A linear relation between loudness and decibels

Similar documents
Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2016 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Modeling Human Perception

Psychology of Perception PSYC Spring 2017 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

Effects of aural combination tones on the loudness of a pure tone masked by an inharmonic pure-tone

Lab 3: Perception of Loudness

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

Lab 4: Perception of Loudness

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Loudness. Loudness is not simply sound intensity!

Technical Discussion HUSHCORE Acoustical Products & Systems

Tactile Communication of Speech

THE MECHANICS OF HEARING

Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 27, , June tleseareh Note RELATION BETWEEN REACTION TIME AND LOUDNESS

Thresholds for different mammals

Outline. 4. The Ear and the Perception of Sound (Psychoacoustics) A.1 Outer Ear Amplifies Sound. Introduction

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS IN INFORMATION INTEGRATION

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Basic Environmental Noise and Noise Perception. 4-Feb-16

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Influence of Frequency on Difference Thresholds for Magnitude of Vertical Sinusoidal Whole-Body Vibration

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

Pigeons and the Magical Number Seven

PERCEPTUAL MEASUREMENT OF BREATHY VOICE QUALITY

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Speech Intelligibility Measurements in Auditorium

Sequential dependencies and regression in psychophysical judgments*

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Loudness Processing of Time-Varying Sounds: Recent advances in psychophysics and challenges for future research

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

SUBJECT: Physics TEACHER: Mr. S. Campbell DATE: 15/1/2017 GRADE: DURATION: 1 wk GENERAL TOPIC: The Physics Of Hearing

Supplement. Aircraft Noise Terminology & Metric

The development of a modified spectral ripple test

Masked Perception Thresholds of Low Frequency Tones Under Background Noises and Their Estimation by Loudness Model

TESTING A NEW THEORY OF PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: TEMPORAL LOUDNESS INTEGRATION

9/29/14. Amanda M. Lauer, Dept. of Otolaryngology- HNS. From Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics, Green & Swets (1966)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Categorical Perception

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

ID# Exam 2 PS 325, Fall 2009

A comparison of method-of-adjustment and forced-choice procedures in frequency discrimination

Hearing. and other senses

functions grow at a higher rate than in normal{hearing subjects. In this chapter, the correlation

3M Center for Hearing Conservation

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100 (4), Pt. 1, October /96/100(4)/2393/22/$ Acoustical Society of America 2393

Sound localization psychophysics

EEL 6586, Project - Hearing Aids algorithms

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

Effects of partial masking for vehicle sounds

Psychoacoustical Models WS 2016/17

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency. (Some slides are adapted from Zhiyao Duan s course slides on Computer Audition and Its Applications in Music)

Learning to detect a tone in unpredictable noise

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Outline. The ear and perception of sound (Psychoacoustics) A.1 Outer Ear Amplifies Sound. Introduction

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Spectrograms (revisited)

The Effect of Analysis Methods and Input Signal Characteristics on Hearing Aid Measurements

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

1 A Short Course on Sound and Human Hearing. Jed Margolin

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

Categorical loudness scaling and equal-loudness contours in listeners with normal hearing and hearing loss

Behavioral generalization

Sources of Variability in Magnitude Estimates

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Range and Regression, Loudness Scales, and Loudness Processing: Toward a Context-Bound Psychophysics

Attention bands in absolute identification

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks

Frequency refers to how often something happens. Period refers to the time it takes something to happen.

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA

Magnitude Estimation of Conceptual Data Dimensions for Use in Sonification

Vibrotactile masking:,a comparison of psychophysical procedures

Review of Methods for Quantifying Tonalness in Noise. Quantifying Tonalness

A LABORATORY STUDY OF SUBJECTIVE ANNOYANCE RESPONSE TO SONIC BOOMS AND AIRCRAFT FLYOVERS. Jack D. Leatherwood and Brenda M.

