Disgust sensitivity and the sex di erence in fears to common indigenous animals

Similar documents
Disgust Sensitivity, Blood± Injection±Injury Fear, and Dental Anxiety

Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1998, 26, Cambridge University Press. Printed in the United Kingdom

A comparison between three and ve factor models of Pakistani personality data

Monitoring and anxiety disorders symptoms in children

The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale-Revised: Psychometric properties and specificity in relation to anxiety disorder symptoms

Reliability, factor structure and validity of the Dutch Eysenck Personality Pro ler

Relationships between thought±action fusion, thought suppression and obsessive±compulsive symptoms: a structural equation modeling approach

Moderating and Mediating Variables in Psychological Research

University of Warwick institutional repository:

Differential aversive outcome expectancies for high- and low-predation fear-relevant animals

Heavy drinking among college students is in uenced by anxiety sensitivity, gender, and contexts for alcohol use

How serious are common childhood fears?

Dissociative experiences and interrogative suggestibility in college students

Subjective sleep problems in later life as predictors of cognitive decline. Report from the Maastricht Ageing Study MAAS)

The revised version of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED-R): Factor structure in normal children


26. Measurement in psychology

A STUDY OF PERSONALITY FACTOR AND JOB STRESS IN HIGH AND LOW BURN OUT GROUPS OF FEMALE PHYSICAL EDUCATION TEACHERS

Specificity of disgust sensitivity in the prediction of behavioral avoidance in contamination fear

Neuroticism, preattentive and attentional biases towards threat, and anxiety before and after a severe stressor: a prospective study

The secular increase in test scores is a ``Jensen e ect''

Trait motivational correlates of athleticism

Social desirability, defensiveness and self-report psychiatric inventory scores

Testing contingency hypotheses in budgetary research: an evaluation of the use of moderated regression analysis $

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS. In this chapter the results of the empirical research are reported and discussed in the following order:

Lucid dreaming: associations with internal locus of control, need for cognition and creativity

Evaluating framing e ects

Running head: MALE AND FEMALE CLERGY 1. In ordained ministry there is neither male nor female? The personality profile of male and

Differences in stress responses : Match effects in gender, ethnicity, and social support van Well, S.M.

Psychometric properties of the Dickman Impulsivity Inventory

The Relationship of Competitiveness and Achievement Orientation to Participation in Sport and Nonsport Activities

The Impact of Emotional Intelligence towards Relationship of Personality and Self-Esteem at Workplace

An activity pacing scale for the chronic pain coping inventory: development in a sample of patients with bromyalgia syndrome

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

The dual pathway model of overeating Ouwens, Machteld; van Strien, T.; van Leeuwe, J. F. J.; van der Staak, C. P. F.

11/24/2017. Do not imply a cause-and-effect relationship

An adult version of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED-A)

Keywords: disgust sensitivity, obsession, obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety, fear.

Neuroticism: moderator or mediator in the relation between locus of control and depression?

Self-Esteem Mediates Relationships between Self-Concept Clarity and Perceptions of the Future

Personality and Individual Differences

LJEAVES II J EYSENCIZ 1\J G Nli\RTIN

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

Testing the Vitamin Model of Job stress in Dutch Health Care Workers Jeurissen, T.; Nyklicek, Ivan

Does Endorsement of the Disease Concept of Alcoholism Predict Humanitarian Attitudes to Alcoholics?

STRESS AND AROUSAL CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS WORKING DAY AND NIGHT SHIFTS

Physical and psychological correlates of appearance orientation

Nonshared Environmental Influences and Personality Differences in Adult Twins

Running head: FREUD VS. EYSENCK 1

anxiety and other variables James A.K. Erskine, Lia Kvavilashvili and Diana E. Kornbrot University of Hertfordshire.

Metacognitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: An open trial

Citation for published version (APA): Pielage, S. B. (2006). Adult attachment and psychosocial functioning. s.n.

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Interpretive Report. Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D.

