PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Ball State University

Similar documents
PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Title: Intention-to-treat and transparency of related practices in randomized, controlled trials of anti-infectives

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS

Jonathan Williman University of Otago, Christchurch New Zealand 06-Nov-2013

VARIED THRUSH MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVIEWS (ROUND 2) Editor Decision Letter

S Imputation of Categorical Missing Data: A comparison of Multivariate Normal and. Multinomial Methods. Holmes Finch.

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Statistical reports Regression, 2010

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Title: Home Exposure to Arabian Incense (Bakhour) and Asthma Symptoms in Children: A Community Survey in Two Regions in Oman

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Author's response to reviews

Title:Video-confidence: a qualitative exploration of videoconferencing for psychiatric emergencies

Title: Identifying work ability promoting factors for home care aides and assistant nurses

Author's response to reviews

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Amare Nigatu

Title:Continuity of GP care is associated with lower use of complementary and alternative medical providers A population-based cross-sectional survey

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Veronika Williams University of Oxford, UK 07-Dec-2015

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Randi Selmer Senior Researcher Norwegian Institute of Public Health Norway

Author s response to reviews

Title: Socioeconomic conditions and number of pain sites in women

MEMO TO: Author FROM: Lauren Montemurri DATE: March 28, 2011 RE: CAM utilization study edits

Title: Prevalence of sexual, physical and emotional abuse in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study

DON M. PALLAIS, CPA 14 Dahlgren Road Richmond, Virginia Telephone: (804) Fax: (804)

Author's response to reviews

MJ - Decision on Manuscript ID BMJ

Publishing Your Study: Tips for Young Investigators. Learning Objectives 7/9/2013. Eric B. Bass, MD, MPH

# BMJ entitled " Complete the antibiotic course to avoid resistance ; non-evidence-based dogma which has run its course?

Title:Bounding the Per-Protocol Effect in Randomized Trials: An Application to Colorectal Cancer Screening

Author s response to reviews

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS. Zou, Yuming; Li, Quan; Xu, Weidong VERSION 1 - REVIEW

You must answer question 1.

Editorial. From the Editors: Perspectives on Turnaround Time

Author's response to reviews

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS

Title: Exploring approaches to patient safety: The case of spinal manipulation therapy

Editorial Note: this manuscript has been previously reviewed at another journal that is not operating a transparent peer review scheme.

DAZED AND CONFUSED: THE CHARACTERISTICS AND BEHAVIOROF TITLE CONFUSED READERS

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS

Tips on Successful Writing and Getting Published Rita F. Redberg, MD, MSc, FACC, FAHA Professor of Medicine Editor, JAMA Internal Medicine

Title: Ego Defense Mechanisms in Pakistani Medical Students: A cross sectional analysis

7 Statistical Issues that Researchers Shouldn t Worry (So Much) About

Chapter 5 Analyzing Quantitative Research Literature

Author's response to reviews

Title:Problematic computer gaming, console-gaming, and internet use among adolescents: new measurement tool and association with time use

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS

Author's response to reviews

Further Properties of the Priority Rule

Evaluators Perspectives on Research on Evaluation

Title:The self-reported health of U.S. flight attendants compared to the general population

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Adrian Barnett Queensland University of Technology, Australia 10-Oct-2014

THE DEATH OF CANCER BY HAROLD W MANNER, STEVEN J. DISANTI, THOMAS L. MICHALSEN

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

DRAFT (Final) Concept Paper On choosing appropriate estimands and defining sensitivity analyses in confirmatory clinical trials

Author's response to reviews

Canada would provide a proposed draft definition for consideration by the next session based on these comments.

Please revise your paper to respond to all of the comments by the reviewers. Their reports are available at the end of this letter, below.

Thank you for considering our manuscript. We appreciate the reviewers comments and have incorporated much of their feedback into the manuscript.

MANAGING SOCIAL ANXIETY: A COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL THERAPY APPROACH (TREATMENTS THAT WORK) BY DEBRA A. HOPE, RICHARD G. HEIMBERG, CYNTHIA L.

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. I have no competing interests 17-Feb-2013

Cohesive Writing Module: Introduction

Title: Attitudes and beliefs of the French public about schizophrenia and major depression. Results from a vignette-based population survey

논문투고및투고후소통하기 : 영문교정작업, 실제논문투고하기, revision 답변달기, query form 작성하기

Term Paper Step-by-Step

Patients To Learn From: On the Need for Systematic Integration of Research and Care in Academic Health Care

Title:Decisions on statin therapy by patients' opinions about survival gains: Cross sectional survey of general practitioners.