The clinical Link. Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emission

CONTRIBUTION OF DIRECTIONAL ENERGY COMPONENTS OF LATE SOUND TO LISTENER ENVELOPMENT

Spectral-peak selection in spectral-shape discrimination by normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Hearing. Juan P Bello

Reference: Mark S. Sanders and Ernest J. McCormick. Human Factors Engineering and Design. McGRAW-HILL, 7 TH Edition. NOISE

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

Absolute Identification Is Relative: A Reply to Brown, Marley, and Lacouture (2007)

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

Sensory Thresholds and Signal Detection. Lecture 13

64 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON BIOMEDICAL ENGINEERING, VOL. 61, NO. 1, JANUARY 2014

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract

Perceptual pitch shift for sounds with similar waveform autocorrelation

Spectral processing of two concurrent harmonic complexes

Hearing. Figure 1. The human ear (from Kessel and Kardon, 1979)

The Hearwig as hearing protector for musicians a practical investigation

Masker-signal relationships and sound level

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2003 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

..." , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1

Transcription:

Perception & Psychophysics 1984, 36 (4), 338-342 A linear relation between loudness and decibels E. C. POULTON Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge, England A total of 37 uninitiated observers made repeated numerical magnitude judgments of the loudness of a sequence of octave band noises spaced at I-dB intervals, from 0 to 5 db above a standard of about 80 db(a), which was called 10. The observers were not instructed to use numbers as ratios. When the median responses are plotted linearly against decibels, they are fitted by straight lines. Each extra decibel adds an average of 1 unit of loudness, range for individual observers 10 through.25 units. This is consistent with the view that the subjectively equal stimulus spacing for the loudness of noise is linear in decibels, and that. the observers use numbers linearly in judging the loudness. The investigation illustrated in Figures I and 2 was conducted to test the effect of a very small stimulus range of5 db on numerical magnitude judgments. Model A of Poulton's (1968) Figure I predicts that as the stimulus range is reduced, the slope of the numerical magnitude judgments will become steeper. This is because the observers' responses do not fully reflect the change in the size of the stimulus range. The investigation was sparked off in 1969 after I read the preprint of the first part of R. Teghtsoonian's (1971) review on the range of sensory magnitude (R. Teghtsoonian, 1969). The investigation is a sequel to Poulton (1969). In the earlier investigation, four separate groups of observers judgedthe loudness of an octave band ofnoise over a geometric range of 35 db. In the sequel, instead of using four different fixed orders for four separate groups of observers, the stimuli were presented in the same fixed order to a single group of observers. As previously, the order of the six stimuli was held constant over two repetitions, to examine the effects of practice unconfounded by changes in asymmetric transfer (Poulton & Freeman, 1966). The effects ofpractice are illustrated in Figure 1, summed over all observers. Practice reduces the variability shown by the group medians; but practice does not eliminate the confusion in the middle of the range of stimuli between the variables 2 and 3 db more intense than the standard. Linear Unbiased Relation Between Loudness and Decibels As in the 1969 investigation, the observers were not instructed to use numbers as ratios. The group medians of Figure 1 and the medians for individual observers of Figure 2 summed over the three judgments of each stimu- The author is grateful to the late A. Carpenter for producing the stimuli and recording them on magnetic tape. M. 1. Nimmo-Smith advised on statistics. Financial support from the British Medical Research Council is also gratefully acknowledged. Requests for reprints should be addressed to E. C. Poulton, M.R.C. Applied Psychology Unit, 15 Chaucer Road, Cambridge CB2 2EF, England. Ius show that there is a linear relation between judged loudness and the intensity of the octave band of noise in decibels. In Panel A offigure 1, the median responses are plotted on a linear scale. On the third and last presentation of the set of six stimuli, when the variability is smallest, the medians are well fitted by a straight line using the method of least squares. The exception is the point.representing an intensity 2 db above the standard, which receives the same median response as the point representing 3 db above the standard. In Panel B of Figure I, the median responses are plotted on a logarithmic scale. On the third presentation, the medians show a systematic deviation from the best fitting straight line. The function is slightly concave downward. Thus, the log-log plot of Panel B is not the appropriate plot for giving a straight line function. The relation between loudness and intensity is linear in decibels.. Figure 2 shows a similar linear relation for the medians of individual observers, averaged over the three judgments of each stimulus. Using the method of least squares, all but 4 of the 37 individual functions appear to be fitted as well by a straight line as by a simple curve. However, functions 19 and 20 look as if they would be fitted better by a concave downward curve, whereas functions 21 and 22 look as if they would be fitted better by a concave upward curve. Plotting the median responses on a log scale would improve the fit of a straight line to the two concave upward functions, but would worsen the fit to the two concave downward functions. Taken overall, the individual observers show a more or less linear relation between loudness and noise level in decibels, like the median observers of Figure 1. The slopes ofthe individual functions in Figure 2 indicate the average number of units of loudness which the observers judge to separate one stimulus from the next largest stimulus. Observer 1 in Panel A appears to use units of 10, whereas Observer 2 uses units of5. Most of the observers in Panel B probably use units of2. The median observers of Figure 1, and many of the observers Copyright 1984 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 338

