Spatial unmasking in aided hearing-impaired listeners and the need for training

Similar documents
Evaluation of a Danish speech corpus for assessment of spatial unmasking

Lateralized speech perception in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners and its relationship to temporal processing

Influence of acoustic complexity on spatial release from masking and lateralization

Speech intelligibility in simulated acoustic conditions for normal hearing and hearing-impaired listeners

Predicting the benefit of binaural cue preservation in bilateral directional processing schemes for listeners with impaired hearing

Sound localization psychophysics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

OpenSound Navigator for Oticon Opn Custom Instruments

Minimum Audible Angles Measured with Simulated Normally-Sized and Oversized Pinnas for Normal-Hearing and Hearing- Impaired Test Subjects

Adapting bilateral directional processing to individual and situational influences

Are hearing aids the better rehabilitative choice when compared to PSAPs? On speech-intelligibility and soundquality,

Binaural processing of complex stimuli

Localization in speech mixtures by listeners with hearing loss

Effect of microphone position in hearing instruments on binaural masking level differences

Oticon Agil success 2011 An International Survey

Lundbeck, Micha; Grimm, Giso; Hohmann, Volker ; Laugesen, Søren; Neher, Tobias

Brian D. Simpson Veridian, 5200 Springfield Pike, Suite 200, Dayton, Ohio 45431

Individual factors in speech recognition with binaural multi-microphone noise reduction: Measurement and prediction

ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

Release from speech-on-speech masking by adding a delayed masker at a different location

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

Running head: HEARING-AIDS INDUCE PLASTICITY IN THE AUDITORY SYSTEM 1

Influence of multi-microphone signal enhancement algorithms on auditory movement detection in acoustically complex situations

NHS HDL(2004) 24 abcdefghijklm

EFFECT OF DIRECTIONAL STRATEGY ON AUDIBILITY OF SOUNDS IN THE ENVIRONMENT. Charlotte Jespersen, MA Brent Kirkwood, PhD Jennifer Groth, MA

3-D SOUND IMAGE LOCALIZATION BY INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES AND THE MEDIAN PLANE HRTF. Masayuki Morimoto Motokuni Itoh Kazuhiro Iida

Best Practice Protocols

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

Combating the Reverberation Problem

Revisiting the right-ear advantage for speech: Implications for speech displays

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Hearing in Noise Test in Subjects With Conductive Hearing Loss

Audiology Today MarApr2011

Spatial processing in adults with hearing loss

Release from informational masking in a monaural competingspeech task with vocoded copies of the maskers presented contralaterally

WIDEXPRESS. no.30. Background

Proven Benefits of Beltone CrossLink Directionality

Linking dynamic-range compression across the ears can improve speech intelligibility in spatially separated noise

The effect of binaural processing techniques on speech quality ratings of assistive listening devices in different room acoustics conditions

WIDEXPRESS THE WIDEX FITTING RATIONALE FOR EVOKE MARCH 2018 ISSUE NO. 38

Speech segregation in rooms: Effects of reverberation on both target and interferer

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Validation Studies. How well does this work??? Speech perception (e.g., Erber & Witt 1977) Early Development... History of the DSL Method

Study of perceptual balance for binaural dichotic presentation

Characterizing individual hearing loss using narrow-band loudness compensation

Manufacturers NAL-NL2 Fittings Fail Real-ear Verification

TOPICS IN AMPLIFICATION

Predicting Directional Hearing Aid Benefit for Individual Listeners

TOLERABLE DELAY FOR SPEECH PROCESSING: EFFECTS OF HEARING ABILITY AND ACCLIMATISATION

Sonia Grewal, Au.D Professional Education Manager Hearing HealthCare Providers 2017 Conference WIDEX CROS & BICROS

Evidence-based Design Leads to Remote Microphone Hearing Instrument Technology

Cochlear implant patients localization using interaural level differences exceeds that of untrained normal hearing listeners