To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors

The Role of Task Specificity in the Relations between Personality and Prosocial Behaviors: A Meta-Analysis

The "Disgusting" Spider: The Role of Disease and Illness in the Perpetuation of Fear of Spiders

A comparison of e ects measured with isotonic and isometric recording: II. Concentration-e ect curves for physiological antagonists

GENETICAL, ENVIRONMENTAL AND PERSONALITY FACTORS INFLUENCING THE AGE OF FIRST SEXUAL INTERCOURSE IN TWINS

Extraversion. The Extraversion factor reliability is 0.90 and the trait scale reliabilities range from 0.70 to 0.81.

The Role of Self-discipline in Predicting Achievement for 10th Graders

University of Groningen. Social phobia and personality disorders van Velzen, C.J.M.

Daniel Boduszek University of Huddersfield

Citation for published version (APA): Weert, E. V. (2007). Cancer rehabilitation: effects and mechanisms s.n.

Lumina Spark Development of an Integrated Assessment of Big 5. Personality Factors, Type Theory & Overextension

(This is a sample cover image for this issue. The actual cover is not yet available at this time.)

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

(Received 30 March 1990)

PERSONALITY OF POLISH SOLDIERS AND THEIR WAY OF STRESS COPING DURING BOSNIA PEACEKEEPING MISSION

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

Experimental Design Notes

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Emotional Intelligence and the Myers Briggs Type Indicator

By Lora A. Connor B.A., 2008, California State University, Long Beach

The Relationship between Personality Dimensions and Religious Orientation

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Occupational stress and gender: a cross-cultural study

Perfectionism, silencing the self, and depression I

Supplementary Materials Are taboo words simply more arousing?

Journal of Anxiety Disorders

Reliability and validity of the International Spinal Cord Injury Basic Pain Data Set items as self-report measures

Correlation and Regression

Commentary: The role of intrapersonal psychological variables in academic school learning

Suggestive Interviewer Behaviour in Surveys: An Experimental Study 1

Associate Prof. Dr Anne Yee. Dr Mahmoud Danaee

warwick.ac.uk/lib-publications

J. A. Gray s reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding stimuli

Alterations in self-perceptions following childhood onset of spinal cord injury

Exposure to lad magazines and drive for muscularity in dating and non-dating young men

The e ect of spiritual beliefs on outcome from illness

Effects of personality stress and lifestyle on hypertension: An Australian twin study

THE TRANSCENDENTAL MEDITATION PROGRAMME IN BRITISH SECONDARY SCHOOLS

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

FAMILY RESILIENCY, UNCERTAINTY, OPTIMISM, AND THE QUALITY OF LIFE OF INDIVIDUALS WITH HIV/AIDS

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note

EYSENCK PERSONALITY INVENTORY: IMPULSMTY/ NEUROTICISM AND SOCIAL DESIRABILITY RESPONSE SET '

FAMILIES OF TEENAGERS WITH DOWN'S SYNDROME: PARENT, CHILD AND SIBLING ADAPTATION

CHAPTER-V SUMMARY, CONCLUSION AND RECOMMENDATION

A NEW DAWN SHEDS LIGHT ON DIABETES PSYCHOLOGY

12/31/2016. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2

Transcription:

PERGAMON Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 BEHAVIOUR RESEARCH AND THERAPY Shorter communication Disgust sensitivity and the sex di erence in fears to common indigenous animals Willem A. Arrindell a, *, Sandra Mulkens b, Jeroen Kok a, Joost Vollenbroek a a Department of Clinical Psychology, Academic Hospital of the University of Groningen, P.O. Box 30.001, 9700 RB Groningen, The Netherlands b Department of Experimental Abnormal Psychology, Maastricht University, P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands Abstract Davey's mediational hypothesis [Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Self-reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: the role of disgust sensitivity. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 541±554.] suggests that the sex di erence in self-assessed animal fears can be accounted for by the sex di erence in disgust sensitivity. An empirical test failed to support this hypothesis in a non-clinical sample (N = 214). Holding constant the in uences of confounders such as age, fear of contamination, sex roles, neuroticism, psychoticism and disgust sensitivity, biological sex kept emerging as a signi cant predictor in relation to four types of animal fears (fear-relevant animals, dry or non-slimy invertebrates, slimy or wet looking animals and farm animals). Other things being equal, high disgust sensitivity either lost its predictive capability (in relation to dry or non-slimy invertebrates and slimy or wet looking animals) or predicted high fear of fear-relevant animals and of farm animals inequivalently across, respectively, the sexes (high in females only) and age groups (high in the old only). A multifactorial, interactionist approach should be advocated in the study of the aetiology of animal fears if progress in this area is to be achieved. # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction In spite of the prevalence of animal fears, there have been few attempts to establish some of the general causal factors which might underlie these fears. Davey (1994) found signi cant relationships in both males and females between fear of animals in the invertebrate and fear- * Corresponding author. E-mail: w.arrindell@ppsw.rug.nl 0005-7967/99/$ - see front matter # 1999 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0005-7967(98)00129-6

274 W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 relevant categories on the one hand and disgust sensitivity levels on the other hand, with females also reporting signi cantly higher levels of disgust sensitivity than their male counterparts. Davey (1994) concluded that between 20 and 25% of the variance in the two main categories of animal fears could be accounted for by individual disgust sensitivity levels and since females exhibited signi cantly higher disgust sensitivity levels than males, this factor could be held responsible for a majority of the gender di erences in prevalent animal fears. In other words, Davey (1994) actually argued that evidence was found for the notion that the magnitude of the sex di erence in animal fears was a ected by disgust sensitivity or that such a sex di erence could be accounted for by sex di erences in disgust sensitivity. Hence, that disgust sensitivity would mediate the biological sex±animal fear relationship (henceforth: the mediational hypothesis). However, Davey's claim is premature for having been drawn on the basis of inadequate statistical data analysis. Baron and Kenny (1986) have noted that to establish a mediated relationship, a series of regression models should be estimated. One should estimate the three following regression equations: rst, regressing the mediator (disgust sensitivity) on the independent variable (biological sex); second, regressing the dependent variable (any dimension of animal fears) on the independent variable and, third, regressing the dependent variable on both the independent variable and on the mediator. Separate coe cients for each equation should be estimated and tested. The nal and most critical test of mediation which was not carried out by Davey (1994) involves observation of the change in the magnitude of the relationship between biological sex and the dependent measures produced by controlling for disgust sensitivity. To establish mediation, the following conditions must hold: (1) the independent variable must a ect the mediator in the rst equation; (2) the independent variable must be shown to a ect the dependent variable in the second equation and (3) the mediator must a ect the dependent variable in the third equation. If these conditions all hold in the predicted direction, then the e ect of the independent variable on the dependent variable must be less in the third equation than in the second. Perfect mediation holds if the independent variable has no e ect when the mediator is controlled: put somewhat di erently, if the association reduces to zero, one has strong evidence for a single, dominant mediator; if a reduction to zero does not occur, this indicates the operation of multiple mediating factors. The aims of the present study were two-fold. Using a non-clinical sample, it set out to (1) address the question as to whether disgust sensitivity could be taken to account for the sex di erence in animal fears and (2) study the predictive capabilities on animal fears of biological sex and disgust sensitivity independent of other potential predictors. These additional predictors included age, fear of contamination, masculinity, femininity and the major Eysenckian dimensions of personality termed psychoticism and neuroticism 1. Aim number (2) extends the Davey (1994) study in the following respect. The inclusion of additional potential correlates and predictors of self-assessed animal fears to the ones that were of central importance to the Davey (1994) study would do justice to the possibility that the 1 Limitations of space preclude an outline of the empirical arguments for inclusion of these speci c predictors. An extended report of this article which is available upon request contains these arguments and the relevant sources on which they are based.