FORUM: QUALITATIVE SOCIAL RESEARCH SOZIALFORSCHUNG

Conflict of interest in randomised controlled surgical trials: Systematic review, qualitative and quantitative analysis

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

BMJ - Decision on Manuscript ID BMJ

Endogeneity is a fancy word for a simple problem. So fancy, in fact, that the Microsoft Word spell-checker does not recognize it.

Title: Selection effects may account for better outcomes of the German Disease Management Program for type 2 diabetes

Methodology for Non-Randomized Clinical Trials: Propensity Score Analysis Dan Conroy, Ph.D., inventiv Health, Burlington, MA

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

Title: The size of the population potentially in need of palliative care in Germany - An estimation based on death registration data

Title: Dengue Score: a proposed diagnostic predictor of pleural effusion and/or ascites in adult with dengue infection

Reliability, validity, and all that jazz

Uses and misuses of the STROBE statement: bibliographic study

ABNORMAL PSYCHOLOGY: AN INTEGRATIVE APPROACH, 7TH EDITION BY DAVID H. BARLOW, V. MARK DURAND

Blast Searcher Formative Evaluation. March 02, Adam Klinger and Josh Gutwill

TACKLING WITH REVIEWER S COMMENTS:

These comments are an attempt to summarise the discussions at the manuscript meeting. They are not an exact transcript.

What Constitutes a Good Contribution to the Literature (Body of Knowledge)?

Title: A Central Storage Facility to Reduce Pesticide Suicides- A Feasibility Study from India

Public Opinion Survey on Tobacco Use in Outdoor Dining Areas Survey Specifications and Training Guide

Stephen Madigan PhD madigan.ca Vancouver School for Narrative Therapy

Developing language writing convincingly (Example from undergraduate Cultural Studies)

Doing High Quality Field Research. Kim Elsbach University of California, Davis

Validity and reliability of measurements

SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING

Dear Dr. Villanueva,

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Title: Health Care Professionals' Attitudes Regarding Palliative Care for Patients with Chronic Heart Failure: An Interview Study

Transcription:

PEER REVIEW HISTORY BMJ Open publishes all reviews undertaken for accepted manuscripts. Reviewers are asked to complete a checklist review form (see an example) and are provided with free text boxes to elaborate on their assessment. These free text comments are reproduced below. Some articles will have been accepted based in part or entirely on reviews undertaken for other BMJ Group journals. These will be reproduced where possible. TITLE (PROVISIONAL) AUTHORS REVIEWER REVIEW RETURNED THE STUDY ARTICLE DETAILS When Data Are Not Missing at Random: Implications for Measuring Health Conditions Frankel, Martin ; Battaglia, Michael; Balluz, Lena; Strine, Tara VERSION 1 - REVIEW Holmes Finch Ball State University 16-Mar-2012 I found the basic premise of the study to be clear, and the authors do a nice job of explaining what they are interested in and why. However, the manuscript itself reads much more like a draft of a research report than a true peer reviewed manuscript ready for publication. For example, there is no literature review at all, despite the fact that the missing data literature is quite rich. I would encourage the authors to do a literature search and become familiar with the work that has been done in this area, much of which has already addressed the primary issue of interest in the study, namely comparing the impact of imputation versus listwise deletion on data analysis. A good place to start is with Schafer and Graham's 2002 Psychological Methods article. This will provide an introduction to the missing data literature and also provide further references. In addition to providing a review of the literature, the authors need to explain what their work adds to that literature. Much has already been done to show that listwise deletion leads to bias, so another study demonstrating that again is probably not too informative. However, issues around sampling weights and missing data are much more interesting, I believe, and have not been so fully studied. However, no mention is really made of this fact. The authors need to place their work in the broader framework of the missing data field. Finally, the imputation methods are not well described and may not be appropriate for the categorical data included in this study. It would be very helpful if the authors could explain what type of multiple imputation they are using. Most methods assume normally distributed data, which is not the case here. Also, for categorical variables like items, how was rounding carried out? There is a great deal of literature on the impact of using these normal based methods with ordinal data and the impact of rounding that should really be discussed in more detail here. RESULTS & CONCLUSIONS The authors present their results in clear language, but again the paper reads much more like a research report for in house use than a formal manuscript ready for peer review. I would encourage the

authors to go back and "formalize" their writing style and the organization of the paper. Also, it would be very helpful for them to tie their results back to the fairly lengthy literature on missing data with surveys and tests that already exists. Finally, what is unique BMJ Open: first published as 10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000696 on 12 July 2012. Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on 23 November 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.