Improvement in Aided Sound Localization with Open Earmolds : Observations in People with High-Frequency Hearing Loss

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

The role of low frequency components in median plane localization

Evidence based selection of hearing aids and features

Lindsay De Souza M.Cl.Sc AUD Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Using limitations of the auditory system to optimize hearing-aid design

Localization 103: Training BiCROS/CROS Wearers for Left-Right Localization

Masker-signal relationships and sound level

Discrimination and identification of azimuth using spectral shape a)

Speech recognition by bilateral cochlear implant users in a cocktail-party setting

Noise reduction in modern hearing aids long-term average gain measurements using speech

Factors That Influence Intelligibility in Multitalker Speech Displays

Unilateral Versus Bilateral Hearing Aid Fittings

Group Delay or Processing Delay

BINAURAL DICHOTIC PRESENTATION FOR MODERATE BILATERAL SENSORINEURAL HEARING-IMPAIRED

The role of periodicity in the perception of masked speech with simulated and real cochlear implants

Role of F0 differences in source segregation

Simultaneous multiple stimulation of the auditory steadystate response (ASSR)

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

Using 3d sound to track one of two non-vocal alarms. IMASSA BP Brétigny sur Orge Cedex France.

MedRx HLS Plus. An Instructional Guide to operating the Hearing Loss Simulator and Master Hearing Aid. Hearing Loss Simulator

Speech intelligibility in background noise with ideal binary time-frequency masking

Non-commercial use only

Audiogram+: GN Resound proprietary fitting rule

Having Two Ears Facilitates the Perceptual Separation of Concurrent Talkers for Bilateral and Single-Sided Deaf Cochlear Implantees

Going beyond directionality in noisy conversations with Dynamic Spatial Awareness

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 THE DUPLEX-THEORY OF LOCALIZATION INVESTIGATED UNDER NATURAL CONDITIONS

Audiology Services. Table of Contents. Audiology Services Guidelines : Hearing services

EXPECT More from Oticon AGIL. a new survey proves it!

Online self-help via controlled discussion forum

The role of high frequencies in speech localization

On the influence of interaural differences on onset detection in auditory object formation. 1 Introduction

HST.723J, Spring 2005 Theme 3 Report

C HAPTER FOUR. Audiometric Configurations in Children. Andrea L. Pittman. Introduction. Methods

Yun, I.J. M.Cl.Sc. (Aud) Candidate School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, U.W.O.

A new tool for subjective assessment of hearing aid performance: Analyses of Interpersonal Communication

Modeling individual loudness perception in cochlear implant recipients with normal contralateral hearing

Binaural advantages in users of bimodal and bilateral cochlear implant devices

A. SEK, E. SKRODZKA, E. OZIMEK and A. WICHER

INTERNATIONAL MATRIX TESTS. Reliable speech audiometry in noise

Masker location uncertainty reveals evidence for suppression of maskers in two-talker contexts

For hearing aid noise reduction, babble is not just babble

Estimating auditory filter bandwidth using distortion product otoacoustic emissions

Topics in Amplification DYNAMIC NOISE MANAGEMENT TM A WINNING TEAM

DSL v5 in Connexx 7 Mikael Menard, Ph.D., Philippe Lantin Sivantos, 2015.

Paediatric Amplification

Sound Localization in Multisource Environments: The Role of Stimulus Onset Asynchrony and Spatial Uncertainty

Transcription:

Spatial unmasking in aided hearing-impaired listeners and the need for training Tobias Neher, Thomas Behrens, Louise Kragelund, and Anne Specht Petersen Oticon A/S, Research Centre Eriksholm, Kongevejen 243, 3070 Snekkersten, Denmark Even though spatial unmasking (SU) in hearing-impaired subjects has been the subject of a number of studies, very little research seems to have been carried out under aided conditions, especially not for more complex speech-on-speech masking situations. As part of an earlier pilot study into aided SU, a group of test subjects were found to exhibit substantial training effects across different visits, despite some initial training. A new training program was therefore designed based on some findings from the perceptual learning and training literature. Nine elderly hearing-aid users with mild-to-moderate, sloping hearing losses were systematically trained in a speech-on-speech SU task. All subjects were bilaterally fitted and only tested with their own hearing aids. Using a new speech corpus suitable for speech-on-speech SU assessment, performance was then determined at two subsequent visits. Whilst there were substantial differences between test subjects, half of them showed SU as large as 10 db. Moreover, performance across the two visits was found to be much more stable. These results hint at the need for thorough training when elderly hearing-aid users are to be tested under complex listening conditions. INTRODUCTION It is well known that spatial hearing can help listeners achieve better speech intelligibility in complex listening situations such as cocktail parties where there are other talkers interfering with a target signal (e.g. Bronkhorst, 2000). In the laboratory, the contribution of spatial cues to speech intelligibility is usually quantified using an SU test paradigm. In such a paradigm, a target signal is presented together with one or more masker signals, whereby the target and masker(s) exhibit either the same or different spatial cues. For each condition, the Target-to-Masker Ratio (TMR) corresponding to a given level of speech recognition is then determined and the difference in TMR taken as a measure of the release from masking afforded by the availability of spatial differences. Whilst considerable research has dealt with the effects of hearing loss on SU [see (Durlach et al., 1981) for an overview], most of that work has used non-representative test signals. Moreover, few studies have been conducted under aided conditions. Consequently, this study was designed to quantify spatial release from speech-on-speech masking in elderly hearing-aid users. In addition, it aimed to find out if systematic training can lead to more stable subject performance over time. This additional aim was motivated by a previous study, as part of which a group of hearing-aid users (with Auditory signal processing in hearing-impaired listeners. 1st International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research (ISAAR 2007). T. Dau, J. M. Buchholz, J. M. Harte, T. U. Christiansen (Eds.). ISBN: 87-990013-1-4. Print: Centertryk A/S.

Tobias Neher, Thomas Behrens, Louise Kragelund, and Anne Specht Petersen hearing losses very similar to the ones of the subjects tested in this study) had been briefly task-trained immediately before the first SU measurement. Since that type of training was found inadequate to prevent substantial performance variability across different visits, a new training program was devised and tested. EXPERIMENTAL CONDITIONS Physical test set-up All training and testing was carried out under anechoic conditions. Four loudspeakers were positioned in the horizontal plane at 0, ±50 and 180 (cf. Fig. 1). The distance to the listening position was about 1.6 m. Below the frontal loudspeaker an LCD screen was hung that was used for displaying instructions. The subject was seated in a custom-made chair equipped with a head rest that was small enough, so that sound reaching a subject s ears from behind was not obstructed. The chair was adjusted as necessary to ensure that the subject s head was located precisely in the middle of the test set-up and that the subject was seated comfortably. The subject was instructed to move as little as possible whenever measurements were made, which was also checked by the experimenter with the help of a video monitoring system. Fig. 1: Physical test set-up and spatial test conditions. Spatial test conditions To quantify SU, speech intelligibility was measured for three spatial test conditions: (1) co-located, (2) displaced F-B, and (3) displaced L-R (cf. Fig. 1). In the co-located condition, three speech signals were presented simultaneously from the frontal loudspeaker. One of the signals served as the target (T) and the other two as maskers (M 1 and M 2 ). In the displaced F-B condition, the target stayed in front and the maskers were presented from behind. In this condition, only high-frequency monaural spatial cues (e.g. Middlebrooks and Green, 1991) were therefore potentially available to the subjects to spatially separate the target from the maskers. In the displaced L-R condition, the target came still from in front, whilst the maskers were presented from the left and right loudspeaker, respectively. In this condition, both interaural and monaural spatial cues were therefore 516

