Edinburgh Research Explorer

Similar documents
Edinburgh Research Explorer

GENDER AND AGE DIFFERENCES ON EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE SCALES OF CHILDREN YEARS OLD: PARENTS REPORT ABSTRACT

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

Reply To: Intranasal Oxytocin Mechanisms Can Be Better Understood, but Its Effects on Social Cognition and Behavior Are Not to Be Sniffed At

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Bahria Journal of Professional Psychology, January 2014, Vol-13, 1, 44-63

Can trait Emotional Intelligence. predict differences in attainment and progress. in secondary school?

IJBARR E- ISSN X ISSN ASSESSMENT OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND ACADEMIC MOTIVATION IN SCHOOL GIRLS

The present study investigated the location of trait

WORKSHOP Maximum Performance Measures of EI: Consensus and Controversy

Wellbeing Measurement Framework for Colleges

Edinburgh Research Explorer

The Relationship of Trait EI with Personality, IQ and Sex in a UK Sample of Employees

Reliability and Validity of the Divided

ABSTRACT. Field of Research: Academic achievement, Emotional intelligence, Gifted students.

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Analysis of the Reliability and Validity of an Edgenuity Algebra I Quiz

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Running head: INCREMENTAL VALIDITY OF THE TEIQue ASF 1. Incremental Validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Adolescent Short

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Effectively coping with task stress: a study of the validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire-Short Form (TEIQue-SF)

Is low Emotional Intelligence a primary causal factor in drug and alcohol addiction?

Self Compassion and Emotional Intelligence of Engineering and Dental College Students

Edinburgh Research Explorer

When Getting Angry Is Smart: Emotional Preferences and Emotional Intelligence

empathy A preliminary study of empathy, emotional intelligence and examination performance in MBChB students INTRODUCTION

Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

Factor structure and correlates of the Mandarin version of the Managing the Emotions of Others (MEOS) scale

Personality Traits And Emotional Intelligence As Predictors Of Learning English And Math Alireza Homayouni a *

Online publication date: 10 August 2010 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Differential Effect of Socio-Demographic Factors on Emotional Intelligence of Secondary School Students in Ernakulam District

Relationship Between Emotional Intelligence And Job Satisfaction Among Health Workers

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1

Do Self-Perceptions of Emotional Intelligence Predict Health-Related Quality of Life? A Case Study in Hospital Managers in Greece

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE: THE EFFECT ON SOCIAL MEDIA USE, GENDER, AND INTERPERSONAL VIOLENCE. Presented by: Gail Grabczynski April 21, 2018

Alternative Methods for Assessing the Fit of Structural Equation Models in Developmental Research

The Internal Structure of Responses to the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Short-Form: An

Psychology in Your Life

ABHINAV NATIONAL MONTHLY REFEREED JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN COMMERCE & MANAGEMENT

The role of emotional intelligence and personality in moral reasoning

Critical Evaluation of the Beach Center Family Quality of Life Scale (FQOL-Scale)

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Examining the Mediating Effect. Relationship between Emotional

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Personality and Individual Differences

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

Proactive interference and practice effects in visuospatial working memory span task performance

The dual pathway model of overeating Ouwens, Machteld; van Strien, T.; van Leeuwe, J. F. J.; van der Staak, C. P. F.

The SELF Page 1 of 5

VALIDATION OF TWO BODY IMAGE MEASURES FOR MEN AND WOMEN. Shayna A. Rusticus Anita M. Hubley University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Trait Emotional Intelligence: Protecting. Health from the Negative Impact of. Stress. Susan Jane Ould BSc (Hons)

Information Structure for Geometric Analogies: A Test Theory Approach

Trait emotional intelligence and decision-related emotions

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Statistical Interaction between Two Continuous (Latent) Variables

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Validating Measures of Self Control via Rasch Measurement. Jonathan Hasford Department of Marketing, University of Kentucky

