pain" counted as "two," and so forth. The sum of point, the hour point, and so forth. These data are

Similar documents
tied at 8 a.m. No cigarettes or solid food were allowed during the following 12-hour experimental period. Two

Discover Activity. Think It Over Inferring Do you think height in humans is controlled by a single gene, as it is in peas? Explain your answer.

LOG- LINEAR ANALYSIS OF FERTILITY USING CENSUS AND SURVEY DATA WITH AN EXAMPLE

Genetic Variations. F1 Generation. Mechanisms of Genetics W W. STAAR Biology: Assessment Activities

ing the fixed-interval schedule-were observed during the interval of delay. Similarly, Ferster

Needle-Punched Non-Woven Jute Floor Coverings: Part II-Dynamic Loading Behaviour and Abrasion Resistance.

Performance Testing of a Semi-Hermetic Compressor with HFC-236ea and CFC-114 at Chiller Conditions

HAEMOLYTIC DISEASE OF THE NEWBORN

powder and liquid and several laboratory prepared mixes were used in this investigation. The materials used to prepare various

J. E. R. STADDON DUKE UNIVERSITY. The relative inability of the usual differential. to ask whether performance under DRL schedules

MATCHING LAYER DESIGN OF AN ULTRASONIC TRANS- DUCER FOR WIRELESS POWER TRANSFER SYSTEM

INTUBATION STUDIES OF THE HUMAN SMALL INTESTINE. XXIII. A METHOD

Fatigued? Or fit for work? How to tell if your workers are tired enough to make mistakes and how to prevent this happening

Changes in Cardiac Output and Stroke Volume

Limited joint mobility (LJM) of the hand in patients with diabetes mellitus: relation to chronic complications

Tellado, J: Quasi-Minimum-BER Linear Combiner Equalizers 2 The Wiener or Minimum Mean Square Error (MMSE) solution is the most often used, but it is e

Placebo effect in clinical practice. RadwanBanimustafaMD Jordan university

Acute Opioid Physical Dependence in Humans: Effect of Varying the Morphine-Naloxone Interval. I

Conditions under which amobarbital sodium influences contrast in consummatory behavior

CEREBRAL CIRCULATORY FUNCTIONS IN MAN'

Syphilis. syphilis. Sera from all stages of syphilis and the. to compare dilution titers with those obtained

Scale-free tests ofan additive model for the size-weight illusion*

IDENTIFICATION OF C-REACTIVE PROTEIN FROM GINGIVAL CREVICULAR FLUID IN SYSTEMIC DISEASE

1976, 9, NUMBER 1 (SPRING) 1976

Methodological Issues Naohito Yamaguchi,1 Yoshiyasu Tamura,2 Tomotaka Sobue,3

REINFORCEMENT OF DRINKING BY RUNNING:

CEREBRAL BLOOD FLOW AND OXYGEN CONSUMPTION IN

Perception of teachers and pupils to the acoustics of classrooms

Comfort, the Intelligent Home System. Comfort Scene Control Switch

A comparison of signaled vs. unsignaled free-operant avoidance in wild and domesticated rats

Article ID: WMC

SYNOPSIS An investigation into the noise levels in a clinical chemistry laboratory has been. 'newer' extension laboratory, are separated by a

Present-day technology has greatly facilitated the

The ability of different strains of rats to acquire a visual nonmatching-to-sample task

Sperm motility index: a new parameter for human sperm evaluation

ACTIVITY USING RATS A METHOD FOR THE EVALUATION OF ANALGESIC. subject and a variety of stimuli employed. In the examination of new compounds

there was no individual learning. On the other hand, it was

Extinction of the overshadowing CS after overshadowing in conditioned taste aversion

Asymmetric Suprathreshold Speech Recognition and the Telephone Ear Phenomenon

where WI and W2 are the weights. Letting w ==

The Role of Herd Immunity in Control of Measles

Perturbation of serum carnitine levels in human adults by chronic renal disease and dialysis therapy14

