Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals

Similar documents
Excessive Daytime Sleepiness Associated with Insufficient Sleep

Patterns of Sleepiness in Various Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence

Daytime Sleepiness and Antihistamines

Fragmenting Sleep Diminishes Its Recuperative Value

Daytime Alertness in Patients with Chronic Insomnia Compared with Asymptomatic Control Subjects

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

Pharmacology and Sleep

Sleepiness: Its Measurement and Determinants

Periodic Leg Movement, L-Dopa, 5-Hydroxytryptophan, and L-Tryptophan

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

T. Roth, Ph.D., T. Roehrs, Ph.D., G. Koshorek, J. Sicldost^, B.A., and

The REM Cycle is a Sleep-Dependent Rhythm

What Is the Moment of Sleep Onset for Insomniacs?

The Forty-Eight Hour Day

Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): A Standard Measure of Sleepiness *

Behavioral Treatment and Sleep. The Effects of Regularizing Sleep-Wake Schedules on Daytime Sleepiness

Milena Pavlova, M.D., FAASM Department of Neurology, Brigham and Women's Hospital Assistant Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School Medical

The Effects of Caffeine on Simulated Night-Shift Work and Subsequent Daytime Sleep

EFFICACY OF MODAFINIL IN 10 TAIWANESE PATIENTS WITH NARCOLEPSY: FINDINGS USING THE MULTIPLE SLEEP LATENCY TEST AND EPWORTH SLEEPINESS SCALE

Sleep latency testing as a time course measure of state arousal

The Alpha Attenuation Test: Assessing Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Narcolepsy-Cataplexy

The Multiple Sleep Latency Test: Individual Variability and Time of Day Effect in Normal Young Adults

sleep latencies Arousal and sleepiness in insomnia patients the tendency to fall asleep

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

I MSLT=multiple sleep latency test; MWI'=mainteDance of I

Disorders of Excessive Daytime Somnolence: Polygraphic and Clinical Data for 100 Patients

Multiple Naps and the Evaluation of Daytime Sleepiness in Patients with Upper Airway Sleep Apnea

Daytime Functioning and Nighttime Sleep Before, During, and After a 146-Hour Tennis Match

The Effects of Short Daytime Naps for Five Consecutive Days

Daytime Sleepiness: An Epidemiological Study of Young Adults

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis

Andrew Tilley, Frank Donohoe, and Sharon Hensby. Department of Psychology, University of Queensland, Queensland, Australia

Why are we so sleepy?

Automobile Accidents in Patients with Sleep Disorders

Daytime Carryover of Triazolam and Flurazepam in Elderly Insomniacs

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Morning Work: Effects of Early Rising on Sleep and Alertness

Beyond Sleep Hygiene: Behavioral Approaches to Insomnia

Selective Slow-Wave Sleep (SWS) Deprivation and SWS Rebound: Do We Need a Fixed SWS Amount per Night?

EFFECTS OF NIGHTTIME NAPS ON BODY TEMPERATURE CHANGES, SLEEP PATTERNS, AND SELF-EVALUATION

H-Reflex Suppression and Autonomic Activation During Lucid REM Sleep: A Case Study

The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing

Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. (Dahlstrom et al., 1972). Sleep status was determined

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance

Circadian Characteristics of Sleep Propensity Function In Healthy Elderly: A Comparison With Young Adults

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

The Consequences of a Week of Insomnia II: Patients with Insomnia

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

Sleep Questionnaire. If yes, what? If yes, how would you describe it? Please explain? If yes, what times are these?

ORIGINAL ARTICLES. Inter-REM Sleep Intervals Distribution in Healthy Young Subjects

Treatment of Chronic Insomnia by Restriction of Time in Bed

How Well do School-aged Children Comply with Imposed Sleep Schedules at Home?

th Ave NE Suite F Bellevue, WA Phone: (425) Fax: (425) Excessive Daytime Sleepiness

Chapter Five. Sleep McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved.