Response latencies are alive and well for identifying fakers on a self-report personality inventory: A reconsideration of van Hooft and Born (2012)

Emotional Intelligence and Task Performance. Nicola S. Schutte*, Edward Schuettpelz**, and John M. Malouff*

Techniques for Explaining Item Response Theory to Stakeholder

Examining the Psychometric Properties of The McQuaig Occupational Test

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Preface INTRODUCTION WHY LEADERSHIP AND EDUCATION? When we are connected to others, we become better people. -Randy Pausch

Replications and Refinements. Effects of Own Versus Other s Fair Treatment on Positive Emotions: A Field Study

Improving business performance with emotional intelligence. Genos emotional intelligence products and services overview

Motivating emotional intelligence: A reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) perspective

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LIFE SATISFACTION AND ATTACHMENT STYLES WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF CHILDREN ON COVERED BY BEHZISTEY IN TEHRAN

Personality and Self-Esteem in Newlyweds. Todd K. Shackelford. Oakland University. Richard L. Michalski. Hollins University

Factor Analysis. MERMAID Series 12/11. Galen E. Switzer, PhD Rachel Hess, MD, MS

emotional intelligence questionnaire

Emotional Intelligence and its Association with Physical Fitness among University Students in India

Version The trait emotional intelligence (trait EI) model successfully integrates and extends EIrelated

FAMILY ENVIRONMENT AND LOCUS OF CONTROL AS DETERMINANTS OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AMONG UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS IN BENUE STATE, NIGERIA

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

Personality Down Under: Perspectives from Australia

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

The Role of Emotional Intelligence in Academic Performance of Male and Female Students in the University of the Punjab

Running head: INCREMENTAL VALIDITY OF THE TEIQue 1. The Incremental Validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue):

EVALUATING AND IMPROVING MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

Tasks of Executive Control TEC. Score Report. Developed by Peter K. Isquith, PhD, Robert M. Roth, PhD, Gerard A. Gioia, PhD, and PAR Staff

Survey Sampling Weights and Item Response Parameter Estimation

A Race Model of Perceptual Forced Choice Reaction Time

Trait Emotional Intelligence and Children s Peer Relations at School

NEO-PI-R Factor Structure in College Students

A Behavioral-Genetic Study of Alexithymia and its Relationships with Trait Emotional Intelligence

Supplemental material on graphical presentation methods to accompany:

Interaction Effects: Centering, Variance Inflation Factor, and Interpretation Issues

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I)

Literature review Solimar Miranda

Development Of Scales To Measure Psychosocial Care Needs Of Children With Seizures And Their Parents. (part 1): An Article From: Journal Of

Relation between emotional intelligence and behavioral symptoms in delinquent adolescents

Assessment Information Brief: REVELIAN EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ASSESSMENT (MSCEIT)

BRIEF REPORT. The effects of optimism and pessimism on updating emotional information in working memory. Sara M. Levens 1 and Ian H.

Management Science Letters

The Study of Relationship between Neuroticism, Stressor and Stress Response

Incremental Validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire Short Form (TEIQue SF)

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Transcription:

Edinburgh Research Explorer A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items Citation for published version: Austin, EJ 2009, 'A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items' Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 46, no. 3, pp. 381-383. DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.025 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.025 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Personality and Individual Differences Publisher Rights Statement: 9 Austin, E. J. (2009). A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items. Personality and Individual Differences, 46(3), 381-383doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.025 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact openaccess@ed.ac.uk providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 15. Nov. 2018

A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items Elizabeth J. Austin* University of Edinburgh Address for correspondence: University of Edinburgh, Psychology, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, UK. 1

A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items Abstract The associations of reaction times (RTs) to the items of two trait emotional intelligence (EI) scales (N = 242, 191) and one ability EI measure (N =331) with EI score were examined. For trait EI an inverted-u relationship between RT and score was found for both scales. This is consistent with the self-schema model of time to respond, with extreme scorers responding more rapidly. For ability EI there was no overall association between RT and EI, but high scorers responded more slowly than low scorers to difficult items. 2