W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 275 relationships of biological sex and/or disgust sensitivity to animal fears might re ect spurious associations. This situation would occur when the relationships between biological sex or disgust sensitivity and animal fears are solely the result of the fact that either one or both of these variables are correlated with both another potential predictor (e.g. fear of contamination) and the criterion measure (self-assessed animal fears) (e.g. Baron & Kenny, 1986). 2. Method 2.1. Subjects 2 The series comprised 214 community S's who volunteered to participate in a study on personality and fears. There were 112 females (52.3%) and 102 males (47.7%) with a joint mean age of 30 yr (S.D. = 13 yr; range: 14±79 yr). 2.2. Measures Animal Fears Questionnaire (AFQ): the list of 35 animals included in the survey was based on Davey (1994, p. 542). Factor analysis of the self-ratings of the present sample of S's (Arrindell, submitted) yielded four factors: (1) fear-relevant animals (e.g. mouse, rat, snake), (2) dry or non-slimy invertebrates (e.g. wasp, cockroach, spider), (3) slimy or wet looking animals (e.g. snail, slug, eel, sh) and (4) farm animals (e.g. cow, goat, horse) 3. Disgust Sensitivity Questionnaire (DSQ): the DSQ (Rozin, Fallon, & Mandell, 1984) is a 24- item self-report measure that is widely used for measuring disgust sensitivity (e.g. Merckelbach, de Jong, Arntz, & Schouten, 1993; Davey, 1994; Mulkens, de Jong, & Merckelbach, 1996; de Jong, Andrea, & Muris, 1997). The DSQ asks S's to rate on a 9-point scale how much they would like to eat `contaminated' food items. It should be pointed out that the DSQ does not measure fear, phobia or avoidance behaviour. The short Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI) is a widely-employed, reliable and valid measure of masculinity (Mas) and femininity (Fem) (Bem, 1981; Arrindell, Kolk, Pickersgill, & Hageman, 1993). Padua Inventory-Contamination subscale: this is a reliable and valid, factorially-derived (Sanavio, 1988) 10-item measure that focuses on contamination fear. The abbreviated Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised is a reliable and valid multidimensional measure of four personality traits: psychoticism (or toughmindedness), extraversion, neuroticism and lie (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1991; Sanderman, Arrindell, Ranchor, Eysenck, & Eysenck, 1995, Dutch version). Measures were scored in such a way that higher scores pointed to stronger (higher) endorsements of the states or traits assessed. 2 The extended report also describes the sample in terms of marital, educational and occupational status. Neither on age nor on any of these variables did the sexes di er from one another. 3 The rst author wishes to thank Professor Graham Davey for his suggestions in naming of the factors.

276 W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 2.3. Statistical analysis As pointed out above, to test mediation three regression equations should be estimated using multiple regression analysis. To rule out the possibility of inequivalence of disgust sensitivity e ects across sex or age, age disgust sensitivity and biological sex disgust sensitivity product terms were included in the regression analyses in addition to an age biological sex product term. Interactions were examined two-sidedly. Multicollinearity between interaction terms and their constituent main e ects was counteracted by centering variables prior to multiplication (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). 3. Results and discussion 3.1. The mediational hypothesis 4 Biological sex (male = 1, female = 2) was signi cantly associated with the alledged mediator (disgust sensitivity) (0.17, P < 0.01, one-tailed), indicating that females reported higher disgust sensitivity levels than their male counterparts (small e ect size in terms of Cohen, 1992), which is a replication of Davey (1994) and others (e.g. Wronska, 1990). Thus, the rst criterion proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986) was met. Again in line with Davey (1994), females reported higher levels on all dimensions of animals fears: medium e ect-sized r's were observed in relation to fear-relevant animals (0.34, P < 0.0001) and dry or non-slimy invertebrates (0.32, P < 0.0001), whereas small e ect-sized r's were yielded in relation to slimy or wet looking animals (0.22, P < 0.001) and farm animals (0.17, P < 0.01). Thus, Baron and Kenny's second criterion was also satis ed. Third, high disgust sensitivity must be related to high self-reported levels of fear. Once more in agreement with Davey (1994), high levels of disgust sensitivity were associated with high levels of self-reported fears of fear-relevant animals (0.35, P < 0.001, medium e ect size), of dry or non-slimy invertebrates (0.25, P < 0.001, medium e ect size) and of slimy or wet looking animals (0.12, P < 0.05, small e ect size). Disgust sensitivity, however, was not signi cantly associated with farm animals (0.10, NS). Thus, condition number 3 was satis ed for three out of four fear measures. The nal and most critical test of mediation is the change in the magnitude of the relationship between biological sex and self-assessed fears produced by controlling for disgust sensitivity. Reduced-form equations, a form of hierarchical multiple regression analysis (Cohen & Cohen, 1983), was estimated to assess change in magnitudes of biological sex±animal fear relationships. A six-step regression equation was estimated in relation to each animal fear dimension in the following order: (1) age, (2) biological sex, (3) disgust sensitivity, (4) age biological sex, (5) age disgust sensitivity, and (6) biological sex disgust sensitivity. The critical comparison involved the change in standardised regression coe cients for biological sex 4 Marital, educational and occupational status and extraversion and lie were not signi cantly associated with any of the dependent variables.

