How Widely is Blind Assessment Used in Scientific Research?

Similar documents
(THIS IS A PRE-PUBLICATION VERSION OF A PAPER PUBLISHED IN THE JOURNAL OF THE SOCIETY FOR PSYCHICAL RESEARCH APRIL 2004)

Psych 1Chapter 2 Overview

How much is that doggy in the window? A brief evaluation of the Jaytee experiments. Richard Wiseman

MSc Psychological Research Methods/ MPsych Advanced Psychology Module Catalogue / 2018

Study Design. Study design. Patrick Breheny. January 23. Patrick Breheny Introduction to Biostatistics (171:161) 1/34

Sampling Controlled experiments Summary. Study design. Patrick Breheny. January 22. Patrick Breheny Introduction to Biostatistics (BIOS 4120) 1/34

Asking and answering research questions. What s it about?

Glossary of Research Terms Compiled by Dr Emma Rowden and David Litting (UTS Library)

Lecture 4: Evidence-based Practice: Beyond Colorado

Using Lectures You May Already Have Prepared As a Basis for Teaching this Course

Combined Movement Theory

Chapter 1 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science

The Elephants We Can & Can t See: Bias & Stereotyping in the Care of the Seriously Ill

Choose an approach for your research problem

Q. & A. With Carol S. Dweck

A survey of the teaching of conscious sedation in dental schools of the United Kingdom and Ireland J A Leitch, 1 N M Girdler 2

RANDOMIZATION. Outline of Talk

The Swiss Government's Remarkable Report on Homeopathic Medicine Posted: 02/15/2012 8:56 am By Dana Ullman

QUESTIONS ANSWERED BY

Unfinished Business from Monday s Lecture

Am I free? Free will vs. neuroscience

Fame: I m Skeptical. Fernanda Ferreira. Department of Psychology. University of California, Davis.

Controlled Test of the Analgesic and Relaxant Properties of Nitrous Oxide

Qualitative research study design

VIEWS AND EXPERIENCES OF DEAF TEACHERS ON INCLUSIVE EDUCATION AND DISABILITY

The Changing Population: Oral health needs and challenges in the next generation

CHAPTER 2 EVALUATING NUTRITION INFORMATION OVERVIEW

Scientific Evidences in Homeopathy: a dynamic database

society. The social perspective is a way of looking at society. It sees society as something over and above the very people who are in that society.

Colaizzi s descriptive phenomenological method

Ellen Mayston Bates Professor of Neurophysiology of Epilepsy (2016)

Chapter 2 Evaluating Nutrition Information

The "Sense of Being Stared At" Does Not Depend On Known Sensory Clues

John Parrotta, Healing Plants of Peninsular India. Oxford & New York: CABI Publishing, 2001.

Biochemical Society Centenary: the Last 25 Years

Acute Stabilization In A Trauma Program: A Pilot Study. Colin A. Ross, MD. Sean Burns, MA, LLP

Citation Characteristics of Research Published in Emergency Medicine Versus Other Scientific Journals

An Industry- Biased Record

Evidence of Learning in a General Education Class

Unconscious Bias Training. Programme Overview. 1 Day or ½ Day? +44 (0) /

Interview with Jochen Kaiser February 2006

Test Bank Chapter 1 Welcome to Positive Psychology

AFFECTS, MOODS, EMOTIONS, AND BELONGING

Between Fact and Fiction: Artifacts and Ethics in Social Research. Toh Wah Seng ABSTRACT

In this second module in the clinical trials series, we will focus on design considerations for Phase III clinical trials. Phase III clinical trials

Journal of the Society for Psychical Research Vol. 65, pp (2001)

Dr. Harold Yuker: Eliminating Barriers. Presented by Hofstra University Library Special Collections

ECONOMIC AND SOCIAL RESEARCH COUNCIL END OF AWARD REPORT

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS VERSION 1 - REVIEW. Veronika Williams University of Oxford, UK 07-Dec-2015

Final Paper: Mental Illnesses: Then and Now

2 Critical thinking guidelines

2. METHODS OF DATA COLLECTION

Research Ethics and Philosophies

Unit Options and Core Texts

Research Accomplishments, Peer-reviewed monographs (1 required for promotion or 2 articles)