GENERAL COMMENTS REVIEWER REVIEW RETURNED THE STUDY GENERAL COMMENTS about this work, in light of the prior literature. I don't believe that they make a strong case for this. I think that your work holds promise and can contribute to the literature, but I believe three important things must be done first: 1) The writing style must read more like a formal journal manuscript than an in house report. 2) The literature on missing data, particularly with regard to categorical data must be included and should inform both the methodology used and the discussion of the results. 3) The unique niche that this paper fills must be explained clearly. I believe it is with regard to missing data and the sampling weights issue, but this is only briefly touched on in the current version. Much more needs to be made of this. David C. Howell Professor Emeritus University of Vermont Burlington, VT USA I have no competing interests 03-Mar-2012 There were no supplemental documents. I think that this is a very good paper with results that should be published. The authors seriously address the problem of data that are missing not at random (MNAR) and illustrate an approach that reduces bias. Perhaps most importantly they illustrate that traditional approaches, using only non-missing data or using only demographic variables for imputation lead to greater bias. The fact that listwise deletion leads to bias is certainly not new, but it is meaningful that the common demographic-only approach also fails to adequately correct bias caused by MNAR. The authors claim that their data are MNAR without discussion. I agree that the results would support that statement, but I would like to see discussion up front that addresses why they assume from the beginning that the data are missing not at random. This does not need to be long, but some discussion would be appropriate. Rubin's missing data theory, which encompasses MCAR, MAR, and MNAR, is not always well understood by readers. About half of the paper is devoted to tables. If this were designed for a hard print journal, I would question whether some of those tables should not be significantly abbreviated to show illustrative results. Perhaps in an electronic form that inclusion of more tabular material is not a problem. I leave that question to the editor. Perhaps the most convincing part of the paper is the validation study. That might benefit from further discussion and/or emphasis. Given that the inclusion of the three mental health measures was so successful, it may seem picky to question the scaling procedure. However I will do it anyway. The assumption seems to be that rescaling the 30 day scale to a 10 point scale is roughly a linear transformation in terms of the underlying variable. However the

REVIEWER REVIEW RETURNED THE STUDY GENERAL COMMENTS difference between scaled scores of 1 and 3 strikes me as minor compared to a difference between scaled scores of 8 and 10. As I say, it seems to have worked, but it still leaves me slightly uneasy. The authors twice try to cover themselves by suggesting that what works with anxiety and depression might not work with other variables. I suppose that is true, but I think that they make too much of that idea. I suspect that this approach would apply to a wide range of variables. There are a number of minor typographical errors (e.g. "DEP 0" instead of "DEP10" and "statically" instead of "statistically"), but I assume that these will be caught and corrected in the final version. Kristen Olson Assistant Professor of Sociology & Survey Research and Methodology University of Nebraska-Lincoln, USA 21-Feb-2012 Details of the imputation procedure are confusing. Did the authors simply impute a probability from the logistic regression model or did they use the probabilities to assign a value of 0/1 on depression? In the validation, it is clear that they assigned 0/1 values, but it is not clear in the main imputation or in the multiple imputation. If only predicted probabilities, I wonder what the usefulness of this method is for users of these data who will be expecting 0/1 values on a dichotomous outcome. It seems like a stochastic assignment of 0/1, as done in the validation section, would dramatically improve the applicability of these findings beyond this paper. One important question that is not addressed in this manuscript but would be a useful addition is the imputation of responses to each of the individual questions that are used in the depression index versus the overall index itself. The index (DEP10) is calculated only on complete cases in the data set. The authors do not say how many persons are missing on all of the questions used in creating DEP10 versus have responses to all but one or two. That is, the authors should include the missing data pattern for the 8 questions used to compute DEP10. In essence, they are throwing away potentially important information about the individual s depression levels that have already been collected. Some empirical evaluation of this would significantly strengthen the manuscript. General comments This manuscript examines two different imputation models for one health outcome a measure of major depression. It does not invent any new methods. Rather, it examines the inclusion of predictors of the outcome in an imputation model. This is important to do, but may be overlooked in imputation routines that do not use an automatic selection procedure for highly correlated predictors. Details of the imputation procedure are confusing. Did the authors simply impute a probability from the logistic regression model or did they use the probabilities to assign a value of 0/1 on depression? In the validation, it is clear that they assigned 0/1 values, but it is not clear in the main imputation or in the multiple imputation. If only