W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 277 from step 2 (total e ect) to step 3 (direct e ect controlling for disgust sensitivity). Table 1 summarizes the outcome. As shown in Table 1, biological sex accounted for 3 to 11% of the variance across the four fear measures after controlling for age. Adding the disgust sensitivity measure (at step 3) also produced signi cant increments in R 2 across all fear measures, accounting for an additional 0.4 to 9% of the variance. The change in regression weights for biological sex from step 2 to step 3 indicates that controlling for disgust sensitivity reduced the b's for biological sex only slightly, namely by 5 to 15%. This is de nitely not a reduction of any b to zero; and the reductions clearly re ect less than 40% change in which case one could have spoken of a sizeable drop, i.e. one where the e ect size (e.g. r) would be reduced from a higher magnitude class to a lower one (cf. Cohen, 1992, p. 157). Biological sex retained its predictive power in relation to each of the measures of self-assessed animals fears, independent of the predictive capabilities of disgust sensitivity. These observations thus refute the claim made by Davey (1994) that disgust sensitivity could be held responsible for sex di erences in animals fears. The signi cant interactions shown in Table 1 will be discussed further on. Table 1 Reduction in standardized regression coe cients for biological sex controlling for disgust sensitivity and possible interactions between main e ects Step Dependent variables fear-relevant animals dry or non-slimy invertebrates slimy or wet looking animals farm animals Step 2 R 2 change for biological sex entry 0.11 *** 0.11 *** 0.05 *** 0.03 * b for biological sex 0.33 *** 0.33 *** 0.22 *** 0.17 ** Step 3 R 2 change for disgust sensitivity entry 0.09 *** 0.05 *** 0.01 *** 0.004 * b for biological sex controlling for disgust sensitivity 0.28 *** 0.30 *** 0.21 ** 0.16 * % Reduction in b for biological sex 15% 9% 5% 6% b for disgust sensitivity 0.31 *** 0.23 *** 0.11 0.07 Step 4: b for age biological sex 0.07 0.02 0.03 0.02 Step 5: b for age disgust sensitivity 0.06 0.02 0.04 0.21 ** Step 6: b for biological sex disgust sensitivity 0.18 ** 0.11 0.05 0.10 Multiple R (R 2 ) 0.49 (0.24) 0.44 (0.19) 0.28 (0.08) 0.30 (0.09) E ect size, f 2 0.32, large 0.24, medium 0.09, small 0.10, small F (df = 6, 206) 11.02 *** 8.27 *** 2.99 ** 3.32 ** Minimum pairwise N = 213. Main e ects have been evaluated one-sidedly, interaction e ects: two-sidedly. * PR0.05, ** PR0.01, *** PR0.001. Regression equations were estimated controlling for age entered in step 1.

