Effects of Combined Frontal and Temporal Lesions

Similar documents
A COMPARISON OF THE EFFECTS OF PARTIAL AND TOTAL LATERAL FRONTAL LESIONS ON TEST PERFORMANCE BY MONKEYS 1

LOCOMOTOR ACTIVITY FOLLOWING LATERAL FRONTAL LESIONS IN RHESUS MONKEYS 1

EFFECT OF AMYGDALECTOMY ON TRANSFER OF TRAINING IN MONKEYS

Dissociable Roles of Mid-Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex in Visual Working Memory

FUNCTION OF CAT'S CAUDATE NUCLEUS IN TASKS INVOLVING SPATIAL DISCONTIGUITY BETWEEN LOCATION OF CUE AND RESPONSE

A SEARCH FOR THE SPATIAL ENGRAM WITHI'N THE FRONTAL LOBES OF MONKEYS

Brain anatomy tutorial. Dr. Michal Ben-Shachar 459 Neurolinguistics

Montreal Neurological Institute and Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec H3A 2B4, Canada

IMPAIRMENTS ON LOCOMOTOR TASK INVOLVING SPATIAL OPPOSITION BETWEEN CUE AND REWARD IN FRONTALLY ABLATED MONKEYS

Posterior association cortex lesions in rats: Mazes, pattern discrimination, and reversal learning

XIXth Century: Localization of Functions to Different Parts of the Brain

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

IMPAIRMENT OF UTILIZATION OF RESPONSE-PRODUCED CUES AFTER FROlNTlOPOLAR LESIONS IN RATS

XIXth Century: Localization of Functions to Different Parts of the Brain

Contrasting Effects on Discrimination Learning after Hippocampal Lesions and Conjoint Hippocampal Caudate Lesions in Monkeys

Structure and Function of the Auditory and Vestibular Systems (Fall 2014) Auditory Cortex (1) Prof. Xiaoqin Wang

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

Visual Learning and Retention after Ablation of Inferotemporal Cortex in Dogs. From the Physiological Laboratory of Prof. K.

Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus

The animal model of human amnesia: Long-term memory impaired and short-term memory intact

Cortical Control of Movement

PHY3111 Mid-Semester Test Study. Lecture 2: The hierarchical organisation of vision

A bilateral cortico-striate projection

Perirhinal Cortex Ablation Impairs Visual Object Identification

9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems

CEREBRUM. Dr. Jamila EL Medany

CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila Elmedany Dr. Essam Eldin Salama

Reversible Deficit in Haptic Delay Tasks from Cooling Prefrontal Cortex

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

Behavioral consequences of selective damage to frontal pole and posterior cingulate cortices

NIH/NICHD History: A Graphic Documentation

Contextual Interference Effects on the Acquisition, Retention, and Transfer of a Motor Skill

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

BLACK-WHITE DISCRIMINATION PERFORMANCE IN FRONTAL RATS

Memory Impairment in Monkeys Following Lesions Limited to the Hippocampus

MINI-REVIEW The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging

BRIEF REPORT. Impairments in Sodium Appetite after Lesions of Gustatory Thalamus: Replication and Extension I

i n < UMEÅ PSYCHOLOGICAL REPORTS Department of Psychology University of Umeó No. 12U 1977 (J A

INFORMATION TO USERS. The following explanation of techniques is provided to help clarify markings or notations which may appear on this reproduction.

Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization

AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN SHUTTLING AND NONSHUTTLING SITUATIONS, WITH AND WITHOUT A BARRIER. Kobe Juvenile Detention and Classification Home

Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Pathologies of postchiasmatic visual pathways and visual cortex

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

The differential effects of additional cortical lesions in rats with single- or. multiple-stage lesions of the visual cortex

E. Irle 1, B. Wowra 2, J. Kunert 1, M. Peper 2, J. Hampl 2, and S. Kunze 2. Introduction

Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas. Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template

THE EFFECTS OF LESIONS OF AUDITORY CORTEX ON DISCRIMINATION OF SOUND LOCALIZATION IN DOG

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

Ch. 18. Brain Mechanisms of Emotion. Bear et al., Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3 rd ed., Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2006, pp