Spatial unmasking in aided hearing-impaired listeners and the need for training potentially available to the subjects. To allow the subjects to identify the target signal in the co-located condition, the first word of the target sentence (the call sign ) was displayed on the LCD screen. The subjects task was to repeat all five words of the target sentence (the call sign scores were not included when estimating speech recognition). Speech material The speech material used was a modified version of the DANTALE II speech corpus (Wagener et al., 2003). A detailed description of this new corpus can be found in the companion paper by Behrens et al. (2007). Briefly, it consists of a large set of Danish sentences, spoken by each of five trained female talkers, that all follow the form name verb numeral adjective object. A typical utterance would be Michael had eight yellow houses. Test subjects and hearing aids Nine experienced, bilateral hearing-aid users with symmetrical, sloping hearing losses (cf. Fig. 2) participated in this study. In terms of hearing loss averaged across the audiometric frequencies of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 khz ( 4FA-HL ), the subjects ranged from 41 to 58 db HL (μ = 48 db HL). In terms of age, they ranged from 51 to 80 yrs (μ = 69 yrs). All subjects used Oticon Syncro hearing aids that were either of the ITE, ITC or CIC type. These hearing aids have an output bandwidth of ca. 7 khz. BTE devices were deliberately excluded to ensure that the hearing-aid microphones were located as close to the ear-canal entrances as possible. Subjects were only tested with their own hearing aids. Prior to all testing the noise reduction and directionality systems were disabled, as these could have modified the masker signals in the displaced conditions. In terms of insertion gain, there was very good correspondence between prescribed and fitted values up to 4 khz; at 6 khz four subjects received amplification that was about 5 db lower than prescribed. Fig. 2: Audiometric data of test subjects. 517

Tobias Neher, Thomas Behrens, Louise Kragelund, and Anne Specht Petersen Estimation of spatial unmasking In order to estimate SU, 50%-correct speech intelligibility thresholds were measured for the three spatial conditions. SU was then calculated by taking the difference between the TMR corresponding to the 50%-correct threshold estimate obtained for the co-located condition and the TMR corresponding to the 50%-correct threshold estimate obtained for either the displaced F-B (SU F-B ) or the displaced L-R (SU L-R ) condition: (Eq. 1) (Eq. 2) The individual 50%-correct threshold estimates were extracted from psychometric functions that had been derived using the method of constant stimuli. Before the start of the actual SU measurement, all subjects completed a brief task brush-up that included a few easy TMRs, so that they could get used to the different conditions again. Next, 30 pre-trials were run per condition to get an indication of where each subject s threshold was likely to lie. The resultant data were then used to derive psychometric functions with the help of a maximum-likelihood estimation procedure. From these functions, a few suitably placed TMRs were extracted and another block of 30 trials was run per condition. Only the data from the pre-trials and subsequent trials were used to estimate the final thresholds. Training program The design of the training program was based on a gradual build-up of the task complexity, concluding with the actual SU task. It consisted of seven steps, which are summarised in Table 1. At the start of each step, the subject was provided with verbal and written instructions regarding the details of the subsequent stimuli. After each stimulus presentation, the experimenter provided verbal feedback to let the subject know if a given response was correct or not. If the subject had made a mistake, the experimenter gave instructions in terms of what aspect of the stimulus to pay attention to and played the same stimulus up to two times more, so that the subject could correct his response. Such feedback provision is known to play an important role in the successful outcome of training programs, including audiological ones (e.g. Sweetow and Palmer, 2005). Step Signal(s) What s new? No. of trials 1 T L/S introduced using one sentence; Task: Locate T 8 2 T Task: Repeat T, locate T 12 3 T Sentence changes across trials 12 4 T + M 1 M1 introduced; Task: Repeat T, locate M 1 12 5 T + M 1 + M 2 M2 introduced; Task: Repeat T, locate M 1 + M 2 12 6 T + M 1 + M 2 Target call sign changes; Task: Repeat T 15 7 T + M 1 + M 2 TMRs change 45 Table 1: Training program details. For each step, the signals presented, the new elements introduced, and the number of trials run are indicated (L/S = loudspeakers). 518