1. Introduction Current research on emotional intelligence (EI) encompasses two distinct measurement approaches and underlying theoretical models. Ability EI is regarded as a subcomponent of intelligence which can be measured by tests similar in design to conventional intelligence tests. Trait EI, assessed by self-report, is regarded as an emotion-related dispositional trait which is a lower-level component of personality (Petrides, Pita, & Kokkinaki, 2007). Whilst there have been numerous studies of the correlates of both trait and ability EI, reaction times (RTs) for responses to EI items have not been studied. For trait EI a similar pattern of associations to those for personality would be expected. The time taken to respond to a personality item can be thought of as depending on two factors: baseline speed, i.e. individual differences in speed of responding which are unrelated to item content, and self-schema effects. Once baseline speed has been accounted for, the self-schema model (Markus, 1977) indicates that extreme scorers should respond more quickly than those with intermediate scores. This is because the process of comparing the item content with an individual s self-knowledge should be faster if the individual has a well-developed self-schema for the relevant trait, which will be the case for extreme scorers. This argument predicts an inverted-u relationship between RT and score, a relationship found in a number of studies (Akrami, Hedlund, & Ekehammar, 2007; Casey & Tryon, 2001; Fekken & Holden, 1992; Kuiper, 1981). The theoretical framework which regards EI as a dispositional trait suggests that the inverted-u relationship should also be found for trait EI. 3

For ability EI item RT measures the time required for an individual to determine the correct response option, which may depend both on EI level and on the respondent s strategy (e.g. guessing). Previous studies of intelligence test item RTs indicate that for some tests RTs for easy items are negatively related to total score, with either a weaker or no association for harder items, where the strategy adopted is more likely to vary with ability level (Lavergne & Vigneau, 1997; Neubauer, 1990; Rafaeli & Tractinsky, 1991; Vigneau, Caissie, & Bors, 2006). The rationale for the research described here was to examine whether trait and ability EI item RTs show the same pattern of associations with test scores as has been found for personality and intelligence tests. From the above discussion the expectation is for an inverted-u relationship between EI score and RT and a negative association between total score and RT for easy but not for hard ability EI items. 2. Method 2.1 Participants The participants were 378 Edinburgh University students (268 female); the mean age of the group was 22.02 years, standard deviation 4.55 years. 2.2 Materials 2.2.1 Situational Test of Emotional Understanding (STEU; MacCann & Roberts, 2008). The items for this ability EI test were derived from Roseman s (2001) 4

appraisal-based emotion model in which the emotion felt by a person is derived from their appraisal of their situation. The test has 42 five-choice items which test respondents knowledge of which emotion is most likely to be felt in situations covering 14 emotions. Because the test items are derived using a theoretical model, the STEU is veridically scored. Details of the development of the STEU and evidence for its reliability and validity are given by MacCann and Roberts (2008). The validity evidence includes correlations of STEU scores with intelligence, other ability EI tests, and emotion-related criteria such as life satisfaction. In the present study the STEU items were presented individually on a computer screen with responses being indicated using a response box and, as recommended by Fazio (1990), participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible without sacrificing accuracy. 2.2.2 Trait EI. The items of two trait EI tests were presented on a computer as described above for the STEU. One test used a subset of 22 items from the modified version of the Schutte et al. (1998) EI scale (SSRI) described by Austin, Saklofske, Huang, and McKenney (2004). These were the items which loaded on this scale s two strongest factors. The second test was the 30-item TEIQue-SF (Petrides & Furnham, 2006). The five-point response scale of the SSRI was retained; for the TEIQue, the usual seven-point scale was replaced by a two-choice (Agree/Disagree) format. A series of non-emotional filler items (10 per test) were placed at intervals amongst the EI items. The RTs for these items were used in the subsequent analyses to control for baseline speed of responding (Fazio, 1990). 1 1 A list of these items is available from the author. 5