Sleep and Ageing. Siobhan Banks PhD. Body and Brain at Work, Centre for Sleep Research University of South Australia

Effect of Sleep Disruption on Sleep, Performance, and Mood

EEG Arousals: Scoring Rules and Examples. A Preliminary Report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders Association

Sleep Better! Improving Sleep for Children. V. Mark Durand, Ph.D. University of South Florida St. Petersburg

Benzodiazepines and memory

INTRINSIC SLEEP DISORDERS. Excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) is a common complaint. Causes of EDS are numerous and include:

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

FEP Medical Policy Manual

Sleep Across the Life Cycle

Article printed from

Simplest method: Questionnaires. Retrospective: past week, month, year, lifetime Daily: Sleep diary What kinds of questions would you ask?

Clinical Trial Synopsis TL , NCT#

The Effects of Presleep Stress on Sleep-Onset Insomnia

SLEEP PROBLEMS DURING ADOLESCENCE: LINKS

WHEN COUNTING SHEEP FAILS: ADMINISTERING SINGLE-SESSION COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL THERAPY FOR INSOMNIA IN A GROUP PSYCHOEDUCATIONAL FORMAT

A Modified Method for Scoring Slow Wave Sleep of Older Subjects

Chewing Can Relieve Sleepiness in a Night of Sleep Deprivation

INSOMNIAS. Stephan Eisenschenk, MD Department of Neurology

Reboot Your Energy. Presented by Jo Lichten, Ph.D., RDN CM12 5/23/ :00 AM - 12:30 PM

Attacking the Roadway Sleep Zombies

Neuropsychological Function in Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (OSAS) Compared to Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease (COPD)

LEARNING MANUAL OF PSG CHART

Cumulative Sleepiness, Mood Disturbance, and Psychomotor Vigilance Performance Decrements During a Week of Sleep Restricted to 4-5 Hours per Night

RESTore TM. Clinician Manual for Single User. Insomnia and Sleep Disorders. A step by step manual to help you guide your clients through the program

I t is now well established that night work negatively

Module 22: Fact or Falsehood?

Subjective daytime sleepiness in schoolchildren

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Procedures in the Sleep Laboratory

The Effects of Sleep Extension on the Athletic Performance of Collegiate Basketball Players

SLEEP, ADOLESCENCE AND SCHOOL Overview of problems and solutions

The sleep disturbance index a measure for structural alterations of sleep due to environmental influences

John McLachlan. Clinical Lead Pulmonary Physiology & Sleep Medicine. President Elect, WA Branch Thoracic Society of Australia & NZ

Sleep & Wakefulness Disorders in Parkinson s Disease: The Challenge of Getting a Good Night s Sleep

YOU REALLY NEED TO SLEEP: Several methods to improve your sleep

Do You Get Enough Sleep?

Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Epidemiology Of Alcohol and Medication As Aids To Sleep in Early Adulthood

Home Sleep Testing Questionnaire

Circadian period and the timing of melatonin onset in men and women: predictors of sleep during the weekend and in the laboratory

Evidence for Circadian Influence on Human Slow Wave Sleep During Daytime Sleep Episodes

The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration

Author's address: Dr. M. BILLIARD, Service de Physiopathologie des Maladies Nerveuses. Faculte de Medecine, F-34 Montpellier (France)

SLEEP HABITS OF HEALTHY YOUNG ADULTS: USE OF A SLEEP QUESTIONNAIRE

Dr Alex Bartle. Director Sleep Well Clinic

Transcription:

Sleep 2(5):449-457, Raven Press, Ltd., New York 989 Association of Professional Sleep Societies Sleep Extension in Sleepy and Alert Normals Timothy Roehrs, Victoria Timms, Ardith Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos, and Thomas Roth Sleep Disorders and Research Center, Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit, Michigan, U.S.A. Summary: Twenty-four healthy, young (2-35 years old) men with no complaints of daytime sleepiness, no habitual napping, and polysomnographically verified normal nocturnal sleep extended their time in bed (TIB) to 0 h for 6 consecutive nights to assess the effects of sleep extension on daytime sleepiness and performance. Twelve subjects had basal average daily sleep latencies of ~6 min on the Multiple Sleep Latency Test and 2 had latencies of ~6 min before TIB was extended. The sleep extension improved daytime sleepiness differentially in the two groups. The degree of improvement was greater in the sleepy subjects than the alert subjects and the pattern of improvement differed between the groups. Sleepy subjects showed an immediate and uniform increase in alertness, while alert subjects did not show improvements until late in the extension. However, sleepy subjects never achieved the baseline level of sleepiness/alertness seen in the alert subjects. Key Words: Multiple Sleep Latency Test-Sleep extension-daytime sleepiness-sleepy and alert subjects. Survey and laboratory studies indicate that when given the opportunity (i.e., on weekends or in the laboratory without time cues), adults without sleep complaints and with objectively verified normal sleep will sleep beyond their habitual sleep times (,2). Further, when sleep is experimentally restricted for a night, the amount of sleep obtained on a subsequent night is extended (3). Thus, Webb and Agnew (3) hypothesized that many healthy adults have an accumulated sleep debt that is a result of chronically insufficient sleep relative to sleep need. However, such data show merely that people can sleep more if given the opportunity. If sleep debt is a truly useful construct, some daytime benefits should be associated with additional sleep and the extent of the benefit should be associated with the degree of the sleep debt. Thus, one critical test of a hypothesized sleep debt is to assess the daytime effects of sleep extension. Some studies of sleep extension suggest it is detrimental. These studies show increases in subjective measures of daytime sleepiness and reduced performance efficiency on vigilance and complex psychomotor tasks in healthy young adults who extended bedtimes from the usual 8 to 0 or h for -2 nights (4-7). However, studies Accepted for publication April 989. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. T. Roehrs at Henry Ford Hospital, 292 West Grand Blvd., Detroit, MI 48202, U.S.A. 449

450 T. ROEHRS ET AL. that have used the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) to assess the effects of sleep extension found improvements in daytime sleepiness/alertness after bedtime was increased to 0 h for 4 consecutive nights (8,9). To determine whether or not the apparent detrimental effects of sleep extension in earlier studies were the result of "oversleeping," a recent study assessed the differential effect of sleep extension after restricted versus normal sleep (0). Bedtime was increased to h for night following either a nominai8.5-h or a restricted 6.5-h bedtime schedule on previous night. Again, mean sleep latency was increased (i.e., sleepiness was reduced) and performance measun~s were improved following extended sleep. These effects were observed regardless of the amount of sleep the night prior to the sleep extension. The failure to find differential benefits of sleep extension in the above study may be due to the acute nature of the sleepiness in the restricted 6.5-h bedtime schedule. It has been shown that a modest restriction of sleep time will accumulate over successive nights to increase one's basal level of sleepiness (). A recent report described the basal daytime sleepiness of a large sample of healthy young adults with no sleep-wake complaints (2). Average daily sleep latency within the sample ranged from 2 to 20 min on the MSLT with ~25% of subjects having unusually short or long sleep latencies. The present study assessed whether or not differences in basal level of sleepiness may interact with the effect of extending sleep time. Subjects were selected for their unusual sleepiness (MSLT average daily sleep latency of :;;;6 min) or alertness (MSLT average daily sleep latency of ;=,:6 min) and their bedtime was extended to 0 h for 6 consecutive nights. No control groups were employed in this study because a specific prediction regarding a differential benefit of sleep extension was being made: Sleepy subjects would benefit and alert subjects would show little or no benefit. Further, several studies have shown there is little change in MSLT scores over consecutive days with a constant 8-h sleep schedule each night (8,3). METHODS Subjects The subjects were 24 healthy, nonsmoking men aged 2-35 years with no complaints of daytime sleepiness, no habitual napping, and normal nocturnal sleep, all determined by the screening described below. They signed informed consents and were paid for participation. Procedures Subjects were recruited from an inventory of subjects who had previously been screened with an MSLT for participation in various earlier studies. In a telephone interview the subjects reported their usual nocturnal bedtimes, risetimes, and total sleep times (see Table O. No exclusions were made based on reported sleep times and bedtimes or risetimes. But subjects with highly irregular sleep schedules (i.e., >2-h variations in bedtimes) were excluded. Subjects also were excluded if they answered yes to questions regarding difficulty sleeping at night, sleepiness during the daytime, and daytime napping. They reported no history of alcohol or drug abuse and no current drug use and drank a weekly average of one to seven alcoholic beverages. Each subject then reported to the sleep center and underwent a medical history and physical examination including blood and urine apalyses to verify normal health and the absence of recent drug use.

