IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor

Similar documents
Support of Mission and Work Scheduling by a Biomedical Fatigue Model

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede

THE PREVALENCE of shiftwork has substantially

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY

Quantitative measurements of sleepiness

Individualized Management of Fatigue and Cognitive Performance Impairment through Biomathematical Modeling

The Hidden Dangers of Fatigue

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center

Fatigue Management for the 21st Century

Comparison of Mathematical Model Predictions to Experimental Data of Fatigue and Performance

TECHNOLOGICAL ADVANCEMENTS, the global. Summary of the Key Features of Seven Biomathematical Models of Human Fatigue and Performance

An introduction to the new EU fatigue management framework (Reg. 83/2014)

Predicting Sleep/Wake Behaviour in Operational Settings

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p.

Individualized performance prediction of sleep-deprived individuals with the two-process model

THE U.S. DEPARTMENT OF DEFENSE (DOD) has. Fatigue Models for Applied Research in Warfighting

Getting Real About Biomathematical Fatigue Models

EFFECTS OF SCHEDULING ON SLEEP AND PERFORMANCE IN COMMERCIAL MOTORCOACH OPERATIONS

THE U.S. DEPARTMENT OF DEFENSE (DOD) has. Fatigue Models for Applied Research in Warfighting

Data Collection Best Practices How to Manage Common Missteps

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

Caffeine dosing strategies to optimize alertness during sleep loss

Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard. Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

When are you too tired to be safe?

TITLE: Homeostatic and Circadian Abnormalities in Sleep and Arousal in Gulf War Syndrome

RESEARCH REPORT 446. The development of a fatigue / risk index for shiftworkers HSE

Managing Sleep to Sustain Performance

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Sleep Deprivation, Fatigue and Effects on Performance The Science and Its Implications for Resident Duty Hours

Fatigue Risk Management

PREDICTIONS OF SLEEP TIMING DURING LAYOVERS ON INTERNATIONAL FLIGHT PATTERNS USING SOCIAL AND CIRCADIAN FACTORS

SMARTCAP VALIDATION. Independent assessment from Universidad de Chile

Priorities in Occupation Health and Safety: Fatigue. Assoc. Prof. Philippa Gander, PhD Director, Sleep/Wake Research Centre

Journal of Theoretical Biology

Implementation from an Airline Perspective: Challenges and Opportunities

Biomathematical Fatigue Modelling in Civil Aviation Fatigue Risk Management. Application Guidance

Prediction of Performance during Sleep Deprivation and Alcohol Intoxication using a Quantitative Model of Work-Related Fatigue

Relations Between Performance and Subjective Ratings of Sleepiness During a Night Awake

Airport Operations. Research Suggests That Some Rotating Work Shift Schedules Do Not Harm Air Traffic Controllers Sleep Patterns

Translating Fatigue Research into Technologic Countermeasures

Bi-directional Relationship Between Poor Sleep and Work-related Stress: Management through transformational leadership and work organization

A Novel Capsule Neural Network Based Model For Drowsiness Detection Using Electroencephalography Signals

A Model for Truck Driver Scheduling with Fatigue Management. Zeb Bowden & Cliff Ragsdale Virginia Tech Transportation Institute

ASLEF. More than. just a union. Rostering Best Practice THE TRAIN DRIVERS UNION

Adaptation of performance during a week of simulated night work

Shift Work, Sleep, Health, Safety, and Solutions. Prof Philippa Gander PhD, FRSNZ Sleep/Wake Research Centre Massey University

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Key FM scientific principles

Evaluation of In-Car Countermeasures to Sleepiness: Cold Air and Radio

Structure of the presentation. 1. Introduction 2. Risk factors of night work. 3. Risk reduction strategies. 4. Recommendations

Alertness and Sleep Health Management Research & Technology. Strategic Opportunity Overview

MANAGING FATIGUE AND SHIFT WORK. Prof Philippa Gander PhD, FRSNZ

NIGHT FIT AT SEAWAY HEAVY LIFTING. Improving the quality of sleep, health and safety at the Oleg Strashnov KM HUMAN FACTORS ENGINEERING

Citation for published version (APA): Lammers-van der Holst, H. M. (2016). Individual differences in shift work tolerance

Fatigue Risk Management

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

FATIGUE RISK MANAGEMENT GUIDING PHILOSOPHY TEAMSTERS CANADA RAIL CONFERENCE

Fitness for duty checks and predicting the likelihood of experiencing fatigue Appendices to the report

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms

Rest Stop #101. Sleep & Fatigue-What s the difference and what to do about it.