17 ~ c:l :;:: 16 L.. c:l 2 15 III 14 c. III ~ -;; 13 u L.. ~ 12 ::J c C.s11 ""CJ ::E 10 rr--'----'--'-'---'----.---,.--,r---r-, "r---r----,---,--.--.--.----,---.-.,..., A Group medians on linear scale LOUDNESS IS LINEAR IN DECIBELS 339..' 2 nḍ 3rd 17... 16~ cr : u III 15 01 :.9 14 ~ c0 c. 13~ d u 12 'iii E ::J C Group 11 c c:l medians on '6 log scale ::E 10 Figure 1. The least squares linear relation between judged loudness and the intensity of the noise in decibels. In Panel A, the median numerical responses of the 37 observers are plotted on a linear scale. On the third preseatatlen, the function is well fitted by a straight line. In Panel B, the responses are plotted logarithmically. On the third presentation, the function is concave downward. in Panel C of Figure 2, use units of 1.0. The observers in Panel D appear to use units of 1.0,.5, or.25. Provided the observers use numbers linearly, it does not matter how many units of loudness are judged to separate one stimulus from the next largest stimulus. One number of units is good as another, just as differences in length can be measured in centimeters or millimeters. An analysis of the very first judgments of the observers Figure 2. The median numerical responses of the individual observers plotted linearly against the noise intensity in decibels. Apart from functions 19 through 22, all the functions appear to be fitted as well by a straight line as by a simple curve. Note the different scale on the ordinate of each panel. in the 1969 investigation shows a similar linear relation between the median judgmentsof loudness and the intensity of the octave band of noise measured in decibels. Rows 2 and 3 of Figure 3 (Poulton, 1982) show the distributions of the very first judgments of the variable 20 db above the standard calledl.d. Using numbers linearly, the medians of 6 and 7 make one unit of loudness correspond to 4 and 3.3 db, respectively. Row 1 of Figure 3 shows that the variable 5 db above the standard receives a median numerical response of2.0. Thus, here one unit of loudness corresponds to 5 db. Since the observers stick almost exclusively to whole numbers, this is consistent with the values of 4 and 3.3 db per unit of loudness for the variable 20 db above the standard. Logarithmic Response Bias Predicting from the medians in the top three rows of Figure 3, the variable 35 db above the standard in the bottom row should receive a median judgment of 11. Instead, it receives a median of 20. The figure shows that this is because above 10 most observers judge in los, instead of using digit intervals as they do below 10 (Banks & Hill, 1974). This produces a logarithmic response bias, which transfers to the subsequent judgments of less intense stimuli (Poulton, in press, Figure 6.5). The logarithmic response bias is produced by a step change in the number of digits used as responses (Poulton, 1979, p. 783). There is no logarithmic response bias in Figures 1 and 2, because there is no step change. The standard is called 10, and the largest individual response ever given is only 80. Hence the linear relation between the numerical magnitude judgments of loudness and decibels, as in the top 3 rows of Figure 3.

