Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

Similar documents
Color Difference Equations and Their Assessment

Role of color memory in successive color constancy

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

Vision Research. Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Suzanne C. Belmore a, Steven K.

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

COLOUR CONSTANCY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETS

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

LEA Color Vision Testing

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

= add definition here. Definition Slide

Development of a new loudness model in consideration of audio-visual interaction

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations

Color Constancy Under Changes in Reflected Illumination

Practice Test Questions

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings

Surface Color Perception under Different Illuminants and Surface Collections

CHANGES IN VISUAL SPATIAL ORGANIZATION: RESPONSE FREQUENCY EQUALIZATION VERSUS ADAPTATION LEVEL

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

Colour preference model for elder and younger groups

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Colour Communication.

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

The Color Between Two Others

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

Visual Transformation of Size

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica

A Factorial Design Experiment in Affective Combination of Visual and Tactile Stimuli in the Context of Keypads

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

METHODOLOGICAL STUDIES OF SIZE CONSTANCY (1) : SIZE CONSTANCY MEASURED BY TWO EXPERIMENTAL METHODS

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Colour Fitting Formulae Used for Predicting Colour Quality of Light Sources

Surround articulation. II. Lightness judgments

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

THE EFFECT OF CO-EXISTENT STIMULI OTHER THAN TEST STIMULUS ON BRIGHTNESS CONSTANCY

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II

Mechanisms of color constancy under nearly natural viewing (color appearance adaptation natural scenes retinex)

An Experimental Study of the Relationship Between Focality and Short-Term Memory of Colors in the Japanese Language

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

3/15/06 First Class Meeting, Pick Up First Readings, Discuss Course, MDF Overview Lecture (Chs. 1,2,3)

Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind

THE EFFECTS OF STRIPED CLOTHING ON PERCEPTIONS OF BODY SIZE

PERCEPTION AND ACTION

Perception Lab Reports. Style Guide. Aaron Bornstein

Supplementary Materials

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Diagram the perceptual principles of hue, saturation, and brightness and the role they play in our perception of color.

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Neurological Basis for Placbeo effect*

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

The Affective Feelings of Colored Fonts

Asymmetries in the time-course of chromatic adaptation and the significance of contrast

Examining Effective Navigational Learning Strategies for the Visually Impaired

Color scaling of discs and natural objects at different luminance levels

Effects of Latitudinal Position on Color Perception: The Case of Brazil. Joshua B. Wortman. University of California, San Diego

Classes of Sensory Classification: A commentary on Mohan Matthen, Seeing, Doing, and Knowing (Oxford: Clarendon Press, 2005)

Perceptual separability and spatial models'

Unique Hue Data for Colour Appearance Models. Part II: Chromatic Adaptation Transform

Dr. Christoph Witzel and Professor Karl Gegenfurtner SpringerReference

Unit 4: Sensation and Perception

Testing the Softproofing Paradigm

The Effect o f Alcohol on Human Information Processing Rate

The New Colour Scales based on Saturation, Vividness, Blackness and Whiteness

Observation is the capacity of the individual to know the environment by the use of his senses. There are two steps in the process of observation:

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2007

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Mean Observer Metamerism and the Selection of Display Primaries

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

An empirical explanation of color contrast

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

The Role of Color and Attention in Fast Natural Scene Recognition

Colour Naming and Classification in a Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted.

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

The Color of Similarity

The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect

Contrast and the justification of effort

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Adaptation and perceptual norms

Psychology and You. Dear Students,

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Differentiation for Novel Dimensions

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Transcription:

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity J. A. Richardson and I. Davies Department of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford GU2 5XH, Surrey, United Kingdom Abstract It is known that colour constancy is more robust when the changes in illumination are restricted to the phases of daylight than it is for changes in most artificial light sources, suggesting that the ability to correctly identify surface colours may depend in part, on the observers familiarity with the light source. The present study examines whether a relatively short period of practice in judging surface colours under different illuminants leads to an improvement in colour constancy, and if so whether the improvement is restricted to the illuminants under which the judgements were made. Five subjects were asked to identify by name (chosen from 11 basic colour terms) a total of 70 colour patches under 4 different broad band illuminants. Subjects then named each colour patch a further 20 times distributed over a 2 week period, 10 times under each of two of the broad band illuminants. Finally, subjects again named the patches under all 4 illuminants. It was found that the degree of categorical colour constancy under broad band illumination was significantly better after the learning trials, and that the improvement was greater for the illuminants used in the learning trials. 1. Introduction Colour constancy is defined as the invariant appearance of surface colours under changes in the spectral composition of the incident illumination. The visual system is apparently better at compensating for some illuminant changes than others, eg. colour constancy is most complete when the colorimetric shift lies along the blue-yellow axis, 1,2 and is more robust when the changes in illumination are restricted to the phases of daylight than it is for changes in most artificial light sources, 3 which suggests that the ability to correctly identify surface colours may depend in part, on the observers familiarity with the light source. The present study examines whether a relatively short period of practice in judging surface colours under different illuminants leads to an improvement in colour constancy, and if so whether the improvement is restricted to the illuminants under which the judgements were made. Colour appearance was examined using a colour naming method which involves subjects naming a large selection of colour surfaces under a number of different illuminants (which vary in chromaticity and/or intensity). This method allows the examination of real surface colours seen without a comparison field, and has been found to provide a reliable measure of colour constancy that does not require the separation of illuminant and surface colour to be made clear explicitly to the subjects. 4 2. Method 2.1. Subjects Five subjects (3 undergraduate and two postgraduate students at The University of Surrey) participated in the experiment. All subjects had normal or corrected to normal vision, and were naive as to the purpose of the experiment. 2.2. Stimuli At a viewing distance of 520mm, sixty-eight colour patches (30mm 30mm) were presented in the centre of a Mondrian surround (120mm horizontally by 60mm vertically). Under a standard white illuminant, the colour set was representative of the whole u, v chromaticity diagram. The Mondrian surround was composed of squares of various chromaticities. There were four versions of the surround which were presented in approximately a quarter of the trials each in a pseuo-random order: a basic Mondrian pattern, its mirror image, the basic pattern upside down, and the mirror image upside down. This insured that the average chromaticity and contrast remained constant while at the same time preventing subjects adapting to the background pattern or using chromatic changes in individual squares in the surround as a reference. 2.3. Equipment The stimuli were presented in a tachistoscope which allowed the light source and display time to be controlled. The stimuli were place in a card holder inside a lamp compartment, opposite a viewing appature. Viewing distance was approximately 510mm, and the stimulus displays subtended a visual angle of 16 horizontally, and 10 vertically. The displays were illuminated by two tube bulbs one aimed downward, and one aimed upward toward the displays at an angle of about 45. Chapter II Color Constancy 333