SLEEP EXTENSION 45 TABLE. Self-reported sleep habits Age (yrs) Total sleep time (h) Bedtime Daily variation (h) Risetime Daily variation (h) Data are means (SO). Sleepy 25.2 (4.9) 7.0 (.0) 2:06 a.m. (60 min) 0.8 (0.3) 7:30 a.m. (55 min) 0.8 (0.3) Alert 23.8 (3.2) 7.55 (0.8) 2:0 a.m. (54 min).2 (0.5) 8:00 a.m. (62 min).2 (0.5) Subjects then spent 8-h night and the following day in the sleep laboratory to reconfirm their level of daytime sleepiness. They reported.5 h before their usual bedtime, with 2230 h being the latest arrival time, ensuring a OOOO-h bedtime and 0800-h risetime. These limits were established to maintain a minimum of 2 h of wakefulness before the first latency test of the MSLT. After arriving at the laboratory, each subject had electrodes attached for a standard polysomnogram, which included bilateral electrooculogram (EOG), submental and tibialis electromyograms (EMG), central (C3/C4) and occipital (Oz) electroencephalograms (EEG), electrocardiograms (V5), and nasooral airflow recorded with thermistors (4). Subjects then went to bed at the established bedtime, remaining in bed for 8 h while the polysomnogram was collected. Upon arising in the morning, subjects bathed and were allowed to eat a light breakfast that included a roll and a noncaffeinated beverage of choice. Electrodes were checked and replaced if necessary to prepare for the MSLT. The MSLT was conducted at 000, 200, 400, and 600 h following the standard procedures (5). For the MSLT, subjects went to bed in a darkened room and were instructed to try to fall asleep, while EOGs, submental EMG, and EEGs, always including an Oz placement, were recorded. The recording was terminated after min of unambiguous stage sleep, the first signs of stage 2 or REM sleep, or 20 min of continuous wake according to standard sleep stage criteria (4). At 030 h subjects were trained on a 20-min divided attention task. The task involves tracking, with ajoystick, a moving target appearing on a video monitor, while responding on a key to the appearance of a target in the periphery or the center of the video screen. Average tracking deviations and reaction times to peripheral and central targets were recorded. At 430 h subjects were trained on a 40-min auditory vigilance task. This task consisted of detecting long tones occurring randomly in a continuous series of shorter tones. Reaction times (in four blocks of 0 min), misses, and false-positives were recorded. After the screening subjects with average sleep latencies on the MSLT of ~6 or ~6 min and whose nocturnal polysomnogram showed no clinical abnormalities were asked to participate. The subjects qualifying underwent 6 consecutive nights of sleep extension (0 h) preceded by a baseline night (8 h). Bedtime was 2330-0730 h on baseline and 230-0730 h on extension nights. Each subject slept in the laboratory (reporting 30 min before the appropriate bedtime) on the baseline night and on extension nights,3, and 6. The following day MSLT and performance testing were conducted as described previously. On nonlaboratory nights subjects slept at home, maintaining the appropriate sleep schedule and their usual daytime activities. Sleep was monitored by actigraph on all study nights, including the baseline night and extension nights I, 3, and 6 when the subjects slept at the laboratory. The actigraph has