340 POULTON 1 2 3 5 7 10 20 30 50 70 100 200 300 500 700 1000.:[L ::J5d~ : OJ 'J :"" 'J1 : r:t oij1.j 20dB ",!.. j :t IIu:J.".i..f: I I 20dB ':[., '.IL.l 35dB ': 01 2 3 5 7 10 20 30 50 70 100 200 300 500 700 1000 Numerical value of response (log scale) Figure 3. Linear responses and the logarithmic response bias in individual very first multiple numerical judgments of the loudness of noise against a less intense standard called 1.0. Each row is for a separate group of between 28 and 33 uninitiated observers. The medians are marked M. The decibel value printed in each row represents the increase in intensity of the stimulus above the standard of about 65 db. Numerical judgments smaller than 10 usually occur at digit intervals. Numerical judgments greater than 10 tend to be multiples of 10, owing to the logarithmic response bias. (After Poulton, 1982, Figure 5.) Difference Between Noises and Pure Tones The linear relation in Figures 1 and 2 between loudness and decibels applies only to noise stimuli, in which the sensitivity per decibel remains constant at suprathreshold levels (Harris, 1950; Miller, 1947). The relation can be described as a linear relation between loudness and sensitivity. Pure-tone stimuli show an apparent logarithmic response bias (Poulton, in press, chap. 6), because the sensitivity per decibel increases with sound pressure level (Houtsma, Durlach, & Braida, 1980; Jesteadt, Wier, & Green, 1977). It is necessary to correct for the increase in sensitivity per decibel in order to obtain the relation between loudness matches and sensitivity which corresponds to the linear relation of Figures 1 and 2 (Houtsma et al., 1980). The difference between noise stimuli and pure tone stimuli is reflected in the nonlinear relation between noise and pure tones, which is found by direct loudness matching. Zwicker (1958) gets 20 observers to match a I-kHz tone and broadband white noise. Scharf (1959) gets 8 observers to match a 1.5-kHz tone and a 1.6-kHz band of white noise centered on 1.5 khz. Both investigators find a similar nonlinear relation (see Poulton, in press, Figure 4.3). Stimulus Range Equalizing Bias The investigation illustrated in Figures 1 and 2 turns out not to be appropriate for studying the stimulus range equalizing bias. The observers use numbers linearly, not as ratios. Thus, it is not appropriate to compare the median geometric response range with the geometric response ranges of the 1969 investigation. The appropriate comparison would use the arithmetic response ranges of the two investigations on the very first judgment. But, unfortunately, the arithmetic response ranges are not comparable. In the investigation of Figures 1 and 2, the standard is given a modulus number of 10 in order to avoid the use of decimals or fractions in responding, whereas in the 1969 investigation the standard has a modulus of 1.0. The Teghtsoonians' (R. Teghtsoonian, 1973, Figure 4 and Table 1; R. Teghtsoonian & M. Teghtsoonian, 1978, Figure 1) investigation of the stimulus range equalizing biases in numerical magnitude judgments of loudness appears to run into similar difficulties. As in the investigation of Figures 1 and 2, the observers are not instructed to use numbers as ratios. But it is not possible to compare the Teghtsoonians' arithmetic response ranges, because each observer selects her own modulus number for the standard. The Teghtsoonians present separate groups of 12 women undergraduates with stimulus ranges of 5, 10,20, or 40 db. Ifthe median undergraduates were to use numbers logarithmically, one would expect a similar proportional change in the size of exponent in comparing stimulus ranges of 5 and 10 db, as in comparing stimulus ranges of 20 and 40 db. Yet, increasing the stimulus range from 5 to 10 db reduces the median exponent to 74 % of its size, whereas increasing the stimulus range from 20 to 40 db reduces the exponent to only 98 % of its size. This

