: Directional Mics & Digital Noise Reduction

Effects of partial masking for vehicle sounds

16.400/453J Human Factors Engineering /453. Audition. Prof. D. C. Chandra Lecture 14

An active unpleasantness control system for indoor noise based on auditory masking

ID# Exam 2 PS 325, Fall 2009

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes

Thresholds for different mammals

THE ROLE OF VISUAL SPEECH CUES IN THE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SYNTHETIC STIMULI BY CHILDREN USING A COCHLEAR IMPLANT AND CHILDREN WITH NORMAL HEARING

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

The Effect of Age, Noise Level, and Frequency on Loudness Matching Functions of Normal Hearing Listeners with Noise Masking

ClaroTM Digital Perception ProcessingTM

Rationale for Binaural Hearing Aid Fittings

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100 (4), Pt. 1, October /96/100(4)/2352/13/$ Acoustical Society of America 2352

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Juan Carlos Tejero-Calado 1, Janet C. Rutledge 2, and Peggy B. Nelson 3

Limited available evidence suggests that the perceptual salience of

Research Article The Acoustic and Peceptual Effects of Series and Parallel Processing

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Audiogram+: The ReSound Proprietary Fitting Algorithm

Dichotic Word Recognition in Young Adults with Simulated Hearing Loss. A Senior Honors Thesis

Evaluating the role of spectral and envelope characteristics in the intelligibility advantage of clear speech

Publication VI. c 2007 Audio Engineering Society. Reprinted with permission.

Evidence specifically favoring the equalization-cancellation theory of binaural unmasking

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 27, , June tleseareh Note RELATION BETWEEN REACTION TIME AND LOUDNESS

Combination of Bone-Conducted Speech with Air-Conducted Speech Changing Cut-Off Frequency

Literature Overview - Digital Hearing Aids and Group Delay - HADF, June 2017, P. Derleth

Fig. 1 High level block diagram of the binary mask algorithm.[1]

Effect of musical training on pitch discrimination performance in older normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

The Verification of ABR Response by Using the Chirp Stimulus in Moderate Sensorineural Hearing Loss

Tuning curves and pitch matches in a listener with a unilateral, low-frequency hearing loss Florentine, Mary; Houtsma, A.J.M.

Validation Studies. How well does this work??? Speech perception (e.g., Erber & Witt 1977) Early Development... History of the DSL Method

Transcription:

Paper No. 556 Proceedings of 20 th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation Pandurangarao N. Kulkarni (1), Prem C. Pandey (2), and Dakshayani S. Jangamashetti (3), (1, 2) Indian Institute of Technology Bombay, Powai Mumbai 400 076, Maharashtra, India (3) Basaveshwar Engineering College, Bagalkot 587102, Karnataka, India PACS: 43.66 Ts and 43.66 Sr. ABSTRACT Earlier studies on binaural dichotic presentation by spectral splitting of speech signal using a pair of complementary comb filters, for improving speech perception by persons with moderate bilateral sensorineural hearing loss, have shown mixed results: from no advantage to improvements in recognition scores corresponding to an SNR advantage of 2-9 db. The filters used in these studies had different bandwidths and realizations. For an optimal performance of the scheme, the perceived loudness of different spectral components in the speech signal should be balanced, especially for components in transition bands which get presented to both the ears. For selecting magnitude responses of such filters, we have investigated the relationship between the signal amplitudes for binaural presentation of a tone to evoke the same loudness as that of the monaural presentation. Listening tests were conducted, on eight normalhearing subjects, for comparing the perceived loudness of monaural presentations to that of the binaural presentation with different combination of amplitudes for the tones presented to the left and right ears, at 250 Hz, 500 Hz, 1 khz, and 2 khz. The sum of the amplitudes of the left and right tones in binaural presentation being equal to that of the monaural tone resulted in monaural-binaural loudness match, indicating that the magnitude response of the comb filters used for dichotic presentation should be complementary on a linear scale. An analysis of the magnitude responses of the comb filters used in earlier studies showed large deviations from the perceptual balance requirement, and those with smaller deviations were more effective in improving speech perception. A pair of comb filters, based on auditory critical bandwidths and magnitude responses closely satisfying the requirement for perceptual balance, was designed as 512-coefficient linear phase FIR filters for sampling frequency of 10 khz. Listening tests on six normal-hearing subjects showed improvements in the consonant recognition scores corresponding to an SNR advantage of approximately 12 db. Tests using 12 subjects with moderate bilateral sensorineural hearing loss showed an improvement in the recognition score in the range 7-31 %. Thus the investigations showed that binaural dichotic presentation using comb filters designed for perceptual balance resulted in better speech perception. INTRODUCTION Several investigations have been reported on binaural dichotic presentation, by splitting the speech signal using a pair of comb filters with complementary magnitude responses, for improving speech perception by persons with moderate bilateral sensorineural hearing loss [1 6]. In this scheme, alternate bands are presented to left and right ears. These studies have shown mixed results: from no advantage to improvements in recognition scores corresponding to an SNR advantage of 2-9 db. All the filters used in these studies have linear phase responses. The variations in the results reported may be because of differences in the magnitude responses. The comb filters for spectral splitting should have a small ripple in the pass bands and a large attenuation in the stop band. As filters have finite transition bands between pass and stop bands, the spectral components of the speech signal in the transition bands are presented to both the ears. For an optimal performance of the scheme, the perceived loudness of different spectral components in the speech signal should be balanced, especially for components in the transition bands. Therefore the two filters should have magnitude responses such that perceived loudness for spectral components in the transition bands is same as that in the pass bands. The objective of the investigation presented in this paper is to study the perceptual balance in binaural hearing, i.e., finding a relationship between the signal amplitudes in the left and the right ear in binaural presentation which will evoke the same loudness as a monaural presentation. Responses of the comb filters used by different researchers are examined for the rquirement of perceptual balance. A pair of comb filters, based on auditory critical bandwidths and magnitude responses closely satisfying the requirement for perceptual balnce, was designed. It was evaluated for improving the speech perception by persons with normal-hearing in the presence of broad-band masking noise and by persons with moderate bilateral sensorineural hearing loss. ICA 2010 1