predicted probabilities, I wonder what the usefulness of this method is for users of these data who will be expecting 0/1 values on a dichotomous outcome. It seems like a stochastic assignment of 0/1, as done in the validation section, would dramatically improve the applicability of these findings beyond this paper. One important question that is not addressed in this manuscript but would be a useful addition is the imputation of responses to each of the individual questions that are used in the depression index versus the overall index itself. The index (DEP10) is calculated only on complete cases in the data set. The authors do not say how many persons are missing on all of the questions used in creating DEP10 versus have responses to all but one or two. That is, the authors should include the missing data pattern for the 8 questions used to compute DEP10. In essence, they are throwing away potentially important information about the individual s depression levels that have already been collected. Some empirical evaluation of this would significantly strengthen the manuscript. Specific comments p. 2, line 4: generally accepted practice health surveys insert in or for between practice and health p. 2, line 10: delete non-cooperation p. 2, line 13: It is odd that the imputation literature has two citations, both of which are quite old. p. 2, line 22: various estimates of health behavior only one health behavior is examined in this article that of depression. p. 2, line 23: standard socio-demographic based imputation The authors should provide a citation or two for surveys that use only demographic variables for imputation. I am familiar with a variety of surveys that use demographics for weighting adjustments, but less familiar with those that use only demographics in imputation models. p. 2, line 36: consist should be consists p. 2, line 39: insert commas between each in the list of socio-demographic factors p. 3, line 45: 88% sensitivity and specificity for major depression is this calculated from the BRFSS data (I don t remember diagnostic interviews being in the BRFSS) or known from other sources? If the latter, it needs a citation. p. 4, line 25: currently pregnant This is highly unusual as a socio- demographic predictor. This is a health characteristic rather than a demographic characteristic. p. 4, lines 56-57/Table 2: I am confused about what is displayed in Table 2. In the preceding paragraph, the authors describe the outcomes as being ordinal with multiple categories, but the table shows only one percentage. Do the mental health and activity limitation lines show only those whose mental health was not good for all of the past 30 days and who had activity limitation on all of the past 30 days? This could be made much clearer. Additionally, I believe that the denominator in each is the number of cases in the column (e.g., the number who are not missing and negative to DEP10), but this took quite a while to identify. In general, the display of this table could be clarified tremendously. p. 5, lines 25-32: How were the statistical tests conducted? The imputed and not imputed estimates are not independent, but there is no mention of what was done to account for this lack of independence. p. 5, lines 44-46: How was the stochastic rounding conducted? Was a random number generated from a uniform distribution and

compared to the predicted probability, or was some other method employed? Please provide more details. p. 5, lines 50-58: This paragraph repeats the findings described in paragraph p. 2, lines 25-32. Consider combining these paragraphs for a more parsimonious discussion. In particular, the authors should move the discussion of the findings from lines 25-32 down to the results section. p. 6: The text has no direct reference to Table 4. It should be added. Also, the headings are incredibly difficult to read. p. 6, lines 22-24: The results do not show that the data missing at random assumptions do not hold. It simply showed that the MAR assumption was made more appropriate with the inclusion of predictors of the Y variable of interest (similar to the arguments Rod Little has advanced for using predictive mean adjustment methods). VERSION 1 AUTHOR RESPONSE Reviewer: Kristen Olson Assistant Professor of Sociology & Survey Research and Methodology University of Nebraska-Lincoln, USA Details of the imputation procedure are confusing. Did the authors simply impute a probability from the logistic regression model or did they use the probabilities to assign a value of 0/1 on depression? In the validation, it is clear that they assigned 0/1 values, but it is not clear in the main imputation or in the multiple imputation. If only predicted probabilities, I wonder what the usefulness of this method is for users of these data who will be expecting 0/1 values on a dichotomous outcome. It seems like a stochastic assignment of 0/1, as done in the validation section, would dramatically improve the applicability of these findings beyond this paper. We agree with the reviewer that many of the details presented in the original submission were confusing. We have attempted to clarify our method including the rationale for each step. One important question that is not addressed in this manuscript but would be a useful addition is the imputation of responses to each of the individual questions that are used in the depression index versus the overall index itself. The index (DEP10) is calculated only on complete cases in the data set. The authors do not say how many persons are missing on all of the questions used in creating DEP10 versus have responses to all but one or two. That is, the authors should include the missing data pattern for the 8 questions used to compute DEP10. In essence, they are throwing away potentially important information about the individual s depression levels that have already been collected. Some empirical evaluation of this would significantly strengthen the manuscript.