A MILESTONE IN GENETICS: Mendel s 1866 Paper

The Cinderella Hypothesis

Module 2/3 Research Strategies: How Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Introduction; Study design

Unconscious Gender Bias in Academia: from PhD Students to Professors

Instructional Goals in Psychology Psychology students Understanding of the Scientific Foundation of the Discipline

Journal of Professional Exercise Physiology

The Lecture Contains:

Clinical Research Facilities. Guide to using the NIHR identity

Most scholars recognize that we are a combination Heredity and environmental factors interact with and affect one another

Unit 1 History and Methods Chapter 1 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

THE PUBLIC AND GENETIC EDITING, TESTING, AND THERAPY

University Grants Commission, New Delhi Recognized Journal No ISSN: Print: ISSN: Online: X

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

AMS5 - MIDTERM Tuesday May 5th, 2015

EXPERIMENTS ON THE SENSE OF BEING STARED AT: THE ELIMINATION OF POSSIBLE ARTEFACTS

Dclin Psych Open Day October 2017

ON GENERALISING: SEEING THE GENERAL IN THE PARTICULAR AND THE PARTICULAR IN THE GENERAL IN MARKETING RESEARCH

ELEMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS Sections VII and XVI. Gustav Theodor Fechner (1860/1912)

was also my mentor, teacher, colleague, and friend. It is tempting to review John Horn s main contributions to the field of intelligence by

Social Research (Complete) Agha Zohaib Khan

Defining principles of Strategic family therapy

MATH& 146 Lesson 6. Section 1.5 Experiments

AND ITS VARIOUS DEVICES. Attitude is such an abstract, complex mental set. up that its measurement has remained controversial.

Eliminative materialism

Multitasking. Multitasking. Intro Problem Solving in Computer Science McQuain

Hypnotherapy Practitioner Diploma. Module 1

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

igh-sensitivity Cardiac Troponin - A Canadian Viewpoint from CIHR funded studies Friday May 30, 2014

Subject Description Form

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies throughout the world

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Psychology Department Assessment

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

Houdmont, J., Cox, T., & Griffiths, A. (2010). Work-related stress case definitions and ABSTRACT

Post-Hoc Rationalism in Science. Eric Luis Uhlmann. HEC Paris. (Commentary on Jones and Love, "Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment?

Gold and Hohwy, Rationality and Schizophrenic Delusion

Aim of Activity: To explore stereotypes, role models and possible occupations

Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology

CLAPA Regional Coordinators Project: Scotland

Transcription:

Alternative Therapies 5(3), 88-91, May 1999 How Widely is Blind Assessment Used in Scientific Research? by Rupert Sheldrake INTRODUCTION In everyday life, as in scientific research, "our beliefs, desires and expectations can influence, often subconsciously, how we observe and interpret things", as a recent article in the Skeptical Inquirer expressed it.(note 1) In experimental psychology and clinical research, these principles are widely recognized, which is why experiments in these subjects are often carried out under blind or double-blind conditions. There is overwhelming experimental evidence that experimenters' attitudes and expectations can indeed influence the outcome of experiments.(note 2) In single-blind experiments, an investigator does not know which samples or treatments are which. But when human subjects are involved, as in medicine and experimental psychology, double-blind procedures can be used to guard against the expectancy of both subjects and investigators. In a double-blind clinical trial, for example, some patients are given tablets of a drug and others are given similar-looking placebo tablets, pharmacologically inert. Neither researchers nor patients know who gets what. In such experiments, the largest placebo effects usually occur in trials in which both patients and physicians believe a powerful new treatment is being tested.(note 3) The inert tablets tend to work like the treatment being studied, and can even induce its characteristic side-effects.(note 4) Likewise, experimenter expectancy effects are well know in experimental psychology, and also show up in experiments on animal behavior.(note 2) In a fascinating historical account, Kaptchuk (note 5) has shown that blind assessment first began in the late 18th century "as a tool for fraud detection mounted by elite mainstream scientists and physicians to challenge the suspected delusions or charlatanism of unconventional medicine." Some of the first experiments were carried out to evaluate mesmerism, and were literally conducted with blindfolds. They took place in France at the house of Benjamin Franklin, the American minister plenipotentiary, who was head of a commission of inquiry appointed by King Louis XVI. The use of blind assessment had been adopted by the mid-nineteenth century by homeopaths, and by the end of that century was taken up by psychologists and psychical researchers. But it was not until the 1930s that the potential of blind techniques combined with no-treatment control groups in clinical trials was widely recognized by mainstream medical researchers, and only after World War II did blind assessment in randomized controlled trials became a standard and normative technique. In medicine and psychology, blind experimentation began as a deterrent against the unconventional, but its general importance has been recognized for orthodox research; it has been internalized. Although researchers in unconventional medicine and their skeptical critics have been aware of the possible effects of expectation and belief for over two hundred years, and conventional medical researchers and psychologists for decades, how widely has this awareness spread throughout the scientific community? What about the beliefs and expectations of experimenters in other branches of science? No one seems to know how important they might be. There seems to be a tacit assumption that scientists in orthodox fields of inquiry are immune from the general principle that "beliefs, desires and expectations can influence, often subconsciously, how we observe and interpret things". In this article I attempt by means of 2 surveys to quantify the attention or inattention to possible experimenter effects in different fields of science. The first surveys was directed toward published reports in the scientific literature to see how frequently blind procedures were used in different branches of science. In the second survey, individuals in 11 British university science departments were asked whether blind methodologies were practiced or taught. The results reveal that blind methodologies are rarely if ever practiced or taught in physics, chemistry, or much of biology. I conclude this article by proposing a simple experimental procedure for assessing the importance of experimenter expectancy effects in areas where their potential influence has so far been neglected. SURVEY METHODS Literature survey A survey of scientific literature was conducted between October 1996 and April 1998. Leading journals were selected in different fields of experimental science, and the most recent numbers available in libraries were examined. The Contents pages were photocopied, and were used for recording the category of each paper listed on them. The papers were then examined in detail, with particular attention to the Methods sections, and classified into one of the following categories: 1. Not applicable: papers that did not involve experimental investigations, for example theoretical or review articles.

2. Blind or double-blind methodologies used. 3. Blind or double-blind methodologies not used. On the basis of this information, the total number of experimental papers surveyed in each journal and the number involving blind techniques were listed, as shown in Table 1. This literature survey was carried out by myself and by Dr Amanda Jacks. Table 1 Numbers of papers reviewed and the number involving blind or double-blind methodologies in a range of scientific journals.* Journal Volumes (and Parts) Number of Papers Blind Methods Physical Sciences Journal of the American Chemical Society 118 (39-41) 86 0 Journal of Applied Physics 80 (11) 76 0 Journal of Physics: Condensed Matter 8 (48-9) 75 0 Totals 237 0 Biological Sciences Biochemical Journal 318-9 (1-3;1) 191 0 Cell 87 (4-5) 29 0 Heredity 76 (1-5) 58 0 Journal of Experimental Botany 46-7 (295-302) 132 0 Journal of Molecular Biology 262 (2-5) 48 0 Journal of Physiology 497-8 (1;1-2) 145 4 Nature 383-4 (6600-10) 108 0 Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (US) 93 (22-3) 203 3 Totals 914 7 (0.8%) Medical Sciences American Journal of Medicine 103-104 (5-6; 1-3) 45 22 Annals of Internal Medicine 128 (2-7) 41 12 British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology 42 (3-5) 49 4 British Medical Journal 313 (7061-6) 53 2 New England Journal of Medicine 338 (9-16) 39 15 Totals 227 55 (24.2%) Psychology and Animal Behaviour Animal Behaviour 52 (1-4) 72 2 British Journal of Psychology 87 (1-3) 21 0 Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 125 (1-3) 23 2 Human Perception and Performance 22 (5-6) 27 3 Totals 143 7 (4.9%) Parapsychology Journal of the Society for Psychical Research (1993-6) 59-61 (830-45) 14 11 Journal of Parapsychology (1994-6) 58 (3)- 60 (2) 13 12 Totals 27 23 (85.2%) *Only papers reporting experimental results were included in this survey; theoretical papers and review articles were excluded. All publications appeared from 1996 through 1998 unless otherwise indicated. Survey of university science departments