2.3 Procedure The data were gathered as part of three studies of associations of EI with other measures not discussed here. The recruitment procedure was identical for each study, via advertising on a website which could be accessed by all students within the university. In each study participants were tested individually in a quiet room. 3. Results Analysis of group differences for the three studies showed no significant differences in age, test scores or proportions of males and females, and the data were merged for subsequent analysis. The internal reliabilities of the EI measures were SSRI.87, TEIQue.81, STEU.78. Since no factor analysis of the STEU has yet been reported, its factor structure was examined; the scree diagram showed a large break between the first and second eigenvalues, suggesting the extraction of a single factor corresponding to the STEU being a unidimensional scale; the majority of the items had large loadings on this factor. Descriptive statistics and correlations amongst the study measures are shown in Table 1. Trait EI scores were uncorrelated with STEU score but significantly correlated with one another. Table 1 near here 6

The RT distributions for each set of items were normalised using a log transform. Mean RTs were then calculated for each trait EI scale and for the two sets of filler items. For the STEU, mean RT was calculated using only correct responses, to control for possible differences in the processes that underlie correct and incorrect responses (Lavergne & Vigneau, 1997). Correlations amongst the RTs were all positive and significant (range.62-.86). STEU score was not significantly correlated with mean STEU item RT, or with the mean RT for easy items (correctly answered by 85% or more of respondents). For hard items (correctly answered by 45% or less of respondents) there was a positive and significant correlation between STEU score and RT (r =.21, N = 316, p <.001). Both trait EI scale scores were significantly negatively correlated with mean item RT (r = -.23, p <.001, N = 242 for the SSRI and r = -.17, p =.02, N = 191 for the TEIQue), but were uncorrelated with filler item RT. The trait EI/RT correlations increased when filler RT was partialled out (r = -.39, -.35 respectively, both p <.001). In order to examine the association between trait EI and item RT in more detail, Z- scores and their squares and cubes were calculated for each EI scale and used as predictors of RT in regression models. This use of centred variables is recommended in order to reduce multicollinearity (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003, p204). To control for baseline speed, filler item RT was also entered as a predictor. The results, presented in Tables 2 and 3, show EI score and squared EI score were significant predictors of RT, with the sign of the quadratic term indicating an inverted U relationship. Adding cubed EI score did not produce a significant increase in R 2 for either model. For the EIS regression analyses were also performed for its two subscales (Optimism/Mood Regulation and Appraisal of Emotion). The results were 7

similar to those for total score, i.e. an inverted-u relationship of subscale score with mean RT for the relevant subset of items, controlling for filler item RT, but for the Appraisal subscale the quadratic term was only marginally significant (p =.055) 2. Tables 2 and 3 near here 4. Discussion These results provide new information about the associations of trait and ability EI item RTs with total score, and allow these associations to be compared with previous findings for personality and intelligence test items. As would be expected, different patterns of associations were found for trait and ability EI. For trait EI an inverted-u relationship between RT and score was found. This is consistent with the self-schema model of speed of responding to personality items (Markus, 1977) and thus supports the placement of trait EI within the personality domain (Petrides et al., 2007). The inverted-u relationship was robust across two different tests and also across two response formats, being found for both a five-point and a two-point response scale. For the ability EI STEU there was no evidence of an overall association of response speed with total score, and the finding for some intelligence tests of a negative association of RT with score for the easier items was not replicated. For harder items there was a positive association, suggesting that high scorers succeed with these items by spending more time considering them. Given the diversity of item types and content of the currently available ability EI tests, this result cannot be 2 Details of these analyses are available on request. 8

