AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

Similar documents
INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

The Development of Preference for or Against Choice-making Opportunities

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

An Evaluation of the Value and Conditioning of Choice as a Reinforcer for Typically. Developing Children. Julie Ann Ackerlund Brandt

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE IPAD WITH THE PROLOQUO2GO APPLICATION FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Presented by, Melissa L. King, M.S.

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

A Comparison of Methods for Marking Correct Responses. in Matching to Sample. A Thesis Presented. Kara Schmidt

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

EFFECTS OF ALTERNATIVE RESPONSES ON BEHAVIOR EXPOSED TO NONCONTINGENT REINFORCEMENT JAVIER VIRUES-ORTEGA BRIAN A. IWATA TARA A. FAHMIE JILL M.

Recent work in applied behavior analysis has begun to examine the variables that influence

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction

USING PERCENTILE SCHEDULES TO INCREASE EYE CONTACT IN CHILDREN WITH FRAGILE X SYNDROME SCOTT S. HALL, NATALEE P. MAYNES, AND ALLAN L.

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida

Comparison of Two Approaches for Identifying Reinforcers in Teaching Figure Coloring to Students with Down Syndrome

A comparison of response-contingent and noncontingent pairing in the conditioning of a reinforcer

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

An Evaluation of Mand and Tact Assessment Procedures

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

BRIEF (TEST-CONTROL) FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT EVALUATION OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR EVOKED BY DIVIDED ATTENTION

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY

OBSERVATIONAL EFFECTS ON PREFERENCE SELECTION FOR FOUR CHILDREN ON THE AUTISM SPECTRUM: A REPLICATION

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

Teaching Language to Young Children. Functional Analysis and Intervention

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Evaluating the Effects of Reinforcer Choice and Reinforcer Variation on the Response Rates of Children with Autism

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND KRIS M. KEENEY GWINNETT COUNTY PUBLIC SCHOOLS, GEORGIA

Identifying & Enhancing the Effectiveness of Positive Reinforcement

topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION JUNE 2012 ISSUE 12

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

---Evidence-base for Antecedent-based Intervention---

TEST-SPECIFIC CONTROL CONDITIONS FOR FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES TARA A. FAHMIE BRIAN A. IWATA ANGIE C. QUERIM JILL M. HARPER

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Evaluation of a presentation and measurement method for assessing activity preference

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

ANALYSIS OF THE EFFECTS OF CHOICE MAKING ON TOY PLAY. Thesis. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Arts in the

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF EXCESSIVE STRAIGHTENING AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR IN AN ADOLESCENT DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM DAVID E. KUHN

6/11/2017. Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism. stereotypy. Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D.

Running Head: AUDITORY PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT THE EFFECTS OF HIGHLY PREFERRED AUDITORY STIMULI IDENTIFIED

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AS TREATMENT FOR PROBLEM BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT STEVEN W. PAYNE AND CLAUDIA L.

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

EVALUATIONS OF DELAYED REINFORCEMENT IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF PICA AND DESTRUCTION OF HOLIDAY DECORATIONS DANIEL R. MITTEER, PATRICK W. ROMANI, BRIAN D. GREER, AND WAYNE W.

FA and RX Workbook (2017) by Gregory P. Hanley, Ph.D., BCBA-D

Effects of delays to response blocking when used as treatment for problem behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement

A TRANSLATIONAL EVALUATION OF THE INTERPOLATED REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURE WITH YOUNG CHILDREN

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

Reinforcing Effects of Social Stimuli on Responding After. Implementation of A Pairing Procedure. A Thesis Presented. Amanda J.