Spatial unmasking in aided hearing-impaired listeners and the need for training Experimental time course The training study comprised three separate visits per subject (cf. Fig. 3). At the first visit, all subjects were trained according to the program outlined in Table 1. At the second and third visit, SU performance was measured as described above. The decision to carry out the training program and first SU measurement at different visits was motivated by some research findings related to perceptual learning. Ortiz and Wright (2005) investigated the effect of a rest period on the performance of subjects who were trained in terms of an interaural time difference discrimination task. They were able to show that performance improved significantly if subjects were allowed to rest for about 9 hrs before the actual measurements were carried out compared to when the measurements were made immediately after the training program. Since for this study it was of interest to determine the longer-lasting effects of the training program on performance stability, a 1-week gap was chosen as the time interval between each pair of visits. Fig. 3: Experimental time course. RESULTS Even though nine test subjects had participated in this study, SU was immeasurable for one of them, as speech intelligibility could not be estimated in the co-located condition. The results reported below are therefore based on the data from the remaining eight subjects. Spatial unmasking performance Fig. 4 displays the mean and raw SU data of the hearing-impaired (HI) subjects as a function of displaced condition (F-B or L-R) and measurement number (#1 or #2). The SU data of nine (trained) normal-hearing (NH) subjects (25-46 yrs; μ = 37 yrs) are also shown for comparison [see (Behrens et al., 2007) for more details]. On average, the HI group obtained ca. 8 db less SU than the NH group, which is broadly in line with data reported by Marrone et al. (2007). In absolute terms, however, both subject groups exhibited several db more SU than the ones of Marrone et al., which could have been due to more training or smaller hearing losses, for example. Furthermore, SU L-R was generally larger than SU F-B, most probably because of the availability of interaural spatial information. Nevertheless, for the HI group there was also much more variation in SU L-R than in SU F-B. Whilst one subject obtained basically no SU L-R, four other subjects exhibited around 10 db of SU L-R and came therefore fairly close to NH performance. This finding is somewhat in contrast to other such data that were obtained using headphone-presented stimuli and that showed less SU in similarly impaired listeners (Kalluri and Edwards, 2007). The large variation in SU L-R furthermore indicates 519

Tobias Neher, Thomas Behrens, Louise Kragelund, and Anne Specht Petersen that the HI subjects differed substantially in terms of their ability to exploit left-right spatial information to achieve release from masking, but that they were more homogeneous in terms of the extent to which they could access high-frequency monaural cues. The NH group, on the other hand, exhibited little variation in SU L-R and much variation in SU F-B. This suggests that these subjects were all able to exploit left-right spatial information, but that they differed substantially in their ability to exploit frontback spatial cues, possibly because these cues are less robust or potent compared to left-right spatial information. Fig. 4: Mean and raw SU data from eight HI and nine NH subjects plotted as a function of displaced condition (F-B or L-R) and measurement number (#1 or #2). Effects of training The efficacy of the training program was assessed by computing training effect and test-retest standard deviation (STD) estimates. For both SUF-B and SUL-R there was an average improvement of just 0.5 db across the two visits (cf. Table 2). The testretest STDs corresponding to the two SU estimates were 2.1 and 1.5 db, respectively. These results compare favourably with those from the earlier SU pilot test, which had produced training effects of 0.9 and 1.6 db and test-retest STDs of 2.9 and 2.1 db, respectively. Hence, it can be concluded that subject performance in both SUF-B and SUL-R became more stable as a result of the new training. SU F-B SU L-R Mean #1 [db] 4.2 6.6 Mean #2 [db] 4.7 7.1 Training Effect [db] 0.5 0.5 Test-Retest STD [db] 2.1 1.5 Table 2: Training effect and test-retest STD estimates. 520