452 T. ROEHRS ET AL. been shown to predict polysomnographically determined sleep or wake with 93% accuracy (6). It also has been shown to be sensitive to the effects of restriction and extension of time in bed (TIB) (7). Actigraphic monitoring of sleep was employed in this phase of the study because the primary purpose of the study was to assess changes' in daytime function associated with extended TIB and not to document changes in sleep occurring during extension. Latency to the first epoch of stage sleep was scored for each latency test according to standard sleep stage scoring criteria (4). Nocturnal sleep efficiency was determined as derived from the actigraph recordings by a previously described algorithm (6). Sleep latency, sleep efficiency, and the previously described measures of performance were submitted to mixed design analyses with group (sleepy or alert) the betweensubjects factor and days (baseline, extension, 3, and 6) the within-subjects factor using the general linear model analysis (SAS Institute). Conservative probability levels as corrected by the Greenhouse-Geisser procedure are reported for the within-subjects factor. Post hoc comparisons were made following the Duncan procedure. RESULTS Self-reported sleep,habits The usual sleep habits of the sleepy and alert subjects are presented in Table. The two groups do not differ significantly in any oftheir self-reported sleep habits, although there are two notable trends in the data. The alert subjects tended to report longer sleep times and later risetimes than the sleepy subjects. Daily variation in sleep schedule was not different between the groups, but then subjects with a >2-h variation were excluded from participation in this study. Daytime sleepiness/alertness Average daily sleep latency on baseline and extension days,3, and 6 for the sleepy and alert subjects is plotted in Fig.. Sleep latency increased significantly from baseline through extension day 6 in both groups (F = 8.57, p < 0.00). The groups differed initially in average daily sleep latency by design, but those significant group differences remained over the whole extension period (F = 78.70, p < 0.00). Post hoc comparisons (p < 0.0) between groups showed differences on baseline and each extension day (, 3, and 6). Sleepy subjects never attained the baseline level of the alert subjects. However, the increase in sleep latency in sleepy subjects never reached an asymptote (see Fig. O. The pattern of increase in average daily sleep latency differed between groups as indicated by a significant groups-by-extension interaction (F = 2.94, p < 0.04). The slope of the function relating sleep latency to days of extension was 0.22 for the sleepy group and 0.0 for the alert group. The exact nature of the differential group increase in sleep latency was assessed by examining the simple effects of sleep extension within the sleepy group and within the alert group. Sleepy subjects showed a significant increase in latency (F = 9.30, P < 0.002) as found on the omnibus analysis. Post hoc tests among days revealed no difference from baseline to extension day, but differences between baseline and extension day 3 (p < 0.0) and 6 (p < 0.007). As well, extension day differed from extension day 3 and 6 (p < 0.0 and 0.04, respectively). The alert subjects also showed a significant increase in latency (F = 4.7, p < 0.02). But the post hoc tests indicated no baseline to extension day or 3 differences. Dif-

SLEEP EXTENSION 453 > u z W f «--.J 0... w W --.J (/) Z «w "2 20 5 0 5 I I r-ot-j --~l;-------- BASE DAY DAY 3 I _-----0 TIS Bh 0h 0h 0h 0h 0h 0h FIG.. Mean sleep latency (±SD) for sleepy (solid circles) and alert subjects (open circles) on the baseline day and extension days,3, and 6. On baseline the time in bed (TIB) was 8 h and on extension days TIB was 0 h. Analyses yielded group effects, p < 0.00; extension effects, p < 0.00; and group-by-extension interaction, p < 0.04. Sleepy subjects (e) showed immediate extension effects and alert subjects (0) showed effects later. j DAY 6 ferences occurred only between baseline and extension day 6 (p < 0.05). Similarly, extension day did not differ from extension day 3 and only a day to 6 difference was found (p < 0.00). Test-by-test analyses of group and sleep extension effects produced results quite similar to those for average daily sleep latency with one notable exception. On the 000-h test, group differences (F = 68.68, p < 0.00) and an extension effect (F = 3.57, p < 0.02) were observed. In addition, a significant group-by-extension interaction was found (F = 5.69, p < 0.00). The interaction is illustrated in Fig. 2. To explore the nature of the interaction, analyses of simple extension effects were conducted within the alert and sleepy groups. For alert subjects there were significant extension effects (F = 3.93, p < 0.03), but sleep latency initially declined with the sleep extension before finally increasing. Post hoc comparisons revealed a difference between baseline and extension day 3 (p < 0.02) and between extension days vs. 6 and 3 vs. 6 (p < 0.04 and 0.02, respectively). In contrast, in sleepy subjects there were significant extension effects (F = 6.56, P < 0.0) and sleep latency increased over the 6 extension days. There were differences between baseline and extension days,3, and 6 (p < 0.05, 0.0, and 0.00, respectively). Extension days did not differ one to the other. Results of analyses of the other latency tests were quite similar to the data for average daily sleep latency. At 200 h group differences were found (F = 52.87, p < 0.00), but the extension effect was not significant. Again on the 400-h test, both group (F = 4.67, P < 0.00) and extension (F = 2.89, P < 0.04) differences were found. Finally, for 600 h, both group (F = 46.76, p < 0.00) and extension (F = 3.0, P < 0.04) effects were found. No group-by-extension interactions were found on the testby-test analyses for the 200-, 400-, and 600-h tests. Sleep, Vol. 2, No.5, /989