Abstract. Learning Objectives: 1. Even short duration of acute sleep deprivation can have a significant degradation of mental alertness.

IMPROVING SAFETY: FATIGUE RISK MANAGEMENT

Sleep, Circadian Rhythms, and Psychomotor Vigilance

A Reliability Study of The Turkish Version of The Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire

Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task

Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Journal of Theoretical Biology

Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents

Decision graph

PDF created with FinePrint pdffactory Pro trial version

Rodney Heller LC, CLEP Senior Lighting Designer & Lighting Evangelist Energy Performance Lighting Cottage Grove, WI

Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Sleep Medicine. journal homepage:

RAA Convention Fatigue Science Initiatives

Sleep and Fatigue Issues in Continuous Operations: A Survey of U.S. Army Officers

Napping on the Night Shift: A Study of Sleep, Performance, and Learning in Physicians-in-Training

Survey about Sleepiness and Adaptation to Night- Shift Workers in Metallurgy Industry

The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing

COMPENSATING FATIGUE BY CAFFEINE

November 24, External Advisory Board Members:

Fatigue in Transit Operations

Implementing Fatigue Risk Management System

Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study.

BIOMATHEMATICAL MODELS of fatigue and performance

PERCLOS: AN ALERTNESS MEASURE OF THE PAST. Circadian Technologies, Inc., Stoneham, Massachusetts, USA

Managing Fatigue in the Workplace

Fatigue. Edzard Boland, HF Research Exchange 6 april 2016

Innovative Fatigue Management Approach in the Trucking Industry

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017

Validation of Fatigue Modeling Predictions in Aviation Operations

ANALYZING IRREGULAR WORKING HOURS LESSONS LEARNED IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF RAS 1.0 THE REPRESENTATION & ANALYSIS SOFTWARE

Sleep Patients with Daytime SOREMs Exhibit More Daytime Sleepiness but Similar Fatigue

FATIGUE MANAGEMENT & MITIGATION

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

Transcription:

Commentary on the Three-Process of Alertness and Broader ing Issues Jaques Reifman and Philippa Gander REIFMAN J, GANDER P. Commentary on the three-process model of alertness and broader modeling issues. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(3, Suppl.):A84 8. IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor was a conceptually simple model that predicted group mean alertness based on the addition of the instantaneous values of three independent processes (9): 1. A sinusoidal circadian process (Process C, sometimes considered as two separately modifiable components, Process C during wake and Process C during sleep); 2. A homeostatic process that declines exponentially toward a minimum during wake (Process S) and increases exponentially toward a maximum during sleep (Process S ); and 3. A sleep inertia process, which results in depressed alertness on awakening, and then exponentially improves toward Process S over 3 h (Process W). Hence, predicted alertness was estimated as the sum of C S - W with 8 h assumed to provide sufficient time for full recovery of Process S. The original version of the model was derived from group mean alertness ratings on a visual analog scale, collected 6 times a day from 12 young volunteers in a 3-wk time-isolation study. For the first 3 d, the participants lived on a 24-h schedule, which was then gradually shortened to 22 h across the remaining 18 d of the study. The circadian and sleep-related components of alertness were derived by comparing the rhythm during entrained conditions and forced internal desynchrony. Regression analyses were then used to compare model predictions with self-reported alertness or fatigue ratings from studies examining normal sleep/ waking schedules, total sleep deprivation for 64 75 h, and abnormal sleep/wake schedules in shift work. Although it is described as a three-process model, Process W is typically omitted from simulations on the basis that the data being modeled do not address functioning during the first 3 h after awakening. In effect, the model is a version of the Borbély two-process model (5). A series of papers published between 1995 and 2000 (1 4,9 11) have explored the model s capacity to predict a range of different outputs, but in each case these involve changes to model parameters and, in some cases, to inputs. This model evolution is summarized in Table I. During this evolution, basic assumptions of the model have been modified over time to improve model fit to the particular data set being modeled. For example, in the simulations of sleep length (3), the previously established set of model parameter values significantly underestimated the duration of daytime sleep episodes. This lead to the proposition that there was circadian modification of the Process S during sleep, in contradiction to the basic model assumption that alertness is the simple arithmetic sum of the model outputs. The best-fitting simulations also used a different acrophase of Process C. To model alertness across successive night shifts (10,11), a series of new assumptions and parameters were introduced. First, it was assumed that the time of waking up sets the phase of Process C. Again, this contradicts the basic model assumption that alertness is the simple arithmetic sum of the outputs of. Second, a first night compensation effect was introduced because shift workers rated themselves as progressively more alert than would be predicted over the course of the first night shift. It was also necessary to model a general lowering of alertness on the second night shift, which was proposed to reflect a cost of the first night shift effect. Third, a time on shift effect was introduced, whereby on night shifts after the first, alertness ratings decreased over the course of each shift before showing a modest improvement at the end of the shift. These new assumptions and parameters are presumably relevant to data sets simulated with earlier formulations of the model, for example, changes in EEG alpha power across the night shift among truck drivers and locomotive engineers. However, the effects of adding these new parameters on model predictions for these other data sets have not been reported. From the Telemedicine and Advanced Research Center, U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command, Ft. Detrick, MD (J. Reifman); and the Sleep/Wake Research Center, Massey University Wellington Campus, Wellington, New Zealand (P. Gander). Address reprint requests to: Jaques Reifman, Ph.D., who is a Senior Research Scientist, U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command, MCMR-AT, 504 Scott St., Ft. Detrick, MD 21702-5012; reifman@tatrc.org. Reprint & Copyright by Aerospace Medical Association, Alexandria, VA. A84 Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol. 75, No. 3, Section II March 2004

TABLE I. SUMMARY OF THE EVOLUTION OF THE SLEEP-WAKE PREDICTOR MODEL. Data Set (ref. #) Measure Simulated Objective Sleepiness 15 male truck drivers (1) EEG alpha power during night drives 11 male train drivers (1) EEG alpha power during night drives 8 males in time isolation (1) EEG alpha power during 28 h without sleep Subjective Sleepiness 8 males in time isolation (1) KSS rating during 28 h with 2 h, 4 h, or 8 h sleep. Objective Sleep Latency 2 groups of 8 males in time Sleep latency after isolation (2) wakefulness of 16 40 h. 8 females in time isolation (2) Sleep latency after wakefulness of 4 12 h Process C Parameters Output 1/() exponential 20:48 r 2 0.80 exponential 20:48 r 2 0.94 exponential 19:48 r 2 0.95 1/() 20:48 r 2 0.70 exponential 20:48 r 2 0.88 20:48 r 2 0.65 Best-Fit Regression 25 males working a rotating 3-shift system (2) Sleep latency before or after different shifts 20:48 N/A errors in both directions, depending on shift 11 male train drivers (2) Sleep latency before or after different shifts Objective Sleep Length 2 groups of 8 males in time isolation (3) Sleep duration after wakefulness of 16 40 h 20:48 amplitude 0.6 asleep at bedtime 16:48 r 2 0.99 N/A errors in both directions, depending on shift 25 males working a rotating 3-shift system (3) Sleep duration before or after different shifts amplitude 0.6 16:48 awake at S C 14.2, intercept correction 0.1 r 2 0.99 11 male train drivers (3) Sleep duration before or after different shifts amplitude 0.6 20:48, intercept correction 0.6 r 2 0.95 6 male normal day workers in time isolation (3) Sleep duration after wakefulness of 4 h Auditory Vigilance Performance 12 males in time isolation (4) Hits on a 30-min task across 64 h of sleep deprivation Alertness Across Shifts Parameter setting 1,114 shift workers on 6 rotating shift systems (four 8-h shift systems, two 12-h shift systems) (10) Retrospective subjective sleepiness (KSS) 20:48 16:48 r 2 0.79 r 2 0.34 to 0.59 r 2 0.63 to 0.88 Parameter setting 23 workers on two rotating 12-h shift systems (10) 2-hourly alertness ratings over 28 days 16:48 r 2 0.23 to 0.35 r 2 0.40 to 0.42 r 2 0.40 to 0.42 (wakeup 11 h) r 2 0.47 to 0.50 Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol. 75, No. 3, Section II March 2004 A85