23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 LOUDNESS OF BINAURAL PRESENTATION Several studies comparing the loudness of binaural and monaural sounds have been reported [7 14]. A study by Scharf [7] reported the binaural level difference for equal loudness (BLDEL) to be about 5 db at low presentation levels, 7 db at moderate presentation levels, and about 6 db at high presentation levels. In another study by Scharf [8], involving dichotic presentation of two tones, the subjects perceived two distinct auditory images. There was no change in the perceived loudness for the tones, when the frequency separation between the tones presented to the two ears was varied over a wide frequency range. In the investigation by Hall and Harvey [9] involving presentation of 2 khz pure tone at 70 and 80 db SPL, the BLDEL was found to be 3 4 db for hearing-impaired subjects and 8 9 db for normal-hearing subjects. For 2 khz tone, at 90 db, both the groups had BLDEL of about 9 db. For tone of 500 Hz, presented at the three levels, the BLDEL was about 9 db for both the groups. In a study by Hawkins et al. [10] using 4 khz pure tone with a presentation level ranging within listener s most comfortable level to the discomfort level, the BLDEL for impaired listeners was in the range of 5 12 db and it was not significantly different from that for listeners with normal hearing. Zwicker and Henning [11] conducted listening tests to match the loudness of monaurally and binaurally presented tone bursts. The presentation consisted of four binaural bursts of 60 ms each alternating with four similar bursts presented monaurally. The subjects were asked to adjust the level of one type of bursts to match the perceived loudness of the other type. The test tones were of frequency 250 Hz, 710 Hz, and 2 khz. The binaural presentation had same intensity in both the ears. An increment of 10 db was needed for the monaural sound to match the perceived loudness of the binaural sound. This difference was almost the same across the three test frequencies and presentation levels. Similar experiment was conducted by adding the stimuli to low pass filtered noise with cutoff frequency of 840 Hz, 1.5 khz, and 4 khz for the test tone of 250 Hz, 710 Hz, and 2 khz respectively. The stimuli were added with (i) in-phase noise and (ii) out-of- phase noise and then presented binaurally with varying inter-aural phase difference. For both the noise conditions, the inter-aural phase difference had a significant effect on perceived loudness for 250 Hz, a moderate effect for 710 Hz, and minimal effect for 2 khz tone bursts. Cheeran [12] conducted listening tests to find the difference in levels of monaural and binaural presentations, such that they evoke the same perceived loudness. The stimuli used were four pure tones (5, 1, 2, and 4 khz) of 1 s duration each, sustained vowel /a/, and broad-band noise. Five normal-hearing subjects participated in the listening tests. The stimulus was presented monaurally and binaurally, one after the other, with an inter-stimulus interval of 1 s. The monaural intensity was fixed at 85 db and binaural intensity was varied in the range 70 84 db. The task of the subject was to mark each binaural sound as high, same, or low depending on the perceived loudness with respect to the monaural sound. This procedure was repeated for different binaural levels. The results showed that the perceived loudness matched when binaural level was 4 12 db lower than the monaural level. Whilby et al. [13] investigated BLDEL for normal and hearing-impaired listeners using 1 khz pure tone of 5 ms and 200 ms duration. They used loudness matching procedure: (1) monaural level was fixed and binaural level was varied and (2) binaural level was fixed and monaural level was varied for equal loudness. The fixed level ranged from 10 to 90 db SL. Their study showed that BLDEL for normal-hearing subjects ranged from 2 to 15 db, and for hearing-impaired listeners it was 1.5 to 12 db. Marks [14] investigated binaural summation of loudness using pure tone stimuli of frequency 0.1,, and 1 khz. A set of nine distinct SPLs were used for left and right ears resulting in a total of 81 combinations of binaural stimuli. Fourteen normal-hearing subjects participated in the test for magnitude estimation of perceived loudness. The study showed a linear additivity of the numerical responses for loudness, for all the test tone frequencies. In the same study [14], an experiment was conducted to obtain a set of equal loudness curves at four presentation levels, using 1 khz test tone for finding various combinations of sound pressure levels to the left and right ears that produced a given level of loudness. The standard tone was presented binaurally, with equal intensity in both the ears, at the four presentation levels of 20, 30, 40, or 50 db SPL. The variable tone was also binaural, set to give a fixed intensity ratio at the two ears. However, in every match, subjects controlled the absolute levels of the left or the right ear components. During matching process, the stimulus sequence was continuous: the standard tone of 1 s duration, 1 s of silence, variable tone of 1s duration, 1 s of silence and so on. The task of the subject was to match the loudness of the variable tone, by controlling either the left or right ear components, to that of the standard tone. Three normal-hearing subjects participated in the listening test. A plot of equal loudness curves were obtained for four presentation levels of the standard tone. The shape of the curves for different presentation level was the same with a small inter-subject variation. It was obsereved that a monaural sound needed to be 5 7 db above the binaural sound for it to evoke same loudness as that of binaural. The main objective of most of these studies was to find BLDEL, for different stimuli and presentation levels, for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. However, for the design of comb filters with perceptually balanced response in the transition band, we need to know the relation between the gains of the two filters (for left and right ears) such that there are no irregular variations in the perceived loudness of spectral components in the transition bands. The aim of our investigation is to study the perceptual balance in binaural listening, as applied to spectral splitting, and hence establishing a relation between the amplitude scaling factors for the left and right ears so that perceptual balance is observed for test tones of different frequencies. METHODOLOGY The listening tests were carried out for obtaining the relation between the two amplitude scaling factors, for the left and the right ears, so that the binaural presentation evokes the same loudness as that of the monaural presentation. As shown in Fig. 1, the input signal was scaled by scaling factor α for the left ear and by β for the right ear for presentation through a pair of headphones. > Input signal a β To left ear To right ear Figure 1. Scheme for perceptual balance test 2 ICA 2010