454 T. ROEHRS ET AL. 0 0 20 /6 T 6 ~~I 0 5 6 6 r---. VJ T T~ c E... 0 /"... '-" I l- «T/ -..J... D... 5 ~/l W... W -..J (f) 0+-----+-----+-----+-----+-----~--~-----4----~ BASE DAY DAY 3 DAY 6 lib 8h 0h 0h 0h 0h 0h 0h FIG. 2. Sleep latency (means ± SD) on the first sleep latency test at 000 h for sleepy (solid triangles) and alert subjects (open triangles). Sleep latency is plotted for baseline when time in bed (TIB) was 8 h and for extension days,3, and 6 when TIB was 0 h. Analyses yielded group effects, p < 0.00; extension effects, p < 0.02; and group-by-extension interaction, p < 0.00. Alert subjects (Ll) showed an initial decline and then an increase and sleepy subjects ("') showed a uniform increase. Performance The divided attention performance measures for each group are presented in Table 2. The data analyses indicated that central and peripheral reaction times were improved over the sleep extension period (F = 4.95, p < 0.00; F = 4.02, p < 0.02, respectively). The post hoc comparisons indicated baseline versus extension day 6 (p < 0.00, 0.02, respectively) differences on both measures and an extension day vs. 6 difference in central reaction time (p < 0.05). No group differences were found on these two measures, but a group-by-extension interaction (F = 2.9, P < 0.05) was found on the central reaction time measure. Group differences were detected on extension day (p < 0.0), but not baseline or extension days 3 and 6. On the measure oftracking (average deviations), no extension or interaction effects were found. However, group differences in average tracking were detected (F = 4.60, P < 0.04). Z scores, the combination of all three measures, showed an overall group difference (F = 4., P < 0.05) and a trend for extension effects. Vigilance performance showed no group differences in reaction time or number of errors (misses and false-positives). Extension effects on reaction time were observed in the last 0 min of the task (F = 2.90, p < 0.05), with the post hoc analyses showing a baseline versus extension day 6 difference (p < 0.03). No interactions were found. Nocturnal sleep efficiency No overall group or extension effects in sleep efficiency were detected, but a significant interaction was found (F = 3.74, p < 0.03). The nocturnal sleep efficiencies are presented in Table 3. The sleepy group had a higher sleep efficiency than the alert group on the baseline (95.4 vs. 90.3%). This difference in sleep efficiency diminished after the

