Error-related negativity predicts academic performance

Similar documents
Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

The Devil You Know. Neuroticism Predicts Neural Response to Uncertainty. Research Report. Jacob B. Hirsh and Michael Inzlicht. University of Toronto

Alterations in Error-Related Brain Activity and Post-Error Behavior Over Time

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Cognition. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Wim Notebaert a, *, Femke Houtman a, Filip Van Opstal a, Wim Gevers b, Wim Fias a, Tom Verguts a

Sensitivity of Electrophysiological Activity from Medial Frontal Cortex to Utilitarian and Performance Feedback

ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing

The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring Predictable Dissociations Between the Error-Related Negativity and N2

Biological Psychology

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions

Running on Empty. Neural Signals for Self-Control Failure. Research Report. Michael Inzlicht 1 and Jennifer N. Gutsell 2

Working memory capacity and task goals modulate error-related ERPs

Acta Psychologica. Conflict and error adaptation in the Simon task. Wim Notebaert, Tom Verguts. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Error-preceding brain activity: Robustness, temporal dynamics, and boundary conditions $

Reliability of error-related brain activity

When things look wrong: Theta activity in rule violation

UC San Diego UC San Diego Previously Published Works

Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update

Conflict-Monitoring Framework Predicts Larger Within-Language ISPC Effects: Evidence from Turkish-English Bilinguals

Running head: LINKING COGNITIVE CONTROL & STRESS REGULATION. Linking Cognitive Control & Stress Regulation:

Relating cognitive and affective theories of the errorrelated

The Neural Basis of Error Detection: Conflict Monitoring and the Error-Related Negativity

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

A Computational Model of Anterior Cingulate Function in Speeded Response Tasks: Effects of Frequency, Sequence, and Conflict

Amy Kruse, Ph.D. Strategic Analysis, Inc. LCDR Dylan Schmorrow USN Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency

Journal of Research in Personality

Journal of Psychophysiology An International Journal

Functional lesions and human action monitoring: combining repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and event-related brain potentials

Running head: AFFECTIVE EVALUATION OF ACTIONS

Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Biological Psychology xxx (2014) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Biological Psychology

Perceptual properties of feedback stimuli influence the feedback-related negativity in the flanker gambling task

Biological Psychology

Biological Psychology

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Drink alcohol and dim the lights: The impact of cognitive deficits on medial frontal cortex function

1: Institute of Psychology, Eötvös Loránd University, Izabella u. 46., H-1064, Budapest,

Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Medial frontal cortex function: An introduction and overview

The Influence of Motivational Salience on Attention Selection: An ERP Investigation

Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process

Interactions of Focal Cortical Lesions With Error Processing: Evidence From Event-Related Brain Potentials

Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: the importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring

When Things Are Better or Worse than Expected: The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Allocation of Processing Resources

Frontal midline theta and N200 amplitude reflect complementary information about expectancy and outcome evaluation

The Effects of Social Exclusion on the ERN and the Cognitive Control of Action Monitoring

Electrophysiological correlates of error correction

Cardiac concomitants of performance monitoring: Context dependence and individual differences

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

To Bet or Not to Bet? The Error Negativity or Error-related Negativity Associated with Risk-taking Choices

Proactive and reactive control during emotional interference and its relationship to trait anxiety

Emotion Down-Regulation Diminishes Cognitive Control: A Neurophysiological Investigation

Neural and Behavioral Measures of Error- Related Cognitive Control Predict Daily Coping With Stress

State sadness reduces neural sensitivity to nonrewards versus rewards Dan Foti and Greg Hajcak

IS THE HIGH PROBABILITY OF TYPE II ERROR AN ISSUE IN ERROR AWARENESS ERP STUDIES?

Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra

Patricia L. Davies Department of Occupational Therapy Colorado State University

Electrophysiological Analysis of Error Monitoring in Schizophrenia

Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Are all medial f rontal n egativities created equal? Toward a richer empirical basis for theories of action monitoring

The Multi-Source Interference Task: The Effect of Randomization

Hierarchical Control over Effortful Behavior by Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Clay Holroyd. Department of Psychology University of Victoria

RUNNING HEAD: COGNITIVE EFFORT AND REWARD VALUATION DURING EXTENDED TASK PERFORMANCE

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

CARDIORESPIRATORY FITNESS AND ACUTE AEROBIC EXERCISE EFFECTS ON NEUROELECTRIC AND BEHAVIORAL MEASURES OF ACTION MONITORING

The feedback correct-related positivity: Sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback

Cognition 117 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Cognition. journal homepage:

The neural consequences of flip-flopping: The feedbackrelated negativity and salience of reward prediction

Outcome expectancy and not accuracy determines posterror slowing: ERP support

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

NeuroImage. Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning

Hypothesis Testing Strategy in Wason s Reasoning Task

The Anterior Cingulate and Error Avoidance

Neural Markers of Religious Conviction Michael Inzlicht, 1 Ian McGregor, 2 Jacob B. Hirsh, 1 and Kyle Nash 2

1 Performance monitoring and anxiety Running head: Performance monitoring and anxiety

Cognitive Control in the Service of Self-Regulation

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

Increasing negative emotions by reappraisal enhances subsequent cognitive control: A combined behavioral and electrophysiological study

Mechanism of Alcohol Related Disruption of the Error Monitoring and Processing System

Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex

Prefrontal cingulate interactions in action monitoring

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Anxiety not only increases, but also alters early error-monitoring functions

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Alcohol impairs brain reactivity to explicit loss feedback

Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control

Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency

The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making

Time-frequency theta and delta measures index separable components of feedback processing in a gambling task

Distinct Frontal Networks Are Involved in Adapting to Internally and Externally Signaled Errors

Early error detection is generic, but subsequent adaption to errors is not:

The Relationship Between Briefly Induced Affect and Cognitive Control Processes: An Event- Related Potential (ERP) Study

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude

Stimulus and Response-Related Conflict Processing in the Human Brain

Transcription:

Psychophysiology, 47 (2010), 192 196. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright r 2009 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00877.x BRIEF REPORT Error-related negativity predicts academic performance JACOB B. HIRSH and MICHAEL INZLICHT Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Abstract Activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) has been linked to the processes of error detection and conflict monitoring, along with the subsequent engagement of cognitive-control mechanisms. The error-related negativity (ERN) is an electrophysiological signal associated with this ACC monitoring process, occurring approximately 100 ms after an error is made. The current study examined the possibility that individual differences in ERN magnitude would predict performance outcomes related to cognitive control. Undergraduate students completed a color-naming Stroop task while their neural activity was recorded via electroencephalogram. Results indicated that a larger ERN following errors was significantly correlated with better academic performance as measured by official student transcripts. A greater ability to monitor performance and engage cognitive-control mechanisms when needed thus appears associated with improved real-world performance. Descriptors: Anterior cingulate cortex, Error-related negativity, Electrophysiology, Cognitive control, Academic performance The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) has been implicated in performance monitoring and conflict detection in humans (Carter et al., 1998; MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger, & Carter, 2000; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, 2004). During situations of high response conflict, activity in the ACC engages the cognitive-control mechanisms of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), subsequently improving behavioral regulation (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Gehring & Knight, 2000; Kerns et al., 2004). An electrophysiological signal associated with this process is the error-related negativity (ERN), a negative polarity wave peaking approximately 100 ms after making an error (Gehring & Willoughby, 2002; Holroyd & Coles, 2002; Yeung, Botvinick, & Cohen, 2004). Recent analyses using twin data indicate that approximately 47% of the variance in ERN magnitude is due to shared genetic factors (Anokhin, Golosheykin, & Heath, 2008), suggesting that the ERN has trait-like properties. From an individual differences perspective, a larger ERN should reflect improved action monitoring and more effective engagement of top-down cognitive-control mechanisms in the PFC (Hester, Fassbender, & Garavan, 2004; Pailing, Segalowitz, Dywan, & Davies, 2002). Improved PFC function, in turn, relates to a greater capacity for control (Kane & Engle, 2002; Miller, 2000), allowing for the topdown regulation of ongoing behavioral processes. Differences in Grants from the Social Science and Humanities Research Council and from the Canada Foundation for Innovation supported this research. Address reprint requests to: Jacob B. Hirsh, Department of Psychology, Sidney Smith Hall, 100 St. George St., Toronto, ON M5S 3G3, Canada. E-mail: jacob.hirsh@utoronto.ca 192 the ability to monitor performance and engage these control mechanisms when needed, as reflected by the ERN, should theoretically relate to important self-regulatory differences. Indeed, larger ERNs correlate with reduced impulsivity (Potts, George, Martin, & Barratt, 2006) and improved emotional regulation (Compton et al., 2008). Over time and across a large number of situations, differences in the ability to monitor and regulate one s behavior can potentially manifest themselves in a variety of different life outcomes (Barkley, 2001; Carver & Scheier, 1998; Robinson, 2007). In the current study, we examined the real-world consequences of effective performance monitoring by correlating the magnitude of the ERN with undergraduate academic performance. It was hypothesized that a greater ability to recruit cognitive-control processes, as reflected in a larger ERN, would be associated with better academic performance. Method Participants Participants were 31 undergraduate students at the University of Toronto Scarborough (14 female). Right-handed participants were selected to avoid physiological differences due to brain laterality. The average age was 19.45 (SD 5 3.08, range 5 18 to 33). Procedure Participants completed an informed consent form, along with a demographics questionnaire detailing their age, gender, and the number of years they had been speaking English. Electrophysiological responses were then measured via electroencephalogram (EEG) as participants completed a standard color-naming Stroop task. Color words were presented in colors that either matched or conflicted with the semantic meaning of the words.