TABLE I. CONTINUED. Data Set (ref. #) Measure Simulated Process C Parameters Output Best-fit Regression Predicting Accident Risk 3 studies of 8-h shift systems (no data on sleep times, modelers assumed them) (11) accident risk on afternoon and night shifts versus morning shift accident risk across 4 night shifts mean 1/(S C) across shift mean 1/(S C) across shift 1st night effect 2 nd night effect time on task component r 2 0.89 r 2 0.97 General ing Issues and the Sleep/Wake Predictor Many of the limitations of the current generation of models of neurobehavioral function may have a common explanation. They are all, in one form or another, variations of Borbély s seminal two-process model, which was originally developed solely to explain sleep regulation (5). Borbély s model was not formulated to predict human fatigue and performance, let alone trends in accident risk, which are the outcomes that the current generation of models are trying to predict. Hence, it is unlikely that model enhancements and refinements around Borbély s original model will provide the answers and the new insights needed to address the well-known research gaps in predicting human performance subject to sleep deprivation across the circadian cycle (7,8). Given the common theoretical basis of the current models, it is also not surprising that there is not great variability in model performance one model performing better in one scenario and worse in another (16). Following, we discuss four modeling issues: 1) model validation and refinement; 2) predicting group vs. individual performance; 3) model prediction uncertainty; and 4) scaling of model outputs within the context of the Sleep/Wake Predictor. However, as articulated above, these issues are prevalent and equally relevant throughout the current generation of sleep and performance models. Validation and Refinement The Sleep/Wake Predictor does not follow a conventional life cycle development protocol of systematic development, validation, and refinement leading to a comprehensive explanation of the underlying mechanisms of sleepiness and alertness. Rather, its evolution appears unsystematic, with ad-hoc adjustments made as necessary, and without reference to formal hypotheses to account for observed model inaccuracies. Whenever the existing model fails to explain new experimental data, new components are added and model parameters are fitted to the new data in order to minimize the differences between predicted and observed values. This is potentially a never-ending process, with the model being continually adjusted as new field and laboratory data become available. For example, as discussed above, to enhance model predictive power for the trend in accident risk among rotating shift workers, Folkard et al. (10,11) assumed that the phase of Process C was variable and dependent on the time of waking. Similar ad hoc adjustments are made in other models. For instance, in the SAFTE, Johnson et al. (12) modified the reservoir concept to avoid performance over predictions during the recovery phase of individuals who are subject to chronic sleep restriction. While this approach allows for a model to be tailored to the specific dataset of interest, it provides no insight on the model s predictive power or generalization ability. There is no way of knowing whether the refined model has an improved ability to generalize to new, unanticipated conditions and scenarios. Furthermore, the tailored model may no longer be predictive for data sets simulated with earlier formulations of the model. As pointed out above, it is not clear if the tailoring of the Sleep/Wake Predictor to predict alertness across successive night shifts (10,11) is applicable to earlier data sets. s derived in this fashion cannot be characterized quantitatively, so it cannot be determined, a priori, to which set of scenarios/conditions the models are applicable and to which they are not. Just knowing when a given model should or should not be employed would be extremely useful. To improve understanding of the underlying neurobehavioral mechanisms, it is more informative to examine how model predictions differ systematically from observations, rather than modifying or adding parameters to improve the fit. In this context, the use of regression models to describe overall fit is unhelpful without consideration of the structure of the residuals. Furthermore, R 2 statistics do not identify at which times of day model predictions are weakest, although this may be of key theoretical and operational significance. Group Prediction vs. Individual Prediction Another characteristic of the Sleep/Wake Predictor that is common across the other models is the focus on the prediction of group averages, necessarily de-emphasizing individual variation in neurobehav- A86 Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol. 75, No. 3, Section II March 2004