Right ear scaling factor 23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 Signal amplitudes were scaled to compensate for any unbalance in the response of the two headphones at the test tone frequencies. The values of α and β were in the range from 0 to 1, with an increment of 0.1. The overall investigation involved two experiments. In the first experiment (Exp. I), perceptual balance was investigated for pure tones of frequencies 250 Hz, 500 Hz, 1 khz and 2 khz, presented at the most comfortable level (MCL) for the individual listener. The second experiment (Exp. II) was conducted to examine the effect of presentation level on perceptual balance. The test involved tone of 500 Hz presented at three presentation levels: MCL 6 db, MCL, and MCL + 6 db. The listening tests were conducted using three-interval, threealternative forced choice (3I, 3AFC) paradigm. Each presentation had three observation intervals: reference (monaural), test (binaural), and reference (monaural), separated by 0.5 s silences. Depending on whether the perceived loudness of the binaural sound was lower than, equal to, or higher than that of the monaural sound, the subject marked the response as L, E, or H on the response sheet. The subject could listen to the sounds more than once before finalizing the response. Eight and six subjects with normal hearing participated in Exp. I and Exp. II, respectively. For binaural presentation, different combinations of α and β were selected randomly. In Exp. I, there were a total of 484 presentations for each subject: 4 test frequencies 11 values of α 11 values of β. In Exp. II, there were a total of 363 presentations for each subject: 3 presentation levels 11 values of α 11 values of β. In both the experiments, a mean of values of β corresponding to a monaural-binaural loudness balance was calculated, for each value of α. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The results of Exp. I are summarized in Table 1. For each α, it gives the β values, averaged across the eight subjects for perceptual balance, for the four frequencies.the standard deviations, given in parentheses, are found to be small, indicating only a small inter-subject variation in the β values. Figure 2 shows a plot of values of β vs. α, obtained for perceptual balance. For all the four frequencies, the plots indicate an approximately linear relationship. The results of Exp. II are summarized in Table 2. It gives the β values for perceptual balance, for three presentation levels: MCL 6 db, MCL, and MCL + 6 db for 500 Hz tones. The standard deviations given in parentheses are similer to those in Exp. I. Figure 3 gives a plot of values of α vs. β, obtained for perceptual balance. For all the three presentation levels, the plots indicate an approximately linear relationship. A plot of scaling factors on db scale, for all the tone frequencies and the three presentation levels, is shown in Fig. 4. The shape of the curves for loudness balance is similar to those reported in the study by Marks (1978). Earlier studies have shown that loudness generally grows as a power function of sound pressure [15 18]. Assuming that the loudness of binaural sound is a power law summation of the two individual sounds [4, 19, 20], the scaling factors α and should have following relationship for perceptual balance, (α) p + (β) p = 1 (1) where, p is the power relating amplitude to the loudness. Table 1. Exp. I: Mean values of β, obtained for perceptual balance, for four test tone frequencies, (s. d. in parentheses, n = 8). Level = MCL. α Frequency (khz) 5 0.5 1.0 2.0 1.00 (0) 1.00 (0) 1.00 (0) 1.00 (0) 0.1 0.91 (4) 0.89 (6) 0.89 (8) 0.91 (5) 0.86 (5) 0.83 (8) 0.82 (8) 0.78 (7) 0.3 0.76 (7) 0.71 (7) 0.76 (7) 0.70 (0.11) 3 (8) 0.57 (9) 4 (9) 1 (0.12) 0.5 6 (9) 5 (6) 0.55 (0.10) 0.50 (0.12) 0.37 (7) 0 (9) 5 (0.11) 1 (0.15) 0.7 9 (0.10) 7 (6) 0.36 (0.14) 8 (0.11) 0.8 0 (6) 0 (6) 1 (9) 0.19 (9) 0.9 0.13 (4) 9 (3) 0.11 (6) 0.12 (8) 1.0 0.10 (3) 8 (3) 6 (5) 7 (6) 1.0 0.8 0.8 1.0 250 Hz 500 Hz 1 khz 2 khz Figure 2. Exp. I: Relation between the two scaling factors α and β for perceptual balance, for four test tone frequencies. Level: MCL. Table 2. Exp. II: Mean values of β, obtained for perceptual balance, for three presentation levels, test tone frequency: 500 Hz, (s.d. in parentheses, n = 6). α Presentation level MCL 6 db MCL MCL + 6 db 1.00 (0) 1.00 (0) 1.00 (0) 0.1 0.88 (8) 0.91 (6) 0.91 (7) 0.78 (6) 0.83 (0.10) 0.85 (8) 0.3 5 (6) 0.72 (8) 0.70 (0.10) 0.59 (0.11) 0.58 (0.10) 0.59 (7) 0.5 8 (0.13) 6 (6) 8 (5) 0.30 (0.13) 1 (0.10) 0.36 (0.11) 0.7 0.19 (0.10) 6 (7) 3 (6) 0.8 6 (6) 0 (7) 4 (4) 0.9 2 (3) 0.10 (3) 2 (3) 1.0 0 (0) 9 (2) 2 (3) ICA 2010 3