ERN & academic performance 193 Participants were instructed to press one of four colored buttons on a response box that corresponded to the font color of the stimulus word (red, green, blue, or yellow). Each word appeared for 200 ms, with a maximum response window of 800 ms. An inter-trial interval of 1000 ms was used. A practice session preceded 5 blocks of 48 trials each (32 congruent, 16 incongruent). Students granted us permission to access their academic transcripts during the consent process. Official transcripts were obtained from the Office of the Registrar, and all identifying information was removed prior to data analysis. All students had completed at least one year of studies, with a mean of 11 courses completed (SD 5 6.47). Academic performance was quantified as the average numeric course grade earned across all courses, as indicated by official transcripts. This measure of performance was used in all subsequent analyses. To ensure that this variable was not confounded by differences in course difficulty, we examined the relationship between the average difficulty of a student s classes (as reflected in the mean of his or her course averages) and the academic performance variable described above. No relationship was found between these variables, indicating that differences in course selection were not significantly influencing the obtained performance measure. Electrophysiological processing. EEG was recorded from 32 Ag/AgCl sintered electrodes in a stretch-lycra cap. Vertical eye movements (VEOG) were monitored via a supra- to sub-orbital bipolar montage. EEG and VEOG recordings were digitized at 560 Hz using ASA acquisition hardware (Advanced Neuro Technology B.V., Enschede, Netherlands) with average-ear references and a forehead ground. Electrode impedances were kept below 5 ko for all recordings. EEG was corrected for VEOG artifacts using the SOBI procedure (Tang, Liu, & Sutherland, 2005). Frequencies below 1 Hz and above 15 Hz were digitally filtered (96 db, zero-phase shift). The signal was baseline corrected by subtracting the average voltage occurring 400 to 200 ms pre-response. Movement artifacts were detected with a 75 mvand 175 mv threshold. Correct and incorrect trials were averaged separately with an epoch from 200 ms pre-response to 800 ms post-response. ERNs were quantified as the peak minimum deflection between 50 and 150 ms post-response at the frontal midline electrode (Fz). Results As predicted by models of self-regulation and cognitive control, academic performance was correlated with ERN magnitude, with better grades being associated with stronger (more negative) ERN responses, r 5.40, po.05 (see Figure 1 for scatter plot). This correlation was significant despite statistically controlling for the effects of participants gender, age, and experience with the English language. A bootstrapped correlation analysis using 10,000 samples computed a 95% confidence interval ranging from 0.13 to 0.60 (SE 5 0.12), indicating that the effect was not driven by outlying values. Similar results were obtained when analyzing the difference wave between correct and incorrect trials (r 5.37) or using the mean rather than minimum voltage between 50 ms and 150 ms post-response on error trials (r 5.34). The magnitude of the ERN was thus significantly associated with academic performance, and this effect was relatively stable across different methods of calculating the ERN. Post-error slowing was used as a behavioral indicator of cognitive control, calculated by subtracting each participant s average reaction time on post-correct trials (M 5 539 ms, SD 5 56 ms) from the average reaction time on post-error trials (M 5 556 ms, SD 5 68 ms) the average reaction time across all trials was 540 ms (SD 5 56 ms). As expected, reaction times following errors and correct trials were significantly different from each other, t(30) 5 2.46, po.05. In keeping with cognitive models of self-regulation, a larger ERN was associated with increased posterror slowing, r 5.47, po.05, theoretically reflecting the engagement of cognitive-control processes in the PFC following errors (Gehring & Knight, 2000). This post-error slowing was in turn associated with higher grades, r 5.42, po.05. A mediation analysis using the product of coefficients method recommended in MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, and Sheets (2002) confirmed that post-error slowing mediated the relationship between ERN size and academic performance (z 0 5 1.68, po.01). Overall accuracy rates for the Stroop task ranged from 81% to 98% (M 5 91.7%, SD 5 4.3%), but these rates did not significantly differ between post-error (M 5 92.4%, SD 5 8.2%) and post-correct (M 5 91.6%, SD 5 4.5%) trials. No relationship was found between Stroop error rates and the ERN or GPA. Table 1 displays a summary of the obtained correlations. Headmaps of the ERN revealed the expected frontocentral spatial distribution (see Figure 2). A single equivalent current dipole model of the post-error ERPs identified a dorsal ACC source (PAN coordinates [mm]: x 5 12.7, y 5 3.3, z 5 37.2; dipole strength 5 54.74 nam), accounting for 98.4% of the signal variance. Although EEG lacks spatial precision, the obtained source in the dorsal ACC implicates the cognitive aspects of ACC function (as opposed to the more rostral, emotional aspects; Bush, Luu, & Posner, 2000). Discussion Academic performance is a gateway to many important life outcomes, influencing the career options that are available to a student. At the broader societal level, achievement in academic domains plays a vital role in sustaining cultural and scientific innovation. The current study suggests that individuals who are better able to monitor their performance and engage cognitivecontrol mechanisms when needed enjoy greater success in undergraduate programs. It further suggests that the ERN can potentially be employed as a neural marker of this ability. Figure 1. Scatter plot of student grades and ERN magnitude. Both scales reflect z-scores of the variables.