We have clearly indicated that the purpose of the imputation was the development of state level estimates of depression (severe depression). For this reason, we chose to focus on a more direct method. A case can be made for the imputation of each of the items in the scale, and efficacy of this approach might be the subject of further research. General comments This manuscript examines two different imputation models for one health outcome a measure of major depression. It does not invent any new methods. Rather, it examines the inclusion of predictors of the outcome in an imputation model. This is important to do, but may be overlooked in imputation routines that do not use an automatic selection procedure for highly correlated predictors. Details of the imputation procedure are confusing. Did the authors simply impute a probability from the logistic regression model or did they use the probabilities to assign a value of 0/1 on depression? In the validation, it is clear that they assigned 0/1 values, but it is not clear in the main imputation or in the multiple imputation. If only predicted probabilities, I wonder what the usefulness of this method is for users of these data who will be expecting 0/1 values on a dichotomous outcome. It seems like a stochastic assignment of 0/1, as done in the validation section, would dramatically improve the applicability of these findings beyond this paper. We hope that our revisions have clarified our methods. One important question that is not addressed in this manuscript but would be a useful addition is the imputation of responses to each of the individual questions that are used in the depression index versus the overall index itself. The index (DEP10) is calculated only on complete cases in the data set. The authors do not say how many persons are missing on all of the questions used in creating DEP10 versus have responses to all but one or two. That is, the authors should include the missing data pattern for the 8 questions used to compute DEP10. In essence, they are throwing away potentially important information about the individual s depression levels that have already been collected. Some empirical evaluation of this would significantly strengthen the manuscript. See our response above. Specific comments p. 2, line 4: generally accepted practice health surveys insert in or for between practice and health p. 2, line 10: delete noncooperation p. 2, line 13: It is odd that the imputation literature has two citations, both of which are quite old.

p. 2, line 22: various estimates of health behavior only one health behavior is examined in this article that of depression. p. 2, line 23: standard socio-demographic based imputation The authors should provide a citation or two for surveys that use only demographic variables for imputation. I am familiar with a variety of surveys that use demographics for weighting adjustments, but less familiar with those that use only demographics in imputation models. p. 2, line 36: consist should be consists p. 2, line 39: insert commas between each in the list of sociodemographic factors p. 3, line 45: 88% sensitivity and specificity for major depression is this calculated from the BRFSS data (I don t remember diagnostic interviews being in the BRFSS) or known from other sources? If the latter, it needs a citation. p. 4, line 25: currently pregnant This is highly unusual as a sociodemographic predictor. This is a health characteristic rather than a demographic characteristic. p. 4, lines 56-57/Table 2: I am confused about what is displayed in Table 2. In the preceding paragraph, the authors describe the outcomes as being ordinal with multiple categories, but the table shows only one percentage. Do the mental health and activity limitation lines show only those whose mental health was not good for all of the past 30 days and who had activity limitation on all of the past 30 days? This could be made much clearer. Additionally, I believe that the denominator in each is the number of cases in the column (e.g., the number who are not missing and negative to DEP10), but this took quite a while to identify. In general, the display of this table could be clarified tremendously. p. 5, lines 25-32: How were the statistical tests conducted? The imputed and not imputed estimates are not independent, but there is no mention of what was done to account for this lack of independence. p. 5, lines 44-46: How was the stochastic rounding conducted? Was a random number generated from a uniform distribution and compared to the predicted probability, or was some other method employed? Please provide more details. p. 5, lines 50-58: This paragraph repeats the findings described in paragraph p. 2, lines 25-32. Consider combining these paragraphs for a more parsimonious discussion. In particular, the authors should move the discussion of the findings from lines 25-32 down to the results section. p. 6: The text has no direct reference to Table 4. It should be added. Also, the headings are incredibly difficult to read. p. 6, lines 22-24: The results do not show that the data missing at random assumptions do not hold. It simply showed that the MAR assumption was made more appropriate with the inclusion of predictors of the Y variable of interest (similar to the arguments Rod Little has advanced for using predictive mean adjustment methods). We appreciate these very specific comments and have tried to address each in our revisions