That Define the Mand and the Tact

ON THE APPLIED USE OF PROGRESSIVE-RATIO SCHEDULES OF REINFORCEMENT HENRY S. ROANE

Comparing the Teaching Interaction Procedure to Social Stories: A Replication Study

FA and RX Workbook (2017) by Gregory P. Hanley, Ph.D., BCBA-D

Derived Textual Control in Recreational Activity Schedules with Children with Autism

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn

A SORTING-TO-MATCHING METHOD TO TEACH COMPOUND MATCHING TO SAMPLE RACHEL S. FARBER

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism

A Comparison of In-Vivo and Video Modeling Procedures for Teaching. Functional Response Chains to Individuals with Developmental Disabilities

Teaching Staff Who Work With Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders to Evaluate the Treatment Integrity of Discrete-Trials Teaching Sessions

TEACHING SIMPLE AUDITORY DISCRIMINATIONS TO STUDENTS WITH AUTISM. Kristine L. Marino, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

AN EVALUATION OF THE EFFECTS OF REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ON PREFERENCE AND ON-TASK BEHAVIOR. Brooke A. Jones

An Evaluation of the Utility of Two Demand Assessments in Identifying Negative. Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Casey E. Bethay

TRANSFER OF MAND TOPOGRAPHIES TO TACT RELATIONS AND VICE VERSA IN TWO VOCAL-VERBAL CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Jill E. Castellani, B.A.

The Persistence-Strengthening Effects of DRA: An Illustration of Bidirectional Translational Research

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

AN EVALUATION OF THE EFFECTS OF SOCIAL INTERACTION ON PREFERENCE AND RESPONSE ALLOCATION. Amber N. Lessor

Matrix of FCT by Outcome and Age (years) Functional Communication Training (FCT)

Noncontingent Reinforcement and the Acquisition of Appropriate Behavior

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

WESTERN MICHIGAN UNIVERSITY

Treatment of Pica Using a Pica Exchange Procedure with Increasing Response Effort

e-newsletter ABA Literature Summary topic : MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND COM- MUNICATION ...

Amanda M. Karsten (née Firth), Ph.D., BCBA-D 1215 Wilbraham Road, Mailbox S-5026 Springfield, MA (413)

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Effects of Differential Reinforcement of Short Latencies on Response Latency, Task Completion, and Accuracy of an Adolescent with Autism

STRATEGIES FOR SUCCESSFUL INSTRUCTION

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC TASKS: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF IDIOSYNCRATIC ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS JENNIFER MCCOMAS

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2016, 49, 1 6 170 175 NUMBER 1(SPRING) AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS TIFFANY KODAK UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN MILWAUKEE AND JASON C. VLADESCU CALDWELL UNIVERSITY The current study compared the differential effects of choice and no-choice reinforcement conditions on skill acquisition. In addition, we assessed preference for choice-making opportunities with 3 children with autism, using a modified concurrent-chains procedure. We replicated the experiment with 2 participants. The results indicated that choice-making opportunities increased treatment efficacy for 2 of the 3 participants, and all 3 participants demonstrated a preference for choice-making opportunities. Key words: autism, choice, concurrent-chains arrangement, instructional efficacy, skill acquisition Providing students with choice-making opportunities appears to be beneficial during instructional programs, because it may result in decreases in problem behavior and increases in academic engagement (Dunlap et al., 1994). In addition, research suggests that the provision of choice increases the frequency of academic responding (Tiger, Toussaint, & Roath, 2010). Although the provision of choice has been shown to increase academic engagement, it has not been demonstrated to improve skill acquisition. In an attempt to address this question, Newman, Needelman, Reinecke, and Robek (2002) evaluated whether providing choice-making opportunities influenced the rate of skill acquisition and The design and data-collection portions of this research were conducted while the investigators were at the University of Nebraska Medical Center s Munroe-Meyer Institute. Address correspondence to Karen Toussaint, Department of Behavior Analysis, University of North Texas, Denton, Texas 76203 (e-mail: karen.toussaint@unt.edu). doi: 10.1002/jaba.263 disruptive behavior during discrete-trial instruction for children with autism. Newman et al. either allowed participants to select the task order and reinforcer at the beginning of an instructional session (i.e., choice) or the experimenter selected both the task and reinforcer (i.e., no choice). Results demonstrated no difference in instructional efficacy. However, the effects of choice on acquisition may be more pronounced by providing opportunities to choose during the consequence portion of instruction rather than as an antecedent manipulation (Fenerty & Tiger, 2010). Thus, one purpose of the current study was to evaluate the effects of choice-making opportunities when they are provided as a consequence. The current analysis also was designed to replicate and extend previous findings that demonstrate preschool children with and without developmental disabilities prefer choice-making opportunities (Brigham & Sherman, 1973; Fisher, Thompson, Piazza, Crosland, & Gotjen, 1997; Thompson, Fisher, & Contrucci, 1998; Tiger, Hanley, & Hernandez, 2006). 170 1