The concept of neural network in neuropsychology

Neural activity to positive expressions predicts daily experience of schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms in adults with high social anhedonia

Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE

-Zeina Assaf. -Omar Odeh. - Maha Beltagy

C:\Documents and Settings\sstensaas\Desktop\dental visual 2010\VisualPath dental 2010.docVisualPath dental 2010.doc

LISC-322 Neuroscience. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation. Visual Field Representation

Ventrolateral Prefrontal Neuronal Activity Related to Active Controlled Memory Retrieval in Nonhuman Primates

Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex Lesions Differentially Influence Choices during Object Reversal Learning

CEREBRUM & CEREBRAL CORTEX

RIGHT HEMISPHERE LANGUAGE MAPPING USING ELECTROCORTICAL STIMULATION IN PATIENTS WITH BILATERAL LANGUAGE

Excellent Network Courses. Department of Neurology Affiliated hospital of Jiangsu University

Hierarchically Organized Mirroring Processes in Social Cognition: The Functional Neuroanatomy of Empathy

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Radial-maze performance in the rat following lesions of posterior neocortex

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

Recovery of function after brain damage: Different processes and the facilitation of one

Middle Temporal Cortical Visual Area Visuospatial Function in Galago senegalensis

Layered organization of cortex: Paleocortex 3 layers hippocampal formation / ventral & medial cortex closest to brainstem

ANALYSIS OF RESIDUAL WEIGHT DISCRIMINATORY ABILITY AND EVOKED CORTICAL POTENTIALS FOLLOWING SECTION OF DORSAL COLUMNS IN MONKEYSI,2

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Visual Recognition in Monkeys Following Rhinal Cortical Ablations Combined with Either Amygdalectomy or Hippocampectomy

The significance of sensory motor functions as indicators of brain dysfunction in children

Amherst. University of Massachusetts Amherst. John Eugene Mauldin University of Massachusetts Amherst

Why do we have a hippocampus? Short-term memory and consolidation

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

The American Approach to Depth Electrode Insertion December 4, 2012

Auditory and Vestibular Systems

Orbitofrontal cortex. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. Approximate location of the OFC shown on a sagittal MRI

brain valuation & behavior

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Announcement. Danny to schedule a time if you are interested.

Chapter 2 Test. 1. Evolutionary structures within the are the most primitive. *a. hindbrain b. thalamus c. forebrain d. midbrain e.

The Effectiveness of Pattern Homogeneity as a Cue in Subcortical Pattern Vision

The human brain. of cognition need to make sense gives the structure of the brain (duh). ! What is the basic physiology of this organ?

Monkeys: A Selective Mnemonic Role for the Hippocampus Memory for the Location of Objects

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

OBJECTIVES. At the end of the lecture, students should be able to: List the cerebral arteries.

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

HOMONYMOUS VISUAL FIELD DEFECTS Perimetric findings and corresponding neuro-imaging results

Homework Week 2. PreLab 2 HW #2 Synapses (Page 1 in the HW Section)

Cerebral Cortex Structure, Function, Dysfunction Reading Ch 10 Waxman Dental Neuroanatomy Lecture. Suzanne Stensaas, Ph.D.

THE ORGANIZATION OF AMYGDALOPETAL PROJECTIONS FROM THE LATERAL HYPOTHALAMUS AND PREOPTIC AREA IN THE RAT

Cortex and Mind Chapter 6

Transcription:

Proceeding8 of the National Academy of Sciences Vol. 66, No. 2, pp. 577-582, June 1970 Effects of Combined Frontal and Temporal Lesions on Learned Behaviors in Rhesus Monkeys* H. F. Harlow,t K. A. Schiltz, A. J. Blomquist, and C. I. Thompsont REGIONAL PRIMATE RESEARCH CENTER, UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN, MADISON Communicated February 16, 1970 Abstract. Delayed response ability, and to a lesser extent visual discrimination performance, is seriously impaired by extensive bilateral damage to the frontal lobes. Reciprocal anatomical connections between the frontal and temporal lobes suggested that massive lesions in both lobes might produce an impairment more complete than that resulting from frontal lobectomy alone. Five monkeys were given combined bilateral frontal and anterior-temporal lesions, and were found to be inferior to both frontal lobectomized monkeys and to unoperated controls on the object discrimination task. The combined lesion did not increase the deficit on delayed response over that obtained after only bilateral frontal lobectomy. Results indicate that the anterior-temporal neocortex is involved in the mediation of visual discrimination ability. Bilateral destruction of the frontal lobes produces a picture of behavioral deficit which is drastic, but the literature is not entirely precise. Delayed response and single alternation performance are severely impaired,1 although some sparing of delayed response has been reported when the lesions have been produced at or before 5 months of age,2 and when older animals have been subjected to intensive training procedures.3 On the other hand, learning tests of equal or greater difficulty involving visual discriminations show considerable sparing. Large bilateral frontal lesions do produce a performance decrement on object discrimination and object discrimination learning-set tasks, but the loss is not complete.' 4 In the hope of achieving more complete and precise behavioral deficits on tasks involving both visual discrimination and delayed response, we examined the combination of bilateral frontal lobectomy with the destruction of other cerebral areas. Kling and Tucker5 found little or no sparing on the delayed response task after surgery in infant monkeys when they combined frontal lesions with damage to the head of the caudate nucleus. However, this combination did not produce an increased deficit on a red-green discrimination. Myers6 pointed out that there is an anatomical rationale for combining destruction of the frontal lobe with bilateral destruction of the anterior portion of the temporal lobes. In general it is known that frontal lobe areas give rise to projections to the temporal lobe and, conversely, the rostral pole of the temporal lobe projects to the frontal lobe. Anatomical studies7'8 have shown that lesions of frontal cortex superior to the sulcus principalis lead to degeneration of fibers 577

no O 578 PSYCHOLOGY: HARLOW ET AL. PROC. N. A. S. coursing to the superior temporal lobe, and lesions of frontal cortex inferior to the sulcus principalis produce degenerating fibers in the cortex of the superior temporal sulcus. Lesions of the orbital region of the frontal lobe lead to degeneration of fibers in the temporal pole. In addition, a lesion in the anterior pole of the temporal lobe leads to degeneration of fibers terminating in the orbital and lateral cortex of the frontal lobes. As further emphasis on the relationship between the frontal lobe and anterior temporal lobe, these two cortical regions are considered by some investigators to be parts of a single functional unit. In the present study no attempt was made to exploit all of these anatomical relationships, but massive bilateral lesions in the frontal lobe were combined with bilateral neocortical lesions in the anterior temporal pole. The effects of this combined lesion upon visual discrimination and delayed response ability were then assessed in comparison with the performance of both normal control subjects and monkeys sustaining damage limited to the frontal lobes. Method. Subjects: Nineteen rhesus monkeys served as subjects. Four monkeys underwent a bilateral frontal lobectomy at 24 months of age, and hereafter are referred to as Group F. Group C-F (control-frontal) was a like-aged group of four monkeys that served as unoperated controls for Group F. Of the remaining 11 monkeys, five sustained a bilateral frontal lobectomy at 20 months, followed by bilateral removal of the anterior tips of the temporal lobes at 34 months of age. These monkeys are hereafter referred to as the frontal-temporal group (Group FT). Group C-FT consisted of six monkeys that were unoperated, but who otherwise were treated in a manner identical to that of Group FT. Group F and C-F animals had limited social experience, and had received no learning tests prior to those reported in the present experiment. However, the results of the learning tests described in this paper were included in a report that is now in press.' Groups FT and C-FT had received intensive social experience, and also had been introduced to an abbreviated set of learning tests shortly after the frontal lesions were made in Group FT. None of the learning data from Groups FT and C-FT have been published previously. Results of the abbreviated learning tests administered prior to the time the temporal lobes were removed are reported in a paper now in preparation. Surgery and histology: Frontal operations from Groups F and FT were performed by aspiration in a single stage by means FRONTAL of standard operative techniques that have been de- LESION scribed in detail elsewhere.9.2 Ablations removed the cortex and all underlying fiber systems in Brodmann's areas 9, 10, 11, and 12, but spared the caudate nucleus. A diagram of the lesion is illustrated in Figure 1. Representative histological sections have been published elsewhere. They indicate that all of the lesions Dff.. were in the planned areas.2 00 0 0 TEMPORAL Temporal lesions were made by aspiration in two LESION operations, spaced about 2 weeks apart. All of the temporal neocortex anterior to the inferior aspect of FIG. 1.-Lateral view of rhesus the central sulcus was removed. The area of the lesion brain. The hatched areas illus- is illustrated in Figure 1. trate the location and extent of Apparatus. All testing was done in the Wisconsin the frontal lobe and temporal lobe lesion. The numbered lines refer general. test apparatus, which has a superstructure to stained frontal sections from containing a 9 X 24 in. tray with either two or three a normal brain used for recon- foodwells, which the observer could cover with stimulus struction of the lesion in operated objects and advance within reach of the monkey. animals. Appropriate responses were rewarded by grapes, rai-