SLEEP EXTENSION 455 TABLE 2. Divided attention performance Extension day Baseline 3 6 Central RTa Sleepy 25.25 24.83 24.08 24.8 (3.36) (3.43) (4.03) (3.25) Alert 25.33 23.42 23.83 24. (4.2) (2.43) (3.6) (3.76) Peripheral RTa Sleepy 23.75 23.58 22.75 23.09 (4.58) (3.2) (3.96) (3.36) Alert 24.33 22.58 2.83 22.22 (3.96) (3.73) (3.07) (4.60) A vg. tracking deviation b Sleepy 95.75 96.92 88.25 89.0 (8.56) (9.7) (9.6) (7.70) Alert 78.58 78.00 78.42 75.22 (8.56) (9.7) (9.5) (6.08) Div. atten. Z score b Sleepy.06 0.99-0.0 0.60 (2.47) (.84) (.83) (2.8) Alert 0.07-0.95-0.96-0.96 (2.28) (.79) (.83) (.9) Data are means (SD). Measures are expressed in arbitrary computer-generated units. For all measures lower scores indicate improved performance. RT, reaction time. a Extension effects, p.;; 0.02. b Group effects, p.;; 0.05. first extension night and the groups were similar for the duration of the extension period. DISCUSSION This study found that extending TIB to 0 h for 6 consecutive nights improved daytime sleepiness/alertness differentially in sleepy versus alert subjects. As predicted, the alert subjects showed very small improvements in daytime sleepiness/alertness as compared to much larger improvements in sleepy subjects. The differential improvement may in part be due to a ceiling effect on the MSLT in the alert subjects. However, the differential benefits of sleep extension in the two groups were evident not only in the degree of improvement, but also in the time course. Specifically, the sleepy subjects showed greater as well as earlier improvements in daytime sleepiness/alertness. Further, the present results parallel findings regarding sleep extension in other studies TABLE 3. Nocturnal sleep efficiency Extension day Baseline 3 6 Sleepy subjects Alert subjects 95.4 (2.75)a 90.3 (5.94) 90.4 (3.96) 89.4 (5.0) 88.4 (5.35) 90.2 (3.75) 88.6 (3.38) 87.7 (4.67) Data are means (SD). Measure is minutes of sleep relative to minutes of bedtime determined by actigraph. a Group.by.night interaction, p < 0.03; groups differed on baseline.

456 T. ROEHRS ET AL. using the MSLT to assess daytime function (8,9). They further support the results of Carskadon et al. (0) in which sieep extension ied to improved daytime function with or without a prior acute ( night) restriction of sleep. The performance results of the present study did not support the hypothesis of a differential improvement in sleepy versus alert subjects. Improvements were found On the divided attention and vigilance tasks over the 6 days of the study. However, the pattern and extent of improvement did not differ between sleepy and alert subjects. The one extension-by-group interaction that was found (on divided attention reaction time) should be interpreted as being spurious; the group difference occurred only on extension day. Given that the improvements in performance were uniform and that a nonextension group was not included in this study, the improvements in performance seen in each group could be due to practice effects. Practice effects could also have masked differential improvements between the groups and thus explain the absence of an interaction. An interesting result of this study is that in the alert group the initial 3 nights of sleep extension produced a decreased sleep latency on the first latency test, 2 h after arising. On the other three latency tests (4, 6, and 8 h after arising) in the alert subjects, there typically was no change or a small improvement in sleep latency. The performance evaluations in the earlier Taub et al. (4-7) studies were generally conducted during the first 2 h after arising (starting 30 min after arising). Further, the sleep extension in those studies was acute, -2 nights. Consequently, the improvement in function that occurred after 3 nights of sleep extension in the alert subjects of this study would not have been observed because of the acute nature of the earlier studies. Thus, the use of unusually alert subjects combined with an acute sleep extension could account for the Taub et al. findings of detrimental effects associated with sleep extension. The reason for this initial sleepiness (i.e., within the first 2 h after arising) in alert subjects is unclear at this time. A number of authors have described a "sleep inertia," a temporary loss of performance efficiency after awakening from sleep (8). Usually, it is thought that sleep inertia is short-lasting, enduring for 5-30 min. However, some studies have found sleep inertia for as long as 2 h. But those findings are under conditions of sleep deprivation and not sleep extension, and further not in alert subjects. Most importantly, the sleepy subjects of the present study did not show an increased sleepiness on the first latency test. It is unlikely that sleep inertia is the cause of the initial sleepiness of the alert subjects in the present study. A circadian dysrhythmia resulting from the change in sleep Jschedule is another possible explanation. The question remains, however, as to why the alert subjects and not the sleepy subjects are sensitive to the dysrhythmia. It could be that the initial level of sleepiness in sleepy subjects is masking circadian effects, while those effects are not being masked in the alert subjects. In other words, there is some threshold level of alertness that must be attained before the circadian dysrhythmic effects can be detected. In sleepy subjects the circadian dysrhythmia is not important relative to the existing sleep debt, while in alert subjects the circadian dysrhythmia, in the absence of a chronic sleep debt, produces its effect. Notice that the alert subjects appear to adapt to the circadian insult as indicated by the increase in latency on the first latency test after night 3 of extension (see Fig. 2). Another interesting finding of this study is that the sleepy subjects over the 6 nights of sleep extension never reached the baseline level of the alert SUbjects. Average daily sleep latency of the sleepy subjects after sleep extension did reach a level comparable