East Tennessee State University. From the SelectedWorks of John J. Wheeler

Query Length Impact on Misuse Detection in Information Retrieval Systems

to the change in the fraction of receptor binding sites occupied as a result of the stimulus (9). Thus,- the response reflects the

THE ANALGESIC EFFECT OF HALOTHANE

Outflow obstruction and bladder stones in Kelantan

Tactile perception of sequentially presented spatial patterns1

AZO DYES AND HEINZ BODIES

THE ANALGESIC PROPERTIES OF SUB-ANAESTHETIC DOSES OF ANAESTHETICS IN THE MOUSE

Selecting Research Participants. Conducting Experiments, Survey Construction and Data Collection. Practical Considerations of Research

optimal temperature of refrigeration and the effect

BACKGROUND. Paul Taylor. The National Clinical FES Centre Salisbury UK. Reciprocal Inhibition. Sensory Input Boosted by Electrical Stimulation

Letter from the Chairman

ANGELO SANTI, LORRAINE WEISE, and DWAYNE KUIPER Wilfrid Laurier University, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada

Exploratory Approach for Modeling Human Category Learning

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Cardiovascular Responses to Dynamic and Static Effort

Studies of Human Adipose Tissue

Abuse Potential of Morphine/ Dextromethorphan Combinations

SMOKING HABITS RELATED TO INJURY AND

Retardation of Sodium Exchange in Dog Erythrocytes by Physiological Concentrations of Aldosterone,

SOME ASPECTS OF CARDIOVASCULAR MORTALITY IN SOUTH AFRICA

A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Phase 3 Study of the Safety and Analgesic Efficacy of MNK-795 Controlled-Release

The effects of the antiprogesterone RU486 (Mifepristone)* on an endometrial secretory glycan: an immunocytochemical study

BLACK WOMEN'S HEALTH STUDY

Beecher as Clinical Investigator: Pain and the Placebo Effect

- d. United Nations Scientific Committee on the Effects of Atomic Radiation report to the General Assembly, with annexes

Dose-Response Relation of Diazoxide

Capillary wave gas exchange in the presence of surfactants

STAT 111 SEC 006 PRACTICE EXAM 1: SPRING 2007

Human taste response as a function of locus of stimulation on the tongue and soft palate*

Support for Acetaminophen 1000 mg Over-the-Counter Dose:

Effect of Atropine on Bradycardia and Hypotension

the Treatment of Angina Pectoris

CONCENTRATIONS OF DIETHYL ETHER IN THE BLOOD OF INTUBATED AND NON-INTUBATED PATIENTS

2007 PERFORMANCE OF WINTER BARLEY (Hordeum vulgare ssp. vulgare) AND SPRING WAXY BARLEY VARIETIES PLANTED IN THE FALL

Multiple-pattern learning by rats on an eight-arm radial maze

OF STIMULATION OF BRAINSTEM RETICULAR FORMATION ON FOOD INTAKE

Daily Warm-Up and Fundamental Exercises 2016 VIRGINIA TECH TRUMPET FESTIVAL DR. J. PEYDEN SHELTON

The goal of research on renin inhibition is to

μ i = chemical potential of species i C i = concentration of species I

Identifying Variable Length Multi-pair Palindromic Patterns with Errors in a DNA Sequence

The Effect of Varying Prophylactic Naps on Performance, Alertness and Mood throughout a 52-Hour Continuous Operation

Dissociation of implicit memory in dementia: Neurological implications

Vibrotactile masking: Temporal integration, persistence, and strengths of representations

Warm up, daily routine & technique (fundamentals)

Context effects on conditioning, extinction, and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation

"We know that four sugar pills a day beats two sugar pills a day... and that's an outrageous finding.