VOL. 66, 1970 PSYCHOLOGY: HARLOW ET AL. 579 sins, small pieces of apple, or shelled corn, depending upon the preferences of individual subjects. Testing procedure: The adaptation procedure used to accustom monkeys to the test situation has been described elsewhere in detail.10 The goal of this procedure is to train the monkey to displace stimulus objects of various dimensions in order to uncover a baited foodwell. This goal was accomplished over a period of 7 to 10 weeks for the monkeys in the present study. After adaptation the monkeys were introduced to a two-object discrimination problem, in which one object ("correct") covered a baited foodwell, and one object ("incorrect") covered an empty foodwell. Each object disctitnination problem consisted of presentation of a pair of objects for 25 trials, and a new problem involving a new pair of stimuli was presented each day for 20 days. Each problem was theoretically solvable after a single presentation of the objects, and the dependent measure was the number of correct choices on trials 2-25 for each problem. Upon completion of object discrimination testing, all subjects were introduced to the delayed-response problem. Stimulus objects consisted of two identical red wooden equilateral triangles 2 in. in depth, which were placed directly over the foodwells. Subjects were allowed to observe a randomly chosen foodwell being baited and, after both foodwells were covered, they were then required to wait either 0 or 5 sec before they were allowed to remove one of the triangles. Ten 0-sec and ten 5-sec trials were given each day for 90 days. Analyses: Because there were unequal numbers of subjects in the four groups, the data were analyzed by an unweighted means analysis of variance,11 using a 4 X 2 (groups x Problem Blocks) design. Individual group comparisons on object discrimination and delayed response performance were subsequently made, using the Newman-Keuls procedure.1 The total number of object discrimination problems and delayed response trials was divided into two blocks, and increases in percentage of correct responses with practice were analyzed by the Problem Blocks factor in the analysis of variance. Results. Object discrimination: As is shown in Figure 2, monkeys subjected to the dual frontal-anterior temporal lesion were inferior to a control group with identical antecedent experience. They also were inferior to the group of monkeys with bilateral frontal lobectomy, and to their control group. The mean number of correct responses out of 480 for the four groups was 402.8 (C-FT), 401.0 (C-F), 388.0 (F), and 345.4 (FT). The standard error of a single mean, used in the Newman-Keuls procedure, is 11.52. Statistically, the two control groups and Group F did not differ, although monkeys with frontal lobectomy showed a tendency to perform at a level somewhat below that of the controls. The performance of Group FT, however, was significantly below that of each of the other three groups (all ps < 0.05). Thus it is clear that bilateral removal of the tips of the temporal lobes in monkeys already sustaining frontal lobectomy produces a deficit on object discrimination performance which is greater than that produced by frontal lobectomy alone. The "Problem Block" factor in the analysis of variance was significant (p <0.05), which indicates that subjects made more correct responses on the second 10 problems than they did on the first 10. Inspection of the data indicated that only two of the 19 subjects, one in Group C-FT and one in Group F, failed to show an improvement on the second block of 10 problems. The Groups X Problem Blocks interaction was not significant, so there was no evidence that amount of improvement differed among the four groups.