278 W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 Table 2 Regressions of age, biological sex, disgust sensitivity, contamination fear, sex roles, two Eysenkian personality dimensions and two interaction terms (age disgust sensitivity and biological sex disgust sensitivity) on four dimensions of animal fears Animal fears fear-relevant animals dry or non-slimy invertebrates slimy or wet looking animals farm animals Predictors b partial r b partial r b partial r b partial r Age disgust sensitivity 0.01 0.01 0.07 0.08 0.04 0.04 0.17 * 0.17 * Masculinity 0.17 ** 0.18 ** 0.18 ** 0.19 ** 0.03 0.03 0.13 * 0.12 * Biological sex 0.28 *** 0.30 *** 0.28 *** 0.31 *** 0.22 *** 0.23 *** 0.19 ** 0.19 ** Neuroticism 0.03 0.04 0.11 * 0.13 * 0.10 0.10 0.07 0.07 Age 0.06 0.06 0.19 ** 0.21 ** 0.19 ** 0.20 ** 0.03 0.03 Biological sex disgust sensitivity 0.18 ** 0.20 ** 0.09 0.11 0.05 0.05 0.08 0.08 Disgust sensitivity 0.22 *** 0.22 *** 0.10 0.11 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 Femininity 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.01 0.20 ** 0.19 ** 0.12 * 0.12 * Psychoticism 0.05 0.05 0.06 0.07 0.12 * 0.12 * 0.11 0.11 Contamination fear 0.15 ** 0.16 ** 0.23 *** 0.24 *** 0.16 * 0.15 * 0.08 0.08 Multiple R (R 2 ) 0.55 (0.30) 0.56 (0.31) 0.40 (0.16) 0.38 (0.15) E ect size, f 2 0.43, large 0.45, large 0.19, medium 0.18, medium F (df = 10, 197) 8.50 *** 8.84 *** 3.65 *** 3.33 *** Minimum pairwise N = 208. Main e ects have been evaluated one-sidedly, interaction e ects: two-sidedly. * PR0.05, ** PR0.01, *** PR0.001. 3.2. Fear of contamination 5, sex roles and Eysenckian personality dimensions held constant The ndings of conventional multiple regression analysis could be summarized as follows (see Table 2). 3.2.1. Fear-relevant animals Low masculinity, being female (the strongest predictor), high disgust sensitivity and high fear of contamination (a replication of Ware, Jain, Burgess, and Davey, 1994) emerged as signi cant, independent predictors of high levels of fear of fear-relevant animals. In addition, the biological sex disgust sensitivity product term also emerged as a signi cant predictor of this type of fear, indicating that, when the in uences of masculinity and fear of contamination were held constant, disgust sensitivity no longer correlated with nor predicted fear levels in males (partial r = 0.05, b = 0.05 NS), whereas in females signi cant relationships of medium e ect sizes were obtained (partial r = 0.35, b = 0.36, t = 3.79, P < 0.001). 5 Fear of contamination is distinct from disgust sensitivity. The overlap between both measures is only 15% (r = 0.39, P < 0.001, one-tailed). In addition, both predictors occupy quite di erent locations in the dimensional space de ned by the major higher-order dimensions of personality in the Eysenckian system (see Arrindell, submitted).