LOUDNESS IS LINEAR IN DECIBELS 341 would happen if the median undergraduates were to follow the median observers of Figure 3 and use numbers linearly for stimulus ranges between 5 and 20 db, but use numbers logarithmically for the largest stimulus range of 40 db. The logarithmic response bias would then counteract the stimulus range equalizing bias in comparing the effects of the 20- and 40-dB stimulus ranges. The Teghtsoonians test the fit of their logarithmic response model to the responses of individual students. But they use as a minimum acceptable coefficient of determination an r 2 of only.8 (R. Teghtsoonian & M. Teghtsoonian, 1978, p. 307). With an n of only 6 points on each function, equally spaced in decibels, the minimum possible coefficient of determination is 3/(n + 1) =.43. To achieve this minimum value, the students have simply to rank order the six stimuli correctly by magnitude (Coleman, Graf, & AU, 1981). Thus, the Teghtsoonians' minimum acceptable r of only.8 does not indicate a particularly good fit to their logarithmic response model. This suggests that many of their undergraduates' so-called power functions may well be produced by using numbers linearly, instead of as ratios. For the groups with median undergraduates who use numbers linearly, it is not appropriate to calculate the median exponents from the geometric response ranges. Nor would it be appropriate to use the arithmetic response ranges, because each undergraduate selects her own modulus number for the standard. Ifthis is so, the Teghtsoonians' investigation faces difficulties similar to those encountered in the investigation of Figures 1 and 2. It is not appropriate for studying the stimulus range equalizing bias for loudness. REFERENCES BANKS, W. P., & HILL,D. K. (1974). The apparent magnitude ofnurnber scaled by random production. Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 102, 353-376. COLEMAN, B. J., GRAF, R. G., & ALF, E. F. (1981). Assessing power function relationships in magnitude estimation. Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 178-180. HARRIS, J. D. (1950). The effect ofsensation-levels on intensive discrimination of noise. American Journal ofpsychology, 63, 409-421. HOUTSMA, A. J. M., DURLACH, N. I., & BRAIDA, L. D. (1980). Intensity perception XI. Experimental results on the relation of intensity resolution to loudness matching. Journal ofthe AcousticalSociety of America, 68, 807-813. JESTEADT, W., WIER, C. C., & GREEN, D. M. (1977). Intensity discrimination as a function of frequency and sensation level. Journal ofthe Acoustical Society ofamerica, 61, 169-177. MILLER, G. A. (1947). Sensitivity to changes in the intensity of white noise and its relation to masking and loudness. Journal ofthe Acoustical Society of America, 19, 609-619. POULTON, E. C. (1968). The new psychophysics: Six models for magnitude estimation. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 1-19. POULTON, E. C. (1969). Choice of first variables for single and repeated multiple estimates of loudness. Journal ofexperimental Psychology, 80, 249-253. POULTON, E. C. (1979). Models for biases in judging sensory magnitude. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 777-803. POULTON, E. C. (1982). Biases in quantitative judgments. Applied Ergonomics, 13, 31-42. POULTON, E. C. (in press). Bias in judgment. San Francisco: Academic Press. POULTON, E. C., & FREEMAN, P. R. (1966). Unwanted asymmetrical transfer effects with balanced experimental designs. Psychological Bulletin, 66, 1-8. SCHARF, B. (1959). Loudness of complex sounds as a function of the number of components. Journal ofthe AcousticalSociety ofamerica, 31, 783-785. TEGHTSOONIAN, R. (1969, April). The range ofsensory magnitude. Unpublished manuscript. TEGHTSOONIAN, R. (1971). On the exponents in Stevens' law and the constant in Ekman's law. Psychological Review, 78, 71-80. TEGHTSOONIAN, R. (1973). Range effects in psychophysical scaling and a revision of Stevens' law. American Journal ofpsychology, 86, 3-27. TEGHTSOONIAN, R., & TEGHTSOONIAN, M. (1978). Range and regression effects in magnitude scaling. Perception & Psychophysics, 24, 305-314. ZWICKER, E. (1958).Uber psychologische und methodisch Grundlagen der Lautheit. Acustica, 8, 237-258. APPENDIX ON MEmOD Observers The observers, 23 women and 14 men, were members of a panel maintained at the Applied Psychology Unit in Cambridge. Theirages ranged from 18 to 73 years, with a medial) age of 49. They were paid for their services. None had previously judged sensory magnitudes. Two additional men, aged 63 and 77, were discarded because they did not provide a complete set of responses. Stimuli White noise of limited amplitude was passed through an octave filter with cutoffs at 700 and 1400 Hz of 20 db per octave. The two stimuli of a pair each lasted.78 sec, with an interval of 1.07 sec between them. Sound pressure levels were changed using a Muirhead attenuator. The stimuli were recorded on a Vortexion magnetic tape recorder (type CBL). The tape recording was played back using a Ferrograph recorder. The stimuli were fed to a Goodmans Axiom 22 loudspeaker, mounted in a wooden enclosure of.17 m", which was resonant at 40 Hz. The loudspeaker was placed against the middle ofone of the long sides of the test room, which was approximately rectangular with 4.9 x 7.6 m sides and a height of 3.2 m. The room contained between 14 and 16 observers seated at tables. The sound pressure level of each stimulus was measured during the investigation using the A scale of a Dawe soundlevel meter set to give the fast response. When the sound-level meter was placed at the different positions normally occupied by an observer, the average sound pressure level of the stimuli changed by about 3 db, depending on the exact location ofthe meter and the number of people in the room. However, the differences between the standard and the variables did not change. Only the average levels will be referred to from now on. The ambient noise level in the room never fell below 38 db(a) re 20 JLN/m 2 Experimental Design The 6 variables were always presented in the same order: 82, 84, 81,85,83, and 80 db(a) re 20 JLN/m 2 The standard presented before each variable had a sound pressure level of 80 db(a). The first pair of stimuli was repeated once. The remaining pairs were not repeated. Each pair was followed by ample time, about 7.5 sec, for the observers to write down their numerical estimates. The sequence of6 variables was repeated twice, making 18 variables in all. The observers were not told that the sequence was repeated. Their response sheets had spaces for the answers to the first 8 variables on the front, and for the last 10 variables on the back. This was done to conceal the structure of the sequence. Procedure Each observer was handed a stenciled answer sheet with 8 spaces at the bottom, one below the other, labeled (a) to (h) and 10 spaces on the back labeled (i) to (r). At the top, the sheet carried the following instructions: "You will hear two sounds, the second louder than the first. Ifthe loudness of the first sound is called 10, what (larger) num-