ioral functioning. This focus is inconsistent with the recent trend for models to move from being basic research tools to practical field-applicable tools used to optimize work scheduling. Because measures of performance may vary substantially from individual to individual, even if a model is capable of accurately predicting mean group performance, this capability would be of limited benefit without knowing how this translates into predicting the performance of a given individual. In many situations, such as mission-critical operations prevalent in the military environment and in commercial aviation, the emphasis is on the performance of one or two key individuals whose performance could be substantially different from those of a group. It would be extremely valuable if the models could predict individual neurobehavioral function and/or estimate the range of inter-individual variations around group predictions, i.e., estimate the prediction error. However, these capabilities may require a completely new modeling paradigm (6,15) and much greater understanding of sleep and human behavior. One potential approach to model individual performance that would be highly applicable to the military environment is to develop personal models based on individualized on-line measurements of physiologic and non-physiologic variables, such as core body temperature, levels of light exposure, and sleep/wake history (15). This approach may require the coupling of existing parametric models with nonparametric models, such as artificial neural networks, in the development of hybrid models that in addition to being amenable to predicting individual performance could also quantitatively assess the reliability of model predictions through estimation of statistical error bounds. In addition to the intrinsic large data requirements posed in the development of hybrid individualized fatigue and performance models, a related issue is how a personal model handles missing data (for example, sleep times). This is a common occurrence in real-world applications, and the substitution of group data may be inadequate for predicting individual performance. Estimating Prediction Uncertainties The applicability of prediction models, be it for predicting group-average or individual performance, is of limited benefit if the model does not also provide a theoretically sound approach for quantitatively estimating the precision of the model predictions for new data for which the outcomes are not known. Thus, in addition to the predicted model value, the model should employ algorithms that simultaneously provide statistical error bounds (such as standard error, confidence intervals, and prediction intervals) to quantitatively determine the bounds within which the predictions may be trusted for a pre-defined coverage probability, e.g., with 95% confidence. The algorithms should explicitly allow for the incorporation of the distribution of the data employed in model development (i.e., the training data) in the estimation of error bars for new data. Hence, when a prediction is made in a region where the training data are sparse, the error bounds should be wide, indicating less reliability in the resulting prediction. In contrast, in regions where the training data are dense, the bounds should be narrower because the model would have been exposed to similar patterns during model development. This would allow for a quantitative assessment of the accuracy of the model predictions, affording their use when error bounds are within pre-specified range and avoiding their usage when the quality of the prediction deteriorates. Unfortunately, the nature of the modeling approach employed in the Sleep/Wake Predictor as well as the other neurobehavioral function models does not lend itself to the evaluation of prediction uncertainties. Formal statistical methods are not routinely employed in developing the models and, therefore, the uncertainty in inferences based on differential equation models and algebraic models and their sensitivity to model specification and parameter estimation error cannot be evaluated (6). Nevertheless, the model by Moore-Ede et al. (14) attempts to infer lower and upper limits about the model prediction. These limits are obtained by employing a proprietary database, rerunning the model with individuals from the database with different conditions, and associating the minimum and maximum predicted alertness values for any individual in the database with the lower and upper limits, respectively. Due to a lack of statistical underpinning in this approach, it is unlikely that the suggested limits around the predictions are meaningful. None of the other models presented at the workshop, however, attempted to provide an assessment of the accuracy of the model predictions. Research is needed to address this fundamental issue and provide statistically based algorithms that make quantitative, objective statements of the reliability of the model predictions. reliability cannot be assured if reliability cannot be quantified. This quantification of model reliability might hold the key for enabling the use of neurobehavioral function models for practical, mission-critical applications. Again, achieving this capability might require developing models from scratch based on modeling paradigms that lend themselves to these functionalities. Scaling of Outputs The outputs of the neurobehavioral functional models are internally scaled into arbitrary numerical units, which are subsequently mapped or normalized to different metrics corresponding to different subjective and objective measures of alertness and performance. Because each model output is normalized to its own arbitrary subjective and objective metric, the models cannot be directly compared with each other and neither can they be directly compared with performance test measurements, such as the Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) and the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS). It is not clear how these normalizations are performed and validated and what sort of mapping function, e.g.,, non, is employed. It is intriguing and disturbing, however, that a model not designed (i.e., normalized) to predict subjective alertness outperforms models tuned to do so in certain applications [see the subjective alertness results of Scenario 2 in Van Dongen Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol. 75, No. 3, Section II March 2004 A87