2 EFFECTS OF CHOICE KAREN A. ON TOUSSAINT EFFICACY AND et al. PREFERENCE 171 METHOD Participants Three preschool-aged children with autism, who attended a university-based early intervention program, participated in the evaluation. Samuel was a 5-year-old boy who used simple sentences to mand and frequently engaged in simple social exchanges. Ethan was a 4-year-old boy who used short phrases to label and request items; he engaged in limited social conversation. Patrick was a 3-year-old boy who used a combination of short phrases and simple sentences to engage in tacting, manding, and social exchanges. Settings and Materials We conducted all sessions in the participants typical learning environment, a room (5 m by 5 m) that contained a child-sized table, chairs, and relevant session materials. We conducted three sessions daily for 3 to 4 days per week. The child sat at the table, and the experimenter sat next to the participant. A secondary observer sat behind and to one side of the participant during some sessions. Measurement, Interobserver Agreement, and Procedural Integrity Observers recorded session data on a trialby-trial basis. We collected data on unprompted correct responses, defined as vocalization of the targeted response before the delivery of the model prompt. Observers also collected data on initiallink selections, defined as participants touching a card, during the concurrent-chains arrangement. A second observer independently recorded data for a minimum of 32% of all sessions. Agreement for unprompted correct responses was scored if both observers independently recorded the same target responses in a trial. Agreement for initial-link selections was scored if both the experimenter and the second observer independently recorded the same initial-link selection. We calculated interobserver agreement by dividing the number of agreements by the total number of trials and converting the result to a percentage. Mean agreement for unprompted correct responses was 99% (range, 83% to 100%) for Samuel and 100% for both Ethan and Patrick during the first evaluation. Initial-link selection agreement was 100% across all participants. A second observer also collected procedural integrity data for a minimum of 22% of sessions. We calculated procedural integrity for each session by dividing the number of correctly implemented trials by the total number of trials and converting this ratio to a percentage. Mean procedural integrity score was 99.4% (range, 90% to 100%) for Samuel, 100% for Ethan, and 99.8% (range, 95% to 100%) for Patrick. Preference Assessments We conducted a paired-item preference assessment (Fisher et al., 1992) with small edible items (e.g., M&Ms) and larger items (e.g., Cheetos) broken into small pieces, approximately 1 cm in diameter. We used the top three items associated with the highest selection percentage. We also conducted a preference assessment for colors and used the three colors associated with the lowest selection percentage to serve as initial-link stimuli in the concurrent-chains arrangement. Procedure and Design We evaluated the effect of contingent choice on correct responses using a nonconcurrent multiple baseline design embedded within a multiple schedule design. Instructional stimuli previously assigned to the control condition served as acquisition targets during the preference phase of the evaluation. We conducted the experiment twice with Samuel and Patrick for replication purposes. Ethan was no longer enrolled in the program at the time of replication. There were six targets in each condition in the initial evaluation, and each target was presented twice. For Patrick and Samuel, targets consisted of intraverbal responses to common wh? questions and questions about functions of objects. Ethan s targets were tacting pictures of