SLEEP EXTENSION 457 to the mean of average daily sleep latency found in a large sample of healthy normal young adults (2). In that sense one could say that the daytime sleepiness of these subjects was normalized. However, it remains to be determined whether additional sleep extension could have produced levels of alertness similar to those of the alert subjects. Other variables, of course, may limit the extent to which sleep extension could increase the MSLT score of the sleepy subjects. The difference between the sleepy and alert subjects after the 6 nights of sleep extension could reflect trait differences in the ability to fall asleep and/or sleep need. Patients with insomnia have been shown to have unusually long latencies on the MSLT compared to a nons elected group of age-matched normals (9). Furthermore, the latencies of the insomnia patients were unrelated to their total sleep times, while the latencies (which were in the normal range) of the normals were related to sleep times. Studies are presently being conducted to further assess the differences between alert and sleepy adult normals without sleep complaints. REFERENCES. Johns M, Gay T, Goodyear M, Masterson J. Sleep habits of healthy young adults: use of a sleep questionnaire. Br J Prev Soc Med 97 ;25:236-4. 2. Webb WB, Agnew HW. Are we chronically sleep deprived? Bull Psychon Soc 975;6:47--8. 3. Webb WB, Agnew HW. Effects on subsequent sleep of an acute restriction of sleep length. Psychophysiology 975;2:367-70. 4. Taub JM, Berger RJ. Extended sleep and performance: the Rip Van Winkle effect. Psychon Sci 969;6:204-5. 5. Taub JM, Globus GG, Phoebus E, Drury R. Extended sleep and performance. Nature 97;233:42-3. 6. Taub JM, Berger RJ. The effects of changing the phase and duration of sleep. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept] 976;2:30-4. 7. Taub M. Effects of ad lib extended-delayed sleep on sensorimotor performance, memory and sleepiness in the young adult. Physiol Behav 980;25:77--87. 8. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Sleep tendency during extension of nocturnal sleep. Sleep Res 979;8: 47. 9. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Nocturnal determinants of daytime sleepiness. Sleep 982;5:S73-8. 0. Carskadon MA, Mancuso J, Keenan S, Littell W, Dement WC. Sleepiness following oversleeping. Sleep Res 986;5:70.. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Cumulative effects of sleep restriction on daytime sleepiness. Psychophysiology 98;8:07-3. 2. Levine B, Roehrs T, Zorick F, Roth T. Daytime sleepiness in young adults. Sleep 988;:39-46. 3. Lipschutz L, Roehrs T, Spielman A, Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A, Lamphere J, Roth T. Caffeine's alerting effects in sleepy normals. Sleep Res 988;7:49. 4. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Los Angeles: Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, University of California at Los Angeles, 968. 5. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM. Guidelines for the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT): a standard measure of sleepiness. Sleep 986;9:59-24. 6. Levine B, Moyles T, Roehrs T, Fortier J, Roth T. Actigraphic monitoring and polygraphic recording in the determination of sleep and wake. Sleep Res 986;5:247. 7. Levine B, Lumley M, Roehrs T, Zorick F, Roth T. The effects of acute sleep restriction and extension on sleep efficiency. In! J Neurosci 988;43:39-43. 8. Naitoh P. Circadian cycles and restorative power of naps. In: Johnson LC, Tepas DI, Colquhoun WP, Colligan MJ, eds. Biological rhythms, sleep and shiftwork. New York: Spectrum, 98:553--80. 9. Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, Young D, Roth T. Daytime alertness in patients with chronic insomnia compared with asymptomatic control subjects. Sleep 988;:54-60. Sleep. Vol. 2. No.5. 989