Right ear scaling factor Right ear scaling factor RMS error 23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 1.0 0.8 MCL + 6 db MCL MCL - 6 db 250 Hz 500 Hz 1 khz 2 khz 0.8 1.0 Figure 3. Exp. II Relation between the two scaling factors α and β on for perceptual balance, for three presentation levels taking MCL as the reference, tone frequency = 500 Hz. 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 p Figure 5. RMS error in approximation of β as computed from Eqn. 1 shown as a function of p. 10 0-10 -20-30 -40 MCL, 250 Hz MCL, 500 Hz MCL, 1 khz MCL, 2 khz MCL+6, 500 Hz MCL- 6, 500 Hz -40-30 -20-10 0 10 Figure 4. Relation between the two scaling factors (α and β) on db scale, for perceptual balance, for three presentation levels and four frequencies. To find an approximate fit to the observed values of β, its values were computed for different values of p (0.3,, 0.8, 1.0, 1.2, and 2), from Eqn. 1. Figure 5 shows the RMS error, in the approximation of β using the power law addition model, as a function of p. For all the four frequencies, minimum error was observed for p 1, indicating that perceptual balance is achieved by α + β 1. Various ranges for the BLDEL have been reported in the earlier investigations: 5 7 db [8], 8 9 db [9], 5 12 db [10], 4 12 db [12], and 2 15 db [13]. In the current investigation, perceptual balance is obtained for the binaural sound when the two amplitude scaling factors are nearly linearly related which means a BLDEL of about 6 db. Thus, the BLDEL obtained in the current investigation nearly falls in the middle of the various ranges of BLDEL reported in the previous investigations. Figure 6. Sum of the left and right ear gains (G l (f) + G r (f)) for different comb filters used for spectral splitting. ACB: filters based on auditory critical bandwidth and designed with perceptually balanced responses, CHE: filters used by Cheeran and Pandey (2004), LUN: Lunner et al. (1993) LYR: Lyregaard (1982). A pair of comb filters based on the auditory critical bandwidth (ACB) and magnitude responses closely satisfying the requirement of perceptual balance were designed as 512- coefficient linear phase FIR filters, at sampling frequency of 10 khz, with pass band ripple less than 1 db and minimum stop band attenuation of 30 db. The magnitude responses of the comb filters used in earlier studies [1, 2, 4] and ACB filters based on perceptual balance were examined for the condition of magnitude responses being complementary on a linear scale. Figure 6 shows a plot of sum of the filter gains, as a function of frequency, for pairs of comb filters as reported earlier by Lyregaard [1], Lunner et al. [2], Cheeran and Pandey [4], along with the ACB filters used in the present investigation. It is observed that the filters used in the current study have much smaller deviations from the condition for perceptual balance as compaired to other filters. Hence these are not likely to introduce any perceptual imbalance due to dichotic presentation. The effectiveness of spectral splitting scheme based on auditory critical bandwidth (ACB) filters was assessed by conducting listening tests, for recognition of consonants, using 4 ICA 2010