342 POULTON ber describes the loudness of the second sound? Please write down your answer in the space (a) below." Before reading the instructions, the investigator pointed to a vertical line, 15 em high, that was drawn in the middle of a blackboard, 85 cm high and 110 em wide, and said: "I have drawn a line on the board. We can call its length 10 units. Suppose I draw a longer line. Its length will be more than 10 units. You could estimate the length in units. I want you to do this, only with sounds. I will playa sound. We can call its loudness 10 units. I will then play you a louder sound. Its loudness will be more than 10 units. I want you to estimate its loudness in units. But first listen to some of the sounds. See how they differ in loudness." The investigator then played the sequence of variable sounds twice, with an additional variable of 81 db (A) inserted between the two sequences. Presenting all the sounds first helped the observers to discriminate between their loudnesses. Without this initial presentation, some observers said that all the sounds were about equally loud, because the maximum difference was only 5 db. Finally the investigator read aloud the instructions typed at the top of the answer sheet, and answered questions. Groups in which an observer asked about the upper limit were told that there was no upper limit. It was up to each observer to judge the loudness, and to write down the corresponding number. The observers were told that the first pair of sounds would be repeated. The first presentation was to let them know what they had to listen to. The second time, they had to make a judgment and write it down. The second pair of sounds was not presented until everyone had written down his or her first answer. The same applied to the third pair of sounds. After this, the tape recorder was not turned off between presentations, except after the eighth pair, when the observers had to turn over their answer sheets. The investigator sat at one of the tables with a soundlevel meter. He noted down the sound pressure levels of the stimuli. (Manuscript received December 9, 1983; revision accepted for publication June 22, 1984.)