Epidemiology of Hepatitis B Infection in Liberian Infants

FIXED-RATIO PUNISHMENT1 N. H. AZRIN,2 W. C. HOLZ,2 AND D. F. HAKE3

Gastric Stasis and Vomiting: Behavioral Treatment

Antibodies Produced by Rabbits Immunized

DEVAKAR REVIEW PAPERS-REHABILITATION: CORRECTIONAL TECHNIQUE

Cyclic AMP Agonist Inhibition Increases at Low Levels of Histamine Release from Human Basophils1

Dietary saturated triacylglycerols suppress hepatic low density

Equine-Assisted Psychotherapy s Impact on the Self-Efficacy of Adult Female Victims of Interpersonal Violence

Serial learning, interitem associations, phrasing cues, interference, overshadowing, chunking, memory, and extinction

BAL IN THE TREATMENT OF GOLD TOXICITY

2 Critical thinking guidelines

Transcription:

FURTHER STUDIES ON THE "PHARMACOLOGY" OF PLACEBO ADMINISTRATION 1 BY LOUIS LASAGNA, VICTOR G. LATIES, AND J. LAWRENCE DOHAN (From the Department of Medicine (Division of Clinical Pharmacology), and the Department of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, Md.) (Submitted for publication July 24, 1957; accepted January 2, 1958) The effects produced hen presumably inert substances ( e.g., lactose and saline) are given to normal and diseased individuals are appreciated by many. Fe physicians ould deny the poer of a placebo to influence pain, anxiety, or other "subjective" states in certain individuals; the ability of placebos to affect "objective" phenomena (such as vomiting) or to produce "side effects" of various sorts has also been reported (1-3). This article is an attempt to describe certain lesser knon aspects of the "pharmacology" of the placebo by depicting, in some detail, the ays in hich the clinical use of inert substances may lead to effects hich are usually considered to be the exclusive property of active agents. Certain implications of the data ill be discussed. METHODS AND RESULTS Time-effect curve in single dose experiments One of the basic indices of pharmacologic activity is the time-effect relationship. When an active drug is given to patients, a maximal effect is typically achieved at a certain point in time. It is not idely appreciated that placebos can also sho this behavior. In Figure 1 are plotted some data obtained in a study of the effects of aspirin or a placebo on postpartum pain. One hundred and tenty-eight patients ere studied in an obstetrical ard during the five day period folloing delivery. Identical appearing capsules ere administered at random to any patient requesting medication for pain. All patients ere intervieed by the same technician under double blind conditions immediately prior to medication, and at stated intervals after medication, and asked ho bad their pain as. 'Supported in part by a grant (B-865-C) from the United States Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health, and in part by a grant from the Upjohn Company. Four arbitrary pain categories ere used; "very severe," "severe,"9 "moderate," and "slight," plus another category for "no pain." Data ere obtained at one-half, one, to, and three hours after medication. The average pain relief scores ere obtained as follos: Each patient as given a pain relief score for each intervie after medication. If a patient described no relief at all at a given intervie, a score of ero as recorded; if a decrease in pain as reported, a score of one, to, three or four as given, depending on the degree of relief reported. Thus, a change from "very severe" to "severe," or from "severe" to "moderate," or from "slight" to "no pain" counted as "one"; a change from "very severe" to "moderate," "severe" to "slight," or "moderate" to "no pain" counted as "to," and so forth. The sum of scores for all patients at a given intervie point as then divided by the number of patients to give a mean pain relief score for the one-half hour point, the hour point, and so forth. These data are plotted in Figure 1. The general similarity in the shape of the curves for those patients receiving aspirin and for those receiving placebo is evident, although the to treatments differ considerably in efficacy. A total pain relief score as obtained for each subject by adding the relief scores at each intervie point. A mean total pain relief score as then calculated by averaging all such scores for each drug. The mean total pain relief score for aspirin over the first three hours after medication as 5.91 ith a standard error of 0.35; for placebo, 3.45 plus or minus 0.44. This difference is significant at the 0.01 level. In analying the placebo curve to see hether there as indeed changing efficacy ith time, a to-tailed sign-test (4) as applied to the 23 patients ho shoed different degrees of relief at the one-half hour and one hour points. Of these, 20 shoed a greater degree of relief at the one hour point than 533