580 PSYCHOLOGY: HARLOW ET AL. PROC. N. A. S. wn Cl (oo 0-5 SECOND DELAYED RESPONSE o- OBJECT DISCRIMINATION U 90 CONTROL (C-F) a) 0 CONTROL (C-FT) Z 90 - FN 0 20 40 _ 80 U) CODT1OKE PROBLEM o FRONTAL-TEMPORAL o ~~~~~~~~~~r(ft) - 0c -- AFRONTAL (F) 0 70 60-2 z CONDITION KEY C.) W 60 CONTROL (C-FTi prbls 50. CE 0 Li CONTROL (C-F) - FRONTAL-TEMPORAL (FT(O- ---0 50 FRONTAL (F) --- 40 (0 20 250 500 PROBLEMS TRIALS FIG. 2.-Performance of operated FIG. 3.-Performance of operated and groups and normal groups of monkeys normal monkeys on 500, combined 0-sec and on 20 object discrimination problems. 5-sec, delayed response trials. 0- and 5-second delayed response: As can be seen in Figure 3, the performance on combined 0- and 5-sec delayed response problems of monkeys subjected to bilateral removal of both the anterior temporal and the frontal lobes was drastically impaired, but no more so than was the performance of monkeys subjected to bilateral removal of the frontal lobes alone. The mean number of correct responses on 1000 trials (combining 500 0-sec and 500 5-sec delay trials) for each of the four groups was 854.0 (C-F), 799.2 (C-FT), 641.2 (FT), and 611.2 (F). The standard error of a single mean is 23.35. Statistically, the performance of the frontal-temporal group did not differ from that of the group sustaining only frontal damage. Similarly, the performance of the two control groups did not differ. However, each of the two control groups were superior in performance to Groups F and FT (all four ps <0.01). The Problem Blocks factor was significant (p <0.01), indicating that subjects made more correct responses on the second 250 trials than they did on the first 250 trials. Inspection of the data revealed that only one of the 19 subjects, in Group C-FT, failed to demonstrate this improvement with practice. The Groups X Problem Blocks interaction was not significant, so there was no evidence that the four groups differed in their rate of improvement. Discussion. A wealth of studies has correlated bilateral destruction of functional areas with learned performances. The classical researches by Jacobsen'2 indicated vast delayed response decrement following bilateral destruction of the frontal granular cortex and apparently no loss of other learning capabilities. Since such data were in contradiction to Lashley's"3 theory of equipotentiality of intellectual function, the researches attracted wide attention, apart from their own importance. Subsequently, it was shown by Pribram" and by Harlow and associates'5 zu