A survey of science departments at 11 British Universities was carried out by telephone by my Research Assistant Jane Turney, an experienced interviewer. She spoke either to professors in these departments, or to other members of the academic teaching staff. She first introduced herself and explained that she was carrying out a survey on the use of blind techniques in the hard sciences, and asked them two questions: 1. Do you ever use blind experimental methodologies in your department? 2. Are students taught about blind methodologies and experimenter effects in general? The results of this survey were tabulated and are shown in Table 2. Table 2 A Survey of Science Departments* Members of the academic staff were interviewed by telephone and asked the following questions: 1. Do you ever use blind experimental methodologies in your department? 2. Are students taught about blind methodologies and experimenter effects in general? Department Number Surveyed Blind Methods Used Blind Methods Taught Physical Sciences Inorganic Chemistry 7 0 0 Organic Chemistry 7 0 0 Physics 9 1 1 Biological Sciences Biochemistry 10 1 2 Molecular Biology 6 1 0 Genetics 8 4 4 Physiology 8 6 6 *(Results of a survey of science departments carried out between December 1996 and February 1997 at the following British universities: Bristol, Cambridge, Edinburgh, Exeter, Imperial College (London), Manchester, Newcastle, Oxford, Reading, Sheffield, University College (London)) RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The widespread neglect of possible experimenter effects The use of blind procedures in different branches of science gives a measure of the importance researchers in that field attach to experimenter effects. In Table 1, I summarize the results of a survey of papers published recently in a range of scientific journals. In the physical sciences, no blind experiments were found among the 237 papers reviewed. In the biological sciences, there were 7 blind experiments out of 914 (0.8%); in psychology and animal behavior, 7 out of 143 (4.9%); and in the medical sciences, 55 out of 227 (24.2%); By far the highest proportion, 23 out of 27 papers (85.2%), was in parapsychology. In the medical journals, out of the 55 reports involving blind methods, only 25 (11.0% of the total) represented double-blind trials. The other 30 employed single-blind methods, with one or more of the investigators carrying out blind evaluations or analyses. The majority of the papers did not involve blind methods. Confirming the findings of the literature survey, the survey of science departments at 11 British Universities confirmed that blind procedures are rare in most branches of the physical and biological sciences. They were neither used nor taught in 22 out of 23 physics and chemistry departments, or in 14 out of 16 biochemistry and molecular biology departments (Table 2). By contrast, blind methodologies were practiced and taught in 4 out of 8 genetics departments, and in 6 out of 8 physiology departments. In most of these departments they are used occasionally rather than routinely, and are mentioned only briefly in lectures. When academic scientists were interviewed for this survey, some did not know what was meant by the phrase "blind methodology". Most were aware of blind techniques, but thought that they were necessary only in clinical research or psychology. They believed that their principal purpose was to avoid biases introduced by human subjects, rather than by experimenters. The commonest view expressed by physical and biological scientists was that blind methodologies are unnecessary outside psychology and medicine because "nature itself is blind", as one professor put it. Some admitted the theoretical possibility of bias by experimenters, but thought it of little importance in practice. And one chemist added, "Science is difficult enough as it is without making it even harder by not knowing what you are working on." Only in exceptional cases are blind techniques used routinely. This survey revealed 3 examples. All 3 involved industrial contracts, according to which the university scientists were required to analyze or evaluate coded samples without knowing their identity.