W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 279 3.2.2. Dry or non-slimy invertebrates Again, low masculinity, being female and high fear of contamination emerged as signi cant, independent predictors of high fear levels. In addition, high neuroticism and young age also predicted high levels of fear. Thus, when additional predictors were introduced, disgust sensitivity lost its predictive capability and just fell short of statistical signi cance (t = 1.51, P = 0.065). 3.2.3. Slimy or wet looking animals Being female, low femininity, young age, high fear of contamination and low toughmindedness (i.e. low psychoticism) were the signi cant, independent predictors of high levels of fears of slimy or wet looking animals. Again, the predictive power of disgust sensitivity vanished when additional predictors were introduced. 3.2.4. Farm animals Being female, low masculinity and low femininity predicted high levels of fear. Moreover, the age disgust sensitivity product term too emerged as a signi cant predictor. Taking signi cant main e ects into account, this signi cant interaction term pointed to non-signi cant negative relationships between disgust sensitivity and fear levels in the young, i.e. S's younger than 30 yr (partial r = 0.10, b = 0.10, t = 1.23, P = 0.11 NS), whereas in the old, i.e. S's of 30 yr or older, signi cant relationships of practically medium e ect sizes were yielded in the opposite direction, with high disgust sensitivity predicting high levels of animal fears (partial r = 0.26, b = 0.27, t = 2.12, P < 0.05). 4. General discussion Davey (1994) suggested that the sex di erence in animal fears could be accounted for by sex di erences in disgust sensitivity. Application in the present study of the appropriate statistical technique for demonstrating the mediational e ect of disgust sensitivity showed no convincing evidence for such an e ect in relation to any of the animal fears dimensions. Instead, biological sex kept predicting self-reports of each type of animal fears, independent of the predictive capability of disgust sensitivity. Even when other potential predictors (age, sex roles, Eysenckian personality dimensions) were also introduced, biological sex kept emerging as a signi cant predictor of each type of animal fears. By contrast, disgust sensitivity either lost its predictive capability in relation to fears of dry or non-slimy invertebrates and of slimy or wet looking animals or revealed complex predictive power in relation to fear of fear-relevant animals and of farm animals. Thus, other things being equal, high disgust sensitivity predicted (a) high levels of fear of fear-relevant animals in females but not in males and (b) high levels of fear of farm animals in the `old', but not in the `young'. The present ndings clearly argue, and obviously so, for a multifactorial, psycho-bio-social approach in the further study of the aetiology of animal fears. Such an approach should not only include background factors (biological sex, age), sex roles, major dimensions of personality, disgust sensitivity and fear of contamination as potential predictors, but also traumatic experiences, sex role stress, genetic aspects (e.g. intrafamilial resemblances in disgust

280 W.A. Arrindell et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy 37 (1999) 273±280 sensitivity and fear of contamination), parental rearing factors (Arrindell, Kolk, Pickersgill, & Hageman, 1993; Davey, Forster, & Mayhew, 1993; de Jong, Andrea, & Muris, 1997), environmental aspects of familiarity or exposure to animals, and interactions among these factors that are of interest. References Arrindell, W. A. (submitted). Phobic dimensions (Vol. IV: the structure of animal fears). Arrindell, W. A., Kolk, A. M., Pickersgill, M. J., & Hageman, W. J. J. M. (1993). Biological sex, sex role orientation, masculine sex role stress, dissimulation and self-reported fears. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 103±146. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173±1182. Bem, S. L. (1981). Bem Sex-Role Inventory: Professional Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155±159. Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Self-reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: the role of disgust sensitivity. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 541±554. Davey, G. C. L., Forster, L., & Mayhew, G. (1993). Familial resemblances in disgust sensitivity and animal phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 41±50. de Jong, P. J., Andrea, H., & Muris, P. (1997). Spider phobia in children: disgust and fear before and after treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 559±562. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1991). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Scales (EPS Adult). London: Hodder and Stoughton. Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P. J., Arntz, A., & Schouten, E. (1993). The role of evaluative learning and disgust sensitivity in the etiology and treatment of spider phobia. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 243±255. Mulkens, S. A. N., de Jong, P. J., & Merckelbach, H. (1996). Disgust and spider phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 464± 468. Rozin, P., Fallon, A., & Mandell, R. (1984). Family resemblance in attitudes to foods. Developmental Psychology, 20, 309±314. Sanavio, E. (1988). Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 26, 169±177. Sanderman, R., Arrindell, W. A., Ranchor, A. V., Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1995). Het Meten van Persoonlijkheidskenmerken met de Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ): Een Handleiding. Groningen, The Netherlands, Noordelijk Centrum voor Gezondheidsvraagstukken. Ware, J., Jain, K., Burgess, I., & Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Disease-avoidance model: factor analysis of common animal fears. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 57±63. Wronska, J. (1990). Disgust in relation to emotionality, extraversion, psychoticism and imagery abilities. In P. J. D. Drenth, J. A. Sergeant, & R. J. Takens (Eds.), European Perspectives in Psychology (Vol. 1). Chichester: Wiley.