23-27 August 2010, Sydney, Australia Proceedings of 20th International Congress on Acoustics, ICA 2010 modified rhyme test (MRT), on six subjects with nornalhearing in the presence of broad-band masking noise. At 75 % recognition score, improvement in the recognition scores corresponding to an SNR advantage of 12 db was observed. Listening tests conducted on 12 subjects with moderate bilateral sensorineural hearing loss showed improvement in the recognition scores in the range 7 31 %. Processing reduced the response time for both the group of subjects (4-0.33 s for normal-hearing subjects and 4-0.57 s for hearingimpaired subjects), indicating a reduced load on perception process. CONCLUSIONS The comb filters used in spectral splitting, for binaural dichotic presentattion to improve speech perception by persons with moderate bilateral sensorineural loss, should have perceptually balanced magnitude responses. The objective of the present study was to investigate the relationship between the two signal amplitudes in binaural presentation which will evoke the same loudness as the monaural presentation. Listening tests conducted with 250 Hz, 500 Hz, 1 khz, and 2 khz pure tones, showed that the sum of the two amplitude scaling factors, should be approximately constant, indicating that the magnitude responses of the comb filters should be complementary on a linear scale. An examination of magnitude responses of the comb filters used in the earlier studies showed a large deviation from the requirement for perceptual balance. A set of comb filters with linear phase responses and magnitude responses nearly complementary on a linear scale was designed, and its effectiveness in improving the speech perception, was assessed by conducting modified rhyme test (MRT) on normal-hearing subjects in the presence of broad-band masking noise and on subjects with moderate bilateral sensorineural hearing loss. These tests showed a significant improvement in speech perception for both the groups of subjects. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT The research is partly supported by a project grant under the National Programme on Perception Engineering, sponsored by the Department of Information Technology, MCIT, Government of India. REFERENCES 1 P. E. Lyregaard, Frequency selectivity and speech intelligibility in noise, Scand. Audiol. Suppl. 15, 113 122 (1982). 2 T. Lunner, S. Arlinger, and J. Hellgren, 8-channel digital filter bank for hearing aid use: preliminary results in monaural, diotic, and dichotic modes, Scand. Audiol. Suppl. 38, 75 81 (1993). 3 D. S. Chaudhari and P. C. Pandey, Dichotic presentation of speech signal using critical filter band for bilateral sensorineural hearing impairment, Proc. 16 th Int. Congress Acoust. (ICA), Seattle, Washington (1998). 4 A. N. Cheeran and P. C. Pandey, Evaluation of speech processing schemes using binaural dichotic presentation to reduce the effect of masking in hearing-impaired listeners, Proc. 18 th Int. Congress Acoust. (ICA), Kyoto, Japan, II, 1523 1526 (2004). 5 A. Murase, F. Nakajima, S. Sakamoto, Y. Suzuki, and T. Kawase, Effect and sound localization with dichoticlistening hearing aids, Proc. 18 th Int. Congress Acoust. (ICA), Kyoto, Japan, II-1519 1522, 2004. 6 P. N. Kulkarni, P. C. Pandey, and D. S. Jangamashetti, Perceptually balanced filter response for binaural dichotic presentation to reduce the effect of spectral masking, (abstract) J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120, 3253 (2006). 7 B. Scharf, Binaural loudness summation as a function of bandwidth, Proc. 6 th Int. Congress Acoust. (ICA), Tokyo. 25 28, 1968. 8 B. Scharf, Dichotic summation of loudness, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 45, 1193 1205 (1969). 9 J. H. Hall, and A. D. Harvey, Diotic loudness summation in normal and impaired hearing, J. Speech Hear. Res. 28, 445 448 (1985). 10 D. B. Hawkins, R. A. Prosek, B. E. Walden, and A. A. Montgomery, Binaural loudness summation in hearing impaired, J. Speech Hear. Res. 30, 37 43 (1987). 11 E. Zwicker, and G. B. Henning, On the effect of interaural phase differences on loudness, Hearing Research. 53, 141 152 (1991). 12 A. N. Cheeran, Speech processing with dichotic presentation for binaural hearing aids for moderate bilateral sensorineural loss, Ph.D. Thesis, School of Biosciences and Bioengineering, Indian Institute of Technology Bombay, India, 2005. 13 S. Whilby, M. Florentine, E. Wagner, and J. Marozeau, Monaural and binaural loudness of 5 and 200 ms tones in normal and impaired hearing, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 119, 3931 3939 (2006). 14 L. E. Marks, Binaural summation of the loudness of pure tones, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 64, 107 113 (1978). 15 S. S. Stevens, The direct estimation of sensory magnitudes - loudness, Am. J. Psychol. 69, 1 15 (1956). 16 G. S. Reynolds and S. S. Stevens, Binaural summation of loudness, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 32, 1337 1344 (1960). 17 J. C. Stevens, and M. Guirao, Individual loudness functions, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 36, 2210 2213 (1964). 18 B. Scharf, and D. Fishken, Binaural summation of loudness: Reconsidered, J. Exp. Psychol. 86, 374 379 ( 1970). 19 H. Fletcher, and W. A. Munson, Loudness, its definition, measurement and calculation, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 5, 82 108 (1933). 20 R. P. Hellman and J. J. Zwislocki Monaural loudness summation at 1000 cps and interaural summation, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 35, 856 865 (1963). ICA 2010 5