194 J. B. Hirsh & M. Inzlicht Figure 2. The relation between academic success and the ERN. (A) Event-related potentials at Fz on error trials for individuals with high and low Grade Point Averages, as derived from a tertiary split of the sample. (B) Spatial distribution of the ERN, quantified as the peak minimum voltage deflection occurring between 50 and 150 ms after an error. (C) Headmap of correlations between GPA and ERN magnitude. (D) Source localization indicates an anterior cingulate generator for the ERN. Table 1. Correlations Between ERN, Grades, Post-Error Slowing, and Stroop Errors ERN Grades PES Grades.40 n PES.47 n.42 n Errors.03.07.24 n po.05, two-tailed. PES 5 Post-Error Slowing; Errors 5 Number of errors on Stroop task. The current results are in keeping with previous research linking the ERN to self-regulatory processes. For instance, a larger ERN has been associated with better stress regulation (Compton et al., 2008), reduced impulsivity (Potts et al., 2006; Ruchsow, Spitzer, Gro n, Grothe, & Kiefer, 2005), and a lower incidence of externalizing disorders (Hall, Bernat, & Patrick, 2007). Given that the ERN appears related to the engagement of self-regulatory control systems, its relationship with academic performance may come as no surprise. Indeed, self-regulatory processes are thought to play an important role in the maintenance of academic goals (Covington, 2000; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). It is important to note that, while the magnitude of the ERN is substantially heritable, approximately half of the variance is accounted for by environmental factors (Anokhin et al., 2008). Thus, while ERN size is related to academic performance, it does not necessarily reflect an immutable cognitive ability. It is certainly possible that the engagement of cognitive-control mechanisms associated with the ERN can be improved through training, something that should be explored in future research. On a related note, it is not yet clear how the ERN is related to general mental ability, which is one of the most effective predictors of academic performance (Higgins, Peterson, Lee, & Pihl, 2007; Neisser et al., 1996). One possibility is that some of the variance captured by the ERN overlaps with standard tests of intelligence, potentially explaining the observed association with academic performance. Contrary to this explanation, however, is the fact that performance on intelligence tests is related more to lateral PFC activity (Duncan et al., 2000; Gray, Chabris, & Braver, 2003), whereas the ERN is associated with ACC activity (Botvinick et al., 2001; Kerns et al., 2004). Although these brain regions interact, they appear to support two distinct aspects of cognitive control, with the ACC serving an evaluative function which can then signal the need for strategic, executive processes in the lateral PFC. An implication of this dissociation between the evaluative and executive components of cognitive control is that these systems might interact to predict performance outcomes. It is possible, for instance, that intelligence would moderate the relationship between the ERN and academic performance, such that engaging the cognitive-control systems of the lateral PFC would only lead to performance improvements when enough cognitive resources were available for deployment. An error-monitoring system would be unlikely to predict performance outcomes if there were insufficient cognitive resources for making behavioral corrections after error detection. An alternative interpretation of the current results is that the ERN may be reflecting increased motivation to perform well on