534 LOUIS LASAGNA, VICTOR G. LATIES, AND J. LAWRENCE DOHAN 0 2.0. U) --4... UA. -0 0, AV X '*ASPIRIN & 1.0 / (N6-7) X / PLACEBO / (Nu 61) 0 0.5 1 2 3 HOURS AFTER MEDICATION FIG. 1. MEAN PAIN RELIEF SCORES AFTER PLACEBO OR ASPIRIN IN PATIENTS SUFFERING FROM POSTPARTUM PAIN at the half-hour intervie, a difference significant at the 0.01 level. Although the mean score for placebo relief is somehat loer at three hours than at one hour, this difference is not statistically significant. "Cumulative" effects of placebos A second basic type of pharmacologic study is the delineation of the effect of repeated doses of a drug. This is often considered to be a reflection of increasing concentration of drug in the blood or body. It is not generally appreciated that placebos can also sho a "build-up" in effect, and that there may be a "carry-over" after cessation of placebo therapy. Presented in Figures 2 and 3 are the data from to separate experiments. In one experiment, 34 patients suffering from tuberculosis, and on antituberculosis drugs, ere given a yello tablet daily for seven days, having been told that the tablets ould increase their "appetite" and improve their "pep and energy." 2 They ere asked to indicate their status each day on sheets, as shon in Table I. For four days after medication as stopped, additional observations ere made. The results, shon in Figure 2, indicate that the placebo therapy as associated ith gradual rises in subjective reports of both appetite and pep. The levels achieved ere maintained after cessation of 21t should be emphasied that the responses dealt ith here are verbal reports. The extent, e.g., to hich food intake ould have been correlated ith "appetite" is an interesting but different question. placebos. In order to provide a baseline against hich the reliability of the changes could be tested, the nine ratings before placebos ere begun ere averaged. A comparison of the mean of the first three ratings made under placebo ith this baseline shoed a rise in appetite scores from 2.51 to 2.93, hich as statistically significant at the 0.01 level [sign-test, to-tailed (4)]. For pep the change as from 2.72 to 2.91. This change as not statistically significant. For both variables the curves continued to rise, hoever, as *the placebo therapy continued. Appetite rose from 2.93 to 3.45 and pep from 2.91 to 3.34, if the means of the first three days' ratings on placebo are compared ith those of the last three of the placebo therapy. Both of these changes are significant at the 0.01 level. The curves did not drop folloing the end of placebo therapy, the rating four days later being 3.53 for appetite, 3.50 for pep. Figure 3 illustrates similar data on a population of 31 hospitalied patients suffering from various chronic illnesses. Here there as a short pre-placebo period of three days, a placebo period of six days, and a post-placebo period of to eeks. (There as, hoever, a lapse of five days during the post-placebo period hen no intervie data ere obtained.) Once again there appears to be a cumulative beneficial effect over the period of placebo administration, ith a partial return to pre-placebo status in the case of "pep" to eeks after stopping placebos. The mean ratings for the pre-placebo period ere 3.05 for 4.00 - PEP 3..'...* APPETITE 3.50 l e PLACEBOS 2.00. _ 1LW..... 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 DAY S FIG. 2. PEP AND APPETITE RATINGS IN 34 TUBERCULOUS PATIENTS TREATED WITH PLACEBO