Reviewer: David C. Howell Professor Emeritus University of Vermont Burlington, VT USA I have no competing interests I think that this is a very good paper with results that should be published. The authors seriously address the problem of data that are missing not at random (MNAR) and illustrate an approach that reduces bias. Perhaps most importantly they illustrate that traditional approaches, using only non-missing data or using only demographic variables for imputation lead to greater bias. The fact that listwise deletion leads to bias is certainly not new, but it is meaningful that the common demographic-only approach also fails to adequately correct bias caused by MNAR. The authors claim that their data are MNAR without discussion. I agree that the results would support that statement, but I would like to see discussion up front that addresses why they assume from the beginning that the data are missing not at random. This does not need to be long, but some discussion would be appropriate. Rubin's missing data theory, which encompasses MCAR, MAR, and MNAR, is not always well understood by readers. At the suggestion of this reviewer we have tried to expand the discussion of Rubin s theory and the context for this paper About half of the paper is devoted to tables. If this were designed for a hard print journal, I would question whether some of those tables should not be significantly abbreviated to show illustrative results. Perhaps in an electronic form that inclusion of more tabular material is not a problem. I leave that question to the editor. We have removed some of the more extensive tables. Perhaps the most convincing part of the paper is the validation study. That might benefit from further discussion and/or emphasis. We have tried to clarify and expand our discussion and emphasis Given that the inclusion of the three mental health measures was so successful, it may seem picky to question the scaling procedure. However I will do it anyway. The assumption seems to be that rescaling the 30 day scale to a 10 point scale is roughly a linear transformation in terms of the underlying variable. However the difference between scaled scores of 1 and 3 strikes me as minor compared to a difference between scaled scores of 8 and 10. As I say, it seems to have worked, but it still leaves me slightly uneasy.

The authors twice try to cover themselves by suggesting that what works with anxiety and depression might not work with other variables. I suppose that is true, but I think that they make too much of that idea. I suspect that this approach would apply to a wide range of variables. There are a number of minor typographical errors (e.g. "DEP 0" instead of "DEP10" and "statically" instead of "statistically"), but I assume that these will be caught and corrected in the final version. Reviewer: Holmes Finch Ball State University I found the basic premise of the study to be clear, and the authors do a nice job of explaining what they are interested in and why. However, the manuscript itself reads much more like a draft of a research report than a true peer reviewed manuscript ready for publication. For example, there is no literature review at all, despite the fact that the missing data literature is quite rich. I would encourage the authors to do a literature search and become familiar with the work that has been done in this area, much of which has already addressed the primary issue of interest in the study, namely comparing the impact of imputation versus listwise deletion on data analysis. A good place to start is with Schafer and Graham's 2002 Psychological Methods article. This will provide an introduction to the missing data literature and also provide further references. In addition to providing a review of the literature, the authors need to explain what their work adds to that literature. Much has already been done to show that listwise deletion leads to bias, so another study demonstrating that again is probably not too informative. However, issues around sampling weights and missing data are much more interesting, I believe, and have not been so fully studied. However, no mention is really made of this fact. The authors need to place their work in the broader framework of the missing data field. Finally, the imputation methods are not well described and may not be appropriate for the categorical data included in this study. It would be very helpful if the authors could explain what type of multiple imputation they are using. Most methods assume normally distributed data, which is not the case here. Also, for categorical variables like items, how was rounding carried out? There is a great deal of literature on the impact of using these normal based methods with ordinal data and the impact of rounding that should really be discussed in more detail here. The authors present their results in clear language, but again the paper reads much more like a research report for in house use than

a formal manuscript ready for peer review. I would encourage the authors to go back and "formalize" their writing style and the organization of the paper. Also, it would be very helpful for them to tie their results back to the fairly lengthy literature on missing data with surveys and tests that already exists. Finally, what is unique about this work, in light of the prior literature. I don't believe that they make a strong case for this. I think that your work holds promise and can contribute to the literature, but I believe three important things must be done first: 1) The writing style must read more like a formal journal manuscript than an in house report. 2) The literature on missing data, particularly with regard to categorical data must be included and should inform both the methodology used and the discussion of the results. 3) The unique niche that this paper fills must be explained clearly. I believe it is with regard to missing data and the sampling weights issue, but this is only briefly touched on in the current version. Much more needs to be made of this. We have explained that our basic purpose was the prediction of severe depression based on a scale that was developed elsewhere. We hope that our revisions have responded to this reviewer s very appropriate comments BMJ Open: first published as 10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000696 on 12 July 2012. Downloaded from http://bmjopen.bmj.com/ on 23 November 2018 by guest. Protected by copyright.