ERN & academic performance 195 the task, rather than the capacity to engage cognitive-control mechanisms. Indeed, previous research has found that increasing the motivational salience of a task leads to larger ERN magnitudes (Hajcak, Moser, Yeung, & Simons, 2005). Individuals who display a larger ERN on the Stroop task may attain better grades because their motivation levels are generally higher, making them more likely to engage cognitive-control mechanisms and adjust their behavioral responses following errors. This view is supported by research in which highly conscientious individuals were more likely to demonstrate a consistently large ERN, whereas less conscientious respondents only displayed a large ERN when additional monetary incentives were provided to motivate accurate performance (Pailing & Segalowitz, 2004). Importantly, conscientiousness is the best personality predictor of academic achievement, independent of cognitive ability (Higgins et al., 2007; Poropat, 2009), and is also related to higher overall levels of performance motivation (Judge & Ilies, 2002). It is thus possible that the size of the ERN is reflecting some of the performance motivation associated with conscientiousness, which would explain the observed relationship with academic outcomes. Future research could expand on these possibilities by separately examining the contributions of cognitive, motivational, and personality factors. It should also be noted that while larger ERN responses were associated with improved performance outcomes in the population examined in the current study, large ERNs are sometimes associated with dysfunctional behavior. In particular, large ERNs have been previously associated with anxiety, especially among clinical populations (Olvet & Hajcak, 2008). It is thus possible that larger ERNs may not always reflect improved selfregulation and performance, particularly at the high ends of the distribution. There may instead be an inverse U-shaped relationship with performance, much like the classic theory of optimum arousal (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908). While the current study provides a promising first inquiry into this topic, further research is needed to more clearly specify the relationship between ERN magnitude, behavioral indicators of cognitive control, and academic performance. In particular, future studies would benefit from using larger sample sizes. The sample size used in the current study is comparable to those used in previous ERN studies, but larger samples would allow for more detailed statistical analyses, including a closer look at potential moderating variables that had somewhat restricted range in this sample (e.g., age and gender). Similarly, while the obtained confidence intervals for the correlation between the ERN and academic performance did not approach zero, a larger sample would help to provide a narrower estimate of the true effect size, as the obtained intervals span a moderately large range. Overall, the current study provides further evidence for the real-world importance of effective performance monitoring. The pursuit of academic goals requires the continual self-regulation of learning, motivation, and cognitive effort. Individual differences in the extent to which self-regulatory resources can be mobilized in response to errors appear to be an important predictor of success in this domain. REFERENCES Anokhin, A., Golosheykin, S., & Heath, A. (2008). Heritability of frontal brain function related to action monitoring. Psychophysiology, 45, 524 534. Barkley, R. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 1 29. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624 652. Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 215 222. Carter, C., Braver, T., Barch, D., Botvinick, M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280, 747. Carver, C., & Scheier, M. (1998). On the Self-Regulation of Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Compton, R. J., Robinson, M. D., Ode, S., Quandt, L. C., Fineman, S. L., & Carp, J. (2008). Error-monitoring ability predicts daily stress regulation. Psychological Science, 19, 702 708. Covington, M. (2000). Goal theory, motivation, and school achievement: An integrative review. Annual Reviews in Psychology, 51, 171 200. Duncan, J., Seitz, R., Kolodny, J., Bor, D., Herzog, H., Ahmed, A., et al. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457 460. Gehring, W. J., & Knight, R. T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 516 520. Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295, 2279 2282. Gray, J., Chabris, C., & Braver, T. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316 322. Hajcak, G., Moser, J., Yeung, N., & Simons, R. (2005). On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology, 42, 151 160. Hall, J., Bernat, E., & Patrick, C. (2007). Externalizing psychopathology and the error-related negativity. Psychological Science, 18, 326 333. Hester, R., Fassbender, C., & Garavan, H. (2004). Individual differences in error processing: A review and reanalysis of three event-related fmri studies using the GO/NOGO task. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 986 994. Higgins, D. M., Peterson, J. B., Lee, A. G. M., & Pihl, R. O. (2007). Prefrontal cognitive ability, intelligence, Big Five personality, and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 298 319. Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the errorrelated negativity. Psychological Review, 109, 679 709. Judge, T., & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to performance motivation: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 797 807. Kane, M., & Engle, R. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in workingmemory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 637 671. Kerns, J., Cohen, J., MacDonald, A., Cho, R., Stenger, V., & Carter, C. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science, 303, 1023 1026. MacDonald, A., Cohen, J., Stenger, V., & Carter, C. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835. MacKinnon, D., Lockwood, C., Hoffman, J., West, S., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83 104. Miller, E. K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 59 65. Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T., Boykin, A., Brody, N., Ceci, S., et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77 101.