PHARMACOLOGY OF PLACEBOS 535 p 4.00r 3.50-3.00 2.50 I_ * I I 2 p..-**...*.v **...Ẇ..0ce V PLACEBOS l - l PEP *...* APPETITE ~1 I] I I I I I I 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 21 23 DAY S FIG. 3. PEP AND APPETITE RATINGS IN 31 PATIENTS HOSPITALIZED FOR VARIOUS CHRONIC DISEASES AND TREATED WITH PLACEBO appetite and 2.78 for pep. Both rose to a mean of 3.22 for the first three days on placebo. Sign tests shoed that only the change for pep as significant (p < 0.01). The ratings rose further to means of 3.63 and 3.39 for appetite and pep, respectively, for the last days of placebo therapy. The increase for appetite is statistically significant at the 0.01 level. As in the first study, the ratings remained at substantially the same levels after placebos ere ithdran. The mean ratings over the next six days ere 3.64 for appetite and 3.24 for pep. To eeks after the stopping of placebos, pep had shon a decrease to a mean of 3.03 for the last three ratings available. This drop differs significantly from the 3.39 of the last three days on placebo. The ratings for appetite did not sho such a decrease, ith a mean for the last three days of 3.51. Effectiveness of placebos in relation to severity of disease Another general characteristic of drugs is the inverse relationship of their efficacy to the severity of a given complaint. The same relationship for placebos has been apparent in some of our on data. In a study on the efficacy of morphine and injected saline on postoperative pain (2), an inverse relationship existed beteen the number of doses of medication required postoperatively and the efficacy of morphine or placebo (see Table II). TABLE I Questionnaire A) Ho is your appetite today? (Check one of the folloing) 1. No appetite at all 2. Poor appetite 3. Fair appetite 4. Good appetite 5. Very good appetite B) Ho is your pep and energy today? (Check one of the folloing) 1. No pep or energy at all 2. Very little pep or energy 3. Fair pep and energy 4. Good pep and energy 5. Very good pep and energy If the average severity of pain of a group of patients can be gauged by the ability of a given dose of morphine to relieve their pain, it ould appear that the patients requiring the largest number of doses of medication had more severe pain and ere less relieved by morphine and placebo. In any case, there is an interesting parallelism beteen the performance of morphine and the performance of placebo. Figure 4 also bears on this point. These data ere collected in the previously described postpartum study, and are a breakdon of the data on complete relief of pain observed after placebo into those observations collected on patients describing their pain initially as "very severe" or "severe as contrasted ith those obtained on patients ith pain initially described as "slight" or "moderate." The percentage of patients reporting complete relief of pain at any given time after medication is higher in the case of the "slightmoderate" group than in the "very severe-severe" TABLE H Pain relief ith morphine or placebo in patients suffering from postoperative pain * Group No. of Pts. Morphine Placebo I (2 doses/pt.) I (4 doses/pt.) m (6 doses/ pt) 12 92% 58% 21 75% 40% 15 61% 40% 3 (8or more doses/pt.) 15 58% 15% *The patients are stratified according to number of doses of medication required during entire postoperative period.