172 EFFECTS OF CHOICE KAREN A. ONTOUSSAINT EFFICACY AND et al. PREFERENCE 3 common objects. During replication of the evaluation, we included 20 targets that were presented once in each condition; these targets consisted of tacting pictures of popular people or characters (e.g., George Washington, Scooby Doo). We attempted to equate targets across conditions by creating sets that contained targets with a similar number of syllables. During the treatment efficacy portion of the evaluation, we conducted a series of three sessions per day. We set the mastery criterion at two consecutive sessions with correct, unprompted responses at or above 90%. We presented conditions in a pseudorandom and counterbalanced order with one exception; all participants experienced a choice session before a no-choice session during the first series of the evaluation. We yoked the reinforcers selected by the participant in the choice condition to the subsequent nochoice condition. As a result, it is possible that the selections made in a choice session were yoked to two subsequent no-choice sessions if a no-choice session randomly preceded a choice session in a given series. We arranged for at least 10 min between each condition to account for satiation associated with food items. We paired each instructional condition with a colored index card, which served as the discriminative stimulus during the instructional efficacy evaluation. Colored cards later served as initial links of a concurrent-chains arrangement in the instructional preference evaluation. Previous research suggests that an effective method for identifying consumer preference for instructional conditions is to measure selection among concurrently available responses (selection of initial links) that determine thecontingenciesinplaceintheterminallink (choice, no choice, or no reinforcement; Hanley, Piazza,Fisher,Contrucci,&Maglieri,1997). Efficacy Evaluation Baseline. The experimenter presented the antecedent stimulus and allowed the participant 5 s to respond. No consequences were provided for correct or incorrect responses. Choice. The experimenter physically guided the participant to touch the relevant initial-link stimulus and then presented the antecedent stimulus. After a correct response, the experimenter presented an array of three edible items and prompted the participant to select one (the experimenter blocked attempts to select multiple items if this occurred). The experimenter provided instructions using a progressive prompt delay to a vocal model prompt (e.g., the experimenter said, spoon ), and we set prompt delays at 0 s, 2 s, 5 s, 7 s, and 10 s. All participants first experienced one session with trials at a 0-s prompt delay, and the second session included trials at a 2-s delay. We increased the prompt delay (5 s, 7 s, 10 s) during the subsequent session if at least 50% of unprompted incorrect responses were errors of omission in an instructional session. The experimenter delivered reinforcement after both unprompted and prompted correct responses until the participant demonstrated two consecutive sessions with at least 50% unprompted correct responses. Thereafter, reinforcement was provided only for unprompted correct responses. Instruction continued until performance reached mastery criterion. No choice. This procedure was identical to the choice condition except that after a correct response, the experimenter delivered an edible item that was yoked to selections in the previous choice condition. In the event that there were more reinforced trials in a no-choice session than in the previous choice session (due to discontinuation of reinforcement for prompted responses after the preset criterion), the experimenter repeated the delivery of items based on the sequence of item selections. Control. The control condition was identical to baseline except that the therapist presented the relevant initial-link stimulus. Preference Evaluation During the subsequent instructional preference evaluation, the experimenter placed the three initial-link stimuli in front of the participant and

4 EFFECTS OF CHOICE KAREN A. ON TOUSSAINT EFFICACY AND et al. PREFERENCE 173 provided the instruction, pick one. After an initial-link selection, the experimenter presented the antecedent stimulus, and responding resulted in access to the respective terminal-link contingencies for that session. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The results of the differential efficacy portion of the evaluation are presented in the first two panels of Figure 1. All participants demonstrated low levels of correct responding during baseline sessions, and correct responding increased for all participants after implementation of the instructional procedures. Samuel demonstrated mastery-level responding in the choice condition in five training sessions compared to 14 training sessions in the no-choice condition. Differential efficacy was replicated, in that Samuel met mastery criterion in one fewer session in the choice condition than in the no-choice condition in his second evaluation. Ethan s performances in Figure 1. The percentage of correct responses during baseline (first phase) and instructional conditions (second phase) for Samuel (first and second panels), Ethan (middle panel), and Patrick (fourth and bottom panels) during the instructional efficacy evaluation. The dashed line represents the mastery criterion. The percentage of unprompted correct responses and condition selection during the instructional preference evaluation are shown in the third phase. The number in parentheses denotes the first or second evaluation for Samuel and Patrick.