196 J. B. Hirsh & M. Inzlicht Olvet, D., & Hajcak, G. (2008). The error-related negativity (ERN) and psychopathology: Toward an endophenotype. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1343 1354. Pailing, P., & Segalowitz, S. (2004). The error-related negativity as a state and trait measure: Motivation, personality, and ERPs in response to errors. Psychophysiology, 41, 84 95. Pailing, P., Segalowitz, S., Dywan, J., & Davies, P. (2002). Error negativity and response control. Psychophysiology, 39, 198 206. Pintrich, P., & De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33 40. Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 322 338. Potts, G., George, M., Martin, L., & Barratt, E. (2006). Reduced punishment sensitivity in neural systems of behavior monitoring in impulsive individuals. Neuroscience Letters, 397, 130 134. Ridderinkhof, K., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443 447. Robinson, M. (2007). Gassing, braking, and self-regulating: Error selfregulation, well-being, and goal-related processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 1 16. Ruchsow, M., Spitzer, M., Grön, G., Grothe, J., & Kiefer, M. (2005). Error processing and impulsiveness in normals: Evidence from eventrelated potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 317 325. Tang, A., Liu, J., & Sutherland, M. (2005). Recovery of correlated neuronal sources from EEG: The good and bad ways of using SOBI. Neuroimage, 28, 507 519. Yerkes, R., & Dodson, J. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 18, 459 482. Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111, 931 959. (Received February 5, 2009; Accepted March 5, 2009)