regarded as likely to prove general, since response strategies and their outcomes in associations between RT and ability level are likely to vary with the type of item and the response format, but the contrast between the results for the STEU and two trait EI measures provides further support for the distinctness of trait and ability EI. These results also link to the distinction which has been made between assessment of EI by typical (TP) and maximal (MP) performance methods and indicate that as well as differing correlation patterns of TP and MP scores with personality and intelligence (Freudenthaler & Neubauer, 2007), the processes underlying responses to TP and MP items differ. Because the above findings relate to a student sample, it would be desirable to replicate this work using a general population sample, particularly with respect to ability EI, where restrictions of score range are possible amongst students. In the case of ability EI, the study of RTs in a wider range of tests is also desirable since the possibility that associations between RTs and test scores may vary between different tests should be investigated. Acknowledgements The STEU was kindly provided by Dr. Carolyn MacCann. This research was funded by the British Academy and the Leverhulme Trust. References 9

Akrami, N., Hedlund, L-E., & Ekehammar, B. (2007). Personality scale response latencies as self-schema indicators: The inverted-u effect revisited. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 611-618. Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., Huang, S. H. S., & McKenney, D. (2004). Measurement of emotional intelligence: Testing and cross-validating a modified version of Schutte et al s (1998) measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 555-562. Casey, M. M., & Tryon, W. W. (2001). Validating a double-press method for computer administration of personality inventory items. Psychological Assessment, 13, 521-530. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latency in social psychological research. In C. Hendrick & M. S. Clark (Eds.), Research methods in personality and social psychology. (pp.74-97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Fekken, G. C., & Holden, R. R. (1992). Response latency evidence for viewing personality traits as schema indicators. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 103-120. 10

Freudenthaler, H. H., & Neubauer, A. C. (2007). Measuring emotional management abilities: Further evidence of the importance to distinguish between typical and maximal performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1561-1572. Kuiper, N. A. (1981). Convergent evidence for the self as a prototype: The inverted- U RT effect for self and other judgements. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 438-443. Lavergne, C., & Vigneau, F. (1997). Response speed on aptitude tests as an index of intellectual performance: A developmental perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 283-290. MacCann, C., & Roberts, R. D. (2008). New paradigms for assessing emotional intelligence: Theory and data. Emotion, 8, 540-551. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78. Neubauer, A. C. (1990). Speed of information processing in the Hick paradigm and response latencies in a psychometric intelligence test. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 147-152. Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2006). The role of trait emotional intelligence in a 11

gender-specific model of organizational variables. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 552-569. Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 273-289. Rafaeli, S., & Tractinsky, N. (1991). Time in computerized tests: A multitrait, multimethod investigation of general-knowledge and mathematical-reasoning on-line examinations. Computers in Human Behavior, 7, 215-225. Roseman, I. J. (2001). A model of appraisal in the emotion system: Integrating theory, research, and applications. In K. R. Scherer & A. Schorr (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. (pp. 68-91). New York: Oxford University Press. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper J. T., Golden, C. J., & Dornheim, L. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence, Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167-177. Vigneau, F., Caissie, A. F., & Bors, D. A. (2006). Eye-movement analysis demonstrates strategic influences on intelligence. Intelligence, 34, 261-272. 12

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations N Mean Standard Deviation 2 3 1. SSRI 242 85.65 11.24 2. TEIQue 191 52.55 4.74.62*** (191) 3. STEU 331 28.46 3.75.03 (195) -.04 (144) Sample sizes vary between measures due to data from three studies being merged. Scores for the TEIQue cannot be compared to those from other studies using this test due to the use of a 0/1 response format for the computerised version of the test. *** p <.001. 13

Table 2. Regression model for SSRI reaction time B Standard error β t p Constant.90.26-3.51.001 Filler RT.89.031.86 28.80 <.001 EI -.06.009 -.25-7.06 <.001 EI 2 -.013.005 -.10-2.73.007 N = 242, R 2 =.79. 14

Table 3. Regression model for TEIQue reaction time B Standard error β t p Constant 1.92.29 6.75 <.001 Filler RT.77.04.83 21.58 <.001 EI -.07.01 -.28-6.09 <.001 EI 2 -.02.01 -.15-3.23.001 N =191, R 2 =.72. 15