174 EFFECTS OF CHOICE KAREN A. ONTOUSSAINT EFFICACY AND et al. PREFERENCE 5 the choice and no-choice conditions were equal; he met mastery criterion within an equal number of choice and no-choice sessions. Patrick s correct responding reached the mastery criterion in three fewer training sessions in the choice condition than in the no-choice condition in the first evaluation and in four fewer training sessions in his second evaluation. We saw a distinct advantage, in that two of three participants (Samuel and Patrick) required fewer instructional sessions to reach the mastery criterion under choice conditions. Efficiency ranged from a savings of one to nine instructional sessions per instructional comparison, which may be viewed as relatively minor. However, this practice may yield a substantial time savings if implemented across several instructional programs over longer periods of time. All participants demonstrated a preference for choice-making conditions (Figure 1, third panel). Samuel showed an exclusive preference for the choice condition. Ethan demonstrated preference for the choice condition by selecting the choice condition for seven of eight sessions. Patrick exclusively selected the choice condition during both evaluations. His correct responding did not reach mastery criterion in the preference evaluation phase of his replication evaluation because his family relocated and he left the program midevaluation. A yoking procedure was used in an attempt to isolate choice as a variable and control for item preference. However, it is possible that our current results may still be attributed to the differential consequences associated with choosing. That is, a selected item in the choice condition may have served as the most preferred item at the moment it was selected but not other moments, such as when it was provided in the nochoice condition (Fisher et al., 1997). One solution has been to use identical reinforcers in both conditions (Thompson et al., 1998). However, we elected to provide a choice among different items to mimic an arrangement that is more likely to occur during clinical practice. Previous research has demonstrated that individuals with typical and atypical development prefer choice-making opportunities (Tiger et al., 2006). Providing choice of reinforcement is one of many ways in which therapists may provide choice-making opportunities and promote personal liberties for individuals with autism and related developmental disabilities (Bannerman, Sheldon, Sherman, & Harchik, 1990). REFERENCES Bannerman, D. J., Sheldon, J. B., Sherman, J. A., & Harchik, A. E. (1990). Balancing the right to habilitation with the right to personal liberties: The rights of people with developmental disabilities to eat too many doughnuts and take a nap. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 79 89. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1990. 23-79 Brigham, T. A., & Sherman, J. A. (1973). Effects of choice and immediacy of reinforcement on single response and switching behavior of children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 425 435. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1973.19-425 Dunlap, G., deperczel, M., Clarke, S., Wilson, D., Wright, S., White, R., & Gomez, A. (1994). Choice making to promote adaptive behavior for students with emotional and behavioral challenges. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 505 518. doi: 10.1901/jaba. 1994.27-505 Fenerty, K. A., & Tiger, J. H. (2010). Determining preschoolers preferences for choice-making opportunities: Choice of task versus choice of consequence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 503 507. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2010.43-503 Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491 498. doi: 10.1901/jaba. 1992.25-491 Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Piazza, C. C., Crosland, K., & Gotjen, D. (1997). On the relative reinforcing effects of choice and differential consequences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 423 438. doi: 10.19 01/jaba.1997.30-423 Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Contrucci, S. A., & Maglieri, K. A. (1997). Evaluation of client preference for function-based treatment packages. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 459 473. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1997.30-459 Newman, B., Needelman, M., Reinecke, D. R., & Robek, A. (2002). The effect of providing choices on skill acquisition and competing behavior of children with

6 EFFECTS OF CHOICE KAREN A. ON TOUSSAINT EFFICACY AND et al. PREFERENCE 175 autism during discrete trial instruction. Behavioral Interventions, 17, 31 41. doi: 10.1002/bin.99 Thompson, R. H., Fisher, W. W., & Contrucci, S. A. (1998). Evaluating the reinforcing effects of choice in comparison to reinforcement rate. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 19, 181 187. doi: 10.1016/ S0891-4222(97)00050-4 Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Hernandez, E. (2006). An evaluation of the value of choice with preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39,1 16. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2006.158-04 Tiger, J. H., Toussaint, K. A., & Roath, C. T. (2010). An evaluation of the value of choice-making opportunities in single-operant arrangements: Simple fixed- and progressive-ratio schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 519 524. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2010.43-519 Received November 13, 2013 Final acceptance July 9, 2015 Action Editor, Michael Kelley