536 U. 0 50 401 301 LOUIS LASAGNA, VICTOR G. LATIES, AND J. LAWRENCE DOHAN POST-PARTUM PAIN REUEF WITH PLACEBO SLIGHT-MODERATE PAIN -- SEVERE-VERY SEVERE PAIN 00, OF.- -- -.-, -O.- O' Q5 i 1 HOURS AFTER MEDICATION FIG. 4. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN REPORTED SEVERITY OF PAIN AND ANALGESIC EFFICACY OF PLACEBO group. The percentage of patients reporting complete relief of pain at some time during the three hour interval post-placebo as 21 per cent in the case of the "very severe-severe" group (n = 19), and 57 per cent in the case of the "slight-moderate" group (n = 42). This difference is significant at the 0.01 level. These data seem most easily interpreted as a less effective handling of the greater therapeutic challenge by placebo. The aspirin data from this study have been similarly analyed and also indicate a similar lessened efficacy in patients ith greater pain. DISCUSSION ".. ftft. I-ft 11-0 There are certain implications in these data. First of all, uncontrolled studies that claim a ne or old drug to have shon unequivocal therapeutic benefit, merely because of "peak effects" or "cumulative effects" or persistent benefit after cessation of treatment, must be interpreted ith considerable caution. A placebo effect is obviously not an "all or none" phenomenon, and such effects are not necessarily turned off or on, like an electric light bulb. Secondly, the time-effect relationships of placebo phenomena may be extremely important in deciding upon the times hen data are to be collected in controlled trials. It is conceivable, for example, that in a certain situation the effects of suggestion are rapidly obtained, but also ear off fairly rapidly. In another situation the effects may require longer to ear off (5). A failure to collect data at points other than the placebo "peak" may give a misleading notion about the efficacy of an active drug being compared against placebo. Thirdly, it appears likely [as suggested in an earlier paper (2)] that the use of patients presenting severe therapeutic challenges may, at least on occasion, render placebo controls less necessary or less important. Several questions are raised by the data presented. Ho can one explain the "cumulative effects" experienced by the patients receiving placebos? At times, patients intervieed on the first fe days on placebos volunteered the information that, "You kno, it takes a hile for these drugs to take hold, Doc." The desire not to "disappoint" a doctor ho is presumably eager for the patient to sho improvement may increase progressively ith the passage of time, as may the degree of reinforcement for a patient ho feels that a "correct" anser (i.e., improvement) ill result in the doctor taking a greater interest in him. In regard to the "carry-over" described, e can only speculate. One possibility is that patients ho are improved on placebos may feel (at least temporarily) that the placebo has achieved a "cure" and that further medication is not needed. It must be difficult for a patient to believe that a doctor ho had observed significant benefit in a patient from the administration of a small pill ould stop or ithhold such a pill if a continued need existed. One might also interpret the placebo reaction in terms of simple learning theory. Typically, in learning experiments, the abrupt ithdraal of the cue for response (here the administration of pills) is not folloed by immediate cessation of response. Instead, one usually sees a gradual and irregular decline. It ould be of great interest to see studies of the effectiveness of prolonged placebo therapy in various clinical situations. It is evident that these studies give no proof that the results described ere caused by the placebos, and ould not have occurred in the absence of their administration. In regard to the pertinence of the data in recommending caution in uncontrolled experiments, such demonstration is not necessary, of course. Nevertheless, it seems unlikely that such data as presented in Figures 2 and 3, ith similar curves in different populations, are really due to chance improvement unrelated

PHARMACOLOGY OF PLACEBOS to the giving of placebos. These groups ere chosen because of the relative stability of their surroundings and their illness at the time of our studies. These data on severity of complaint and placebo efficacy are somehat at variance ith those reported by Beecher (6), ho found that patients studied early in the postoperative period (here pain is presumably more severe than later) are handled almost as ell by placebo as by morphine, hereas later in the postoperative course morphine performs much better than placebo. The results ere interpreted as shoing that placebos ork most effectively hen the stress is greater. Hoever, it is of interest that Keats has found (in similar patients studied by a similar technique) placebo and morphine yielding pain relief in 27 per cent and 60 per cent, respectively, of patients early in the postoperative course, and 55 per cent and 80 per cent, respectively, late in the postoperative course (7). Keats' findings are thus in agreement ith those in the present report. SUMMARY Data are presented to indicate that subjective responses to placebos can mimic certain characteristics of "active" drugs, such as "peak effects," "cumulative effects," "carry-over effects," and varying efficacy depending on the severity of the complaint being treated. Certain implications of these facts are discussed. REFERENCES 537 1. Wolf, S. Effects of suggestion and conditioning on the action of chemical agents in human subjectsthe pharmacology of placebos. J. clin. Invest. 1950, 29, 100. 2. Lasagna, L., Mosteller, F., von Felsinger, J. M., and Beecher, H. K. A study of the placebo response. Amer. J. Med. 1954, 16, 770. 3. Wolf, S., and Pinsky, R. H. Effects of placebo administration and occurrence of toxic reactions. J. Amer. med. Ass. 1954, 155, 339. 4. Dixon, W. J., and Massey, F. J. Introduction to Statistical Analysis. Ne York, McGra-Hill, 1951. 5. Gruber, C. M., Jr., Miller, C. L., Finneran, J., and Chernish, S. M. The effectiveness of d-propoxyphene hydrochloride and codeine phosphate as determined by to methods of clinical testing for relief of chronic pain. J. Pharmacol. exp. Ther. 1956, 118, 280. 6. Beecher, H. K. Evidence for increased effectiveness of placebos ith increased stress. Amer. J. Physiol. 1956, 187, 163. 7. Keats, A. S. Personal communication.