VOL. 66, 1970 PSYCHOLOGY: HARLOW ET AL. 581 that bilateral frontal lesions and bilateral posterior lesions produced complementary learning syndromes, with discrimination learning and learning-set adversely affected and delayed response unaffected by posterior lesions, and discrimination learning and learning-set almost unimpaired by frontal granular lesions which destroyed or drastically impaired delayed response capabilities. Teuber'6 used the phrase "double dissociation of symptoms" to describe the two differential syndromes. The double bilateral frontal and anterior-temporal lesion which we described in this paper produced a significant performance decrement on a series of object discrimination problems, when the double-lesioned animals' performance was compared with that of monkeys with bilateral frontal lesions only, or with that of normal controls. Previously, Meyer,17 Riopelle et al.,18 and Chow'9 had found that bilateral temporal lobectomy or bilateral temporal decortication impaired discrimination and learning-set performance, but they did not describe the effect of smaller temporal lesions. Pribram20 has long emphasized that neocortical bilateral temporal lesions adversely affected "visually guided behavior," but he emphasized and reemphasized that the critical temporal-lobe area was the inferolateral portion of the posterior temporal lobes. The position of the posterior temporal lobes is distinct from the anterior poles, and our data give strong presumptive evidence that the discrimination loss is not limited to lesions in the infero-lateral part of the posterior temporal lobe. Myers2' placed a number of monkeys suffering from bilateral destruction of the anterior portions of the temporal lobes in the free environment of an island near Puerto Rico. None of these monkeys rejoined their social groups, and all of them shortly afterwards perished or disappeared. Although we have no measures of the social capabilities of our frontal-temporal monkeys, their extreme intellectual deficits should impose serious hazards to socialization. Thus, there is presumptive evidence that bilateral destruction of the anterior temporal poles impairs social performance as well as learned performance. The primary finding of the present research was the disclosure that bilateral removal of anterior temporal neocortex superimposed upon bilateral frontal lobectomy significantly impaired performance on a series of 20 object discrimination problems. The lesioned monkeys also showed near total loss of delayedresponse performance, but the loss was not greater than that produced by bilateral destruction of the frontal lobes. The results obtained from these combined neocortical lesions suggest that there is reason to believe that multiple lesions of the neocortex made on the basis of anatomical facts will lead to productive research. * This research was supported by USPHS grants MH-11894 and RR-00167 from the National Institutes of Health to the University of Wisconsin Primate Laboratory and Regional Primate Research Center respectively. t Requests for reprints may be addressed to Dr. H. F. Harlow, University of Wisconsin, Regional Primate Research Center, 1223 Capitol Court, Madison, Wisc. 53706. t Present address: Department of Behavioral Science, Milton S. Hershey Medical Center, Pennsylvania State University, Hershey, Pa. 1 Pribram, K. H., W. A. Wilson, and J. Connors, Exptl. Neurol., 6, 36 (1962).

582 PSYCHOLOGY: HARLOW ET AL. PROC. N. A. S. 2Harlow, H. F., A. J. Blomquist, C. I. Thompson, and K. A. Schiltz, in The Neuropsychology of Development, ed. R. L. Isaacson (New York: Wiley, 1968), p. 79. 8 Harlow, H. F., C. I. Thompson, A. J. Blomquist, and K. A. Schiltz, Brain Research, in press. 4 Warren, J. M., and H. F. Harlow, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 45, 119 (1952). 5Kling, A., and T. J. Tucker, Brain Research, 6, 428 (1967). 6 Myers, R. E., in Brain Mechanism Underlying Speech and Language (New York: Grune & Stratton, 1967), p. 61. 7Pandya, D. N., and H. G. J. M. Kuypers, Brain Research, 13, 13 (1969). 8 Kuypers, H. G. J. M., M. K. Szwarcbart, M. Mishkin, and H. E. Rcsvold, Exp. Neurol., 11, 245 (1965). 9 Harlow, H. F., K. Akert, and K. A. Schiltz, in The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior, ed. J. M. Warren and K. Akert (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964), p. 126. 10 Harlow, H. F., K. A. Schiltz, and M. K. Harlow, in Proceedings of the 2nd International Congress of Primatology, in press. 11Winer, B. J., Statistical Principles in Experimental Design (New York, McGraw-Hill, 1962). 12Jacobsen, C. F., Comp. Psychol. Monogr., 13, 3 (1936). 13 Lashley, K. S., Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1929). 14Pribram, K. H., Ann. Rev. Psychol., 11, 1 (1960). 16 Harlow, H. F., R. T. Davis, P. H. Settlage, and D. R. Meyer, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 45, 419 (1952). 16 Teuber, H. L., Ann. Rev. Psychol., 6, 276 (1955). 17Meyer, D. R., in Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior, ed. H. F. Harlow and C. N. Woolsey (Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1958), p. 173. Is Riopelle, A. J., R. G. Alper, P. N. Strong, and H. W. Ades, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 46, 145 (1953). 19 Chow, K. L., J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 47, 194 (1954). 10 Pribram, K. H., and M. Mishkin, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 48, 198 (195;5)). 21Myers, R. E., and C. Swett, Jr., Brain Research, in press.