(16)]. The larger issue, though, is whether the outputs provided by the model (e.g., subjective alertness, objective alertness) are appropriate measures of the outcome that the model is trying to predict (e.g., accident risk, functional field performance) (13). This is of particular concern as the Sleep/Wake Predictor (and other models) are being proposed to predict accident risk when, in fact, the links between sleep regulation, alertness, and work performance are not well established. Clearly, there is a need to standardize the metrics that models are mapped to and to validate the relationship between the output measures provided by the models and the actual field performance measures of interest. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors thank the reviewers for their constructive feedback. Jaques Reifman was supported by the Combat Casualty Care and Military Operational Medicine research programs of the U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command, Ft. Detrick, MD. Philippa Gander was supported by the University of Otago, the Wellington Medical Research Foundation, and the Maurice and Phyllis Paykel Trust. The opinions or assertions contained herein are the private views of the authors and are not to be construed as official or as reflecting the views of the U.S. Army or the U.S. Department of Defense. REFERENCES 1. Åkerstedt T, Folkard S. Validation of the S and C components of the three-process model of alertness regulation. Sleep 1995; 18:1 6. 2. Åkerstedt T, Folkard S. Predicting sleep latency from the threeprocess model of alertness regulation. Psychophysiology 1996; 33:385 9. 3. Åkerstedt T, Folkard S. Predicting duration of sleep from the three-process model of alertness regulation. Occup Environ Med 1996; 53:136 41. 4. Åkerstedt, T, Folkard S. The three-process model of alertness and its extension to performance, sleep latency, and sleep length. Chronobiol Int 1997; 1997:14:115 23. 5. Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol 1982; 1:195 204. 6. Brown EN, Luithardt H. Statistical model building and model criticism for human circadian data. J Biol Rhythms 1999; 14: 609 16. 7. Dinges DF, Achermann P. Commentary: Future considerations for models of human neurobehavioral function. J Biol Rhythms 1999; 14:598 601. 8. Dinges DF. Critical research issues in development of biomathematical models of fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(3, Suppl.):A181 91. 9. Folkhard S, Åkerstedt T. A three-process model of the regulation of alertness-sleepiness. In: Broughton RJ, Ogilvie R, eds. Sleep, Arousal and Performance: Problems and Promises. Boston: Birkhäuser; 1992:11 26. 10. Folkard S, Åkerstedt T, Macdonald I, et al. Beyond the threeprocess model of alertness: estimating phase, time on shift and successive night effects. J Biol Rhythms 1999; 14:577 87. 11. Folkard S, Åkerstedt T, Macdonald I, et al. Refinement of the three-process model of alertness to account for trend in accident risk. In: Hornberger S, Knauth P, Costa G, Folkard S, eds. Shiftwork in the 21 st Century. Frankfurt: Peter Lang; 2000:49 54. 12. Johnson MJ, Belenky G, Redmond DP, et al. Modulating the homeostatic process to predict performance during chronic sleep restriction. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(3, Suppl.): A141 6. 13. Klerman EB, Jewett ME. Commentary. building, quantitative testing, and model comparison. J Biol Rhythms 1999; 14: 621 4. 14. Moore-Ede M, Heitmann A, Croke D, et al. Circadian alertness simulator for fatigue risk assessment in transportation: Application to reduce frequency and severity of truck accidents. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(3, Suppl.):A107 18. 15. Reifman J. Alternative methods for modeling fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(3, Suppl.):A173 80. 16. Van Dogen HPA. Comparison of mathematical model predictions to experimental data of fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med 2004; 75(3, Suppl.):A15 36. A88 Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol. 75, No. 3, Section II March 2004