Limitations on the use of blind methodologies In the biological and physical sciences, the fact that almost no published research involves blind techniques reflects the fact that researchers, reviewers and journal editors in these fields assume they are unnecessary. They are not part of their scientific culture. There may be many situations in which they would be desirable and informative, but practically no attention has yet been paid to this possibility.. By contrast, blind methods are part of the culture of medical research, both conventional and unconventional. In this context, the publication of so many papers in mainstream medical journals that do not involve blind techniques indicates that these methods are not always appropriate or applicable. I have not carried out an analysis of the situations where blind methods are not used in mainstream medical research, because my primary focus was on the biological and physical sciences. But an analysis of this kind would probably be very illuminating. One kind of medical research not involving blind methods is the study of case histories. In a recent paper in this journal, Lukoff and colleagues (note 6) observed that: "The highly regarded randomized controlled clinical trial, though often powerful and useful, is neither feasible nor ideal for understanding the effects of many unconventional treatment approaches". My survey suggests that blinded, randomized, controlled trials are also neither feasible nor ideal for studying many conventional treatment approaches. A SIMPLE EXPERIMENT TO TEST FOR EXPERIMENTER EFFECTS Although most "hard" scientists take it for granted that blind techniques are unnecessary in their own field of study, this assumption is so fundamental that it deserves to be tested empirically.(note 7) In all branches of experimental science we can ask: do the expectations of researchers introduce a bias, conscious or unconscious, into the way they carry out the experimental procedures, make their observations or select data? I propose the following procedure to test this matter. Take a typical experiment that involves a test sample and a control - for example the comparison of an inhibited enzyme with an uninhibited control in a biochemical experiment. Then carry out the experiment both under open conditions, and also under blind conditions, in which the samples are labeled A and B. In student practical classes, for instance, half the class would do the experiment blind. The other half would know which sample is which, as usual. If no significant experimenter effects are found in such tests, then for the first time there will be evidence to support the belief that blind techniques are unnecessary. On the other hand, significant differences between the results under open and blind conditions would reveal the existence of experimenter effects. Further research would then be needed to find out whether the experimenters' expectations were influencing experimental systems themselves, or merely the way that the data were recorded or selected. The more independent investigations, the better. It cannot be healthy for the supposed objectivity of regular science to rest on untested assumptions.(note 8) This is an inquiry in which the critical skills of skeptics could play a major role. The use of blind methodologies, pioneered by skeptics in the field of unconventional medicine, has now been internalized within medicine and psychology, resulting in improved rigor and a more sophisticated awareness of the effects of experimenter bias. The so-called hard sciences have largely escaped skeptical inquiry, but there seems no good reason why they should continue to be granted this immunity.(note 9) Perhaps it will turn out, after all, that "hard" scientists need not bother with blind techniques. They may indeed be exceptions to the principle that "our beliefs, desires and expectations can influence, often subconsciously, how we observe and interpret things." On the other hand, they may be like everybody else, including researchers in psychology and medicine. Acknowledgments I am grateful to Dr Amanda Jacks for her help with the literature review and to Jane Turney for carrying out the university survey. This work was supported by the Institute of Noetic Sciences, Sausalito, CA and the Lifebridge Foundation of New York. References 1. Mussachia, M. Objectivity and repeatability in science. Skeptical Inquirer 1995; 19 (6): 33-35, 56. 2. Rosenthal, R. Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research. New York: John Wiley; 1976. 3. Roberts, A.H., Kewman, D.G., Mercier, L. & Hovell, H. (1993) The power of nonspecific effects in healing: implications for psychosocial and biological treatments. Clinical Psychology Review 13, 375. 4. White, L., Tursky, B. & Schwartz, G. (eds) Placebo: Theory, Research and Mechanisms. New York: Guilford Press; 1985. 5. Kaptchuck, T.J. Intentional ignorance: a history of blind assessment in medicine. Bulletin of the History of Medicine 1998; in the press. 6. Lukoff, D., Edwards, D. and Miller, M. The case study as a scientific method for researching alternative therapies. Alternative Therapies in Health and Medicine 1998; 4: 44-52.

7. Sheldrake, R. Seven Experiments that Could Change the World. London: Fourth Estate;1994. 8. Sheldrake R. Experimenter effects in scientific research: how widely are they neglected? J Sci Exploration 1998;12(1):73-78 9. Sheldrake R. Could experimenter effects occur in the physical and biological sciences? Skeptical Inquirer 1998;22(3):57-58