2.4. Procedure Subjects first familiarized themselves with the eleven basic colour terms which were used as response categories. The colour samples were presented one at a time in a random order which differed for each trial. Each stimulus was presented for 750 msec. On each trial 0subjects were asked to choose the most appropriate colour category for each of the 68 target patches. In the first set of trials each colour patch was presented four times once under each of four different illuminants : white, u = 0.22, v = 0.48; green, u = 0.19, v = 0.52; blue, u = 0.18, v = 0.52; orange, u = 0.26, v = 0.53. Four subjects then named each colour patch a further 20 times distributed over a 2 week period, 10 times under each of the green and blue illuminants. Finally, all subjects again named the patches under all 4 illuminants. 3. Results For each subject the colour name assigned to each colour patch served as the standard colour name. The deviations from the standard name when the same patch is viewed under the test illuminants was calculated and used as a measure of colour constancy. Figures 1, 2, and 3 show the number of deviations from the standard colour names for each subject under the green, blue and orange illuminants respectively, before and after training. It was found that the degree of colour constancyassigning the same name to the same colour patch under different illuminants for the light sources under which subjects trained (blue and green) was significantly better after the learning trials than before them, (blue: t = 7.78, df 4, p < 0.001; green: t = 4.05, p < 0.01). However, this improvement was not evident for the orange illuminant which subjects did not train under (t = 0.57, df 4, p > 0.05). In order to show the colour category shifts caused by the different light sources before and after the practice trials, the centroid locations for each category in u, v colour space were calculated for each observer. The centroids for all 5 observers under the three test illuminants are plotted in Figures 4, 5 and 6. As a further measure of the degree of colour constancy the centroid values were used to calculate a two dimensional version of the Brunswik ratio 5 : br = d obs /d prf where d obs = and d prf are the Euclidean distances between the location of the standard and the observed locations, and between the standard and perfect constancy respectively. A ratio of 0 indicates a complete lack of constancy, a ratio of 1 indicates perfect constancy, and a ratio of above 1 indicates overconstancy. Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of Bruswik Ratios for all 5 subjects for the Three Test Illuminants Before and After Training. Before Training After Training Illuminant Mean SD Mean SD Green 0.809 0.221 0.922 0.145 Blue 0.754 0.252 0.910 0.080 Orange 0.895 0.144 0.923 0.143 Table l shows the Brunswik Ratios for the three test illuminants. The Brunswik ratios also show that there is an improvement in colour constancy for all three test illuminants after the learning trials, and that the improvement is greater for the illuminants under which the trials took place. It should be noted however that the Brunswik ratio for the orange illuminant is nearer to 1.0 (perfect constancy) than it is for the green and blue illuminants before the learning trials. 4. Discussion In sum, it was found that the degree of colour constancy assigning the same name to the same colour patch under different broad band illuminants was significantly better after a relatively short period of practice in categorizing surface colours, and that the improvement was greater for the illuminants which practice took place. Explanations of colour constancy have ranged from sensory accounts, such as chromatic and contrast adaptation, to higher level, cognitively oriented explanations which emphasize the role of learning and memory for object colours. The results reported here suggest the importance of learning and memory, not only for object/surface colours, but also for the light source under which the surface colour is being judged. With regard to perceptual learning, the fact that subjects were not provided with feedback as to the accuracy of their responses, is consistent with Gibson s argument that mere exposure to the relevant stimuli in the absence of feedback is sufficient for learning to occur. The findings of this study thus suggest that models which attempt to predict which deviations from colour constancy will occur for a given illumination should consider individual observers familiarity with the illuminant. 5. References 1. McCann J., McKee S., & Taylor T. (1976) Quantitative studies in retinex theory. Vision Research, 16, 445-458 2. Worthey J. (1985) Limitations of colour constancy. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 2 249-258 3. Judd, D. (1940) Hue, Saturation and lightness of Surface colours with chromatic illumination. Journal of the Optical Society of America 30, 2-32 4. Troost J. & deweert C. (1991) Naming versus matching in colour constancy. Perception and Psychophysics, 50 591-602 5. Brunswik, E. (1928) Zur Entwicklung der Albedowwahnehmung. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie. 64 216-227 6. Gibson E. ( 1969) Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. Appleton Century Crofts. New York published previously in SPIE, Vol. 2414, page 50 334 Recent Progress in Color Processing

Figure 1. Number of deviations from standard colour names under the green illuminant Figure 2. Number of deviations from standard colour names under the blue illuminant Chapter II Color Constancy 335

Figure 3. Number of deviations from the standard colour names under the orange illuminant Figure 4. Centroid locations of basic colours under the green and standard illuminants 336 Recent Progress in Color Processing

Figure 5. Centroid locations of basic colours under the blue and standard illuminants * Centroid locations of basic colours expected under the blue illuminant if colour constancy were perfect. Figure 6. Centroid locations of basic colours under the orange and standard illuminants * Centroid locations of basic colours expected under the orange illuminant if colour constancy were perfect. Chapter II Color Constancy 337