Spatial unmasking in aided hearing-impaired listeners and the need for training Effects of hearing loss The effects of hearing loss on SU performance were examined by computing product-moment correlations between the different SU measures as well as 4FA-HL and indices of low-frequency and high-frequency hearing ability. To that end, the average hearing loss across the audiometric frequencies of 0.125, 0.25, 0.5, and 0.75 khz ( 4FA-HL low ) as well as 3, 4, 6, and 8 khz ( 4FA-HL high ) was calculated for each subject. The results are shown in Table 3, where correlations significant at the p < 0.05 level have been marked with an asterisk. There are strong (negative) correlations for SUL-R but not for SUF-B. Moreover, SUL-R seems to be pre-dominantly correlated with hearing ability in the lower frequencies. One could speculate that this was because the subjects with worse low-frequency hearing thresholds were less able to exploit interaural time difference information, which is known to dominate spatial perception in the low frequencies (Wightman & Kistler, 1992). 4FA-HL 4FA-HL low 4FA-HL high SU F-B #1-0.01 0.05-0.42 SU F-B #2-0.02-0.15 0.36 SU L-R #1-0.92* -0.82* -0.61 SU L-R #2-0.94* -0.81* -0.60 Table 3: Product-moment correlations between the various SU estimates and 4FA-HL, 4FA-HL low, and 4FA-HL high (* = p < 0.05). To further illustrate the negative correlation between degree of hearing loss and SUL-R, Fig. 5 displays scatter plots of SUL-R #2 and 4FA-HL or 4FA-HL low, together with lines of best fit. (a) (b) Fig. 5: Scatter plots of SU L-R #2 and (a) 4FA-HL or (b) 4FA-HL low. (No systematic relationships between the various SU measurements and age, gender, hearing-aid type or high-frequency insertion gain were found.) 521

Tobias Neher, Thomas Behrens, Louise Kragelund, and Anne Specht Petersen CONCLUSIONS The results reported above show that even though, on average, elderly hearing-aid users obtain significantly less spatial release from speech-on-speech masking than NH subjects, some can come fairly close to NH performance. Furthermore, the results point towards a strong relationship between SUL-R and (low-frequency) hearing loss. SUF-B, in turn, is generally smaller than SUL-R and also more variable between NH and within HI subjects, which can probably be traced back to the (high-frequency) spatial cues being less robust or potent. In terms of changes in subject performance over time, this study indicates that thorough training can help stabilise the performance of elderly hearing-aid users that are tested under complex listening conditions. In view of the relatively small number of test subjects used, however, further research is needed to substantiate these findings as well as to separate the effects of hearing loss and aiding on SU performance. REFERENCES Behrens, T., Neher, T., and Burmand Johannesson, R. (2007). Evaluation of speech corpus for assessment of spatial release from masking, Proceedings of the International Symposium on Audiological and Auditory Research, Helsingør, Denmark, Aug. 29-31. Bronkhorst, A. W. (2000). The cocktail party phenomenon: A review of research on speech intelligibility in multiple-talker conditions, Acta Acust. Acust. 86, 117-128. Durlach, N. I., Thompson, C. L., and Colburn, H. S. (1981). Binaural interaction in impaired listeners: A review of past research, Audiol. 20, 181-211. Kalluri, S., and Edwards, B. (2007), Impact of hearing impairment and hearing aids on benefits due to binaural hearing, 19th International Congress on Acoustics, Madrid, Spain, Sept. 2-7. Marrone, N. L., Mason, C. R., and Kidd, G. Jr. (2007). Spatial release from speechon-speech masking in adult hearing aid users, 153rd Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, Salt Lake City, Utah, June 4-8. Middlebrooks, J. C., and Green, D. M. (1991). Sound localization by human listeners, Ann. Rev. Psychol. 42, 135-159. Ortiz, J. A., and Wright, B. A. (2005). Effects of different amounts of brief training and rest on the generalization of learning from interaural-level-difference to interaural-time-difference discrimination, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 117, 2561. Sweetow, R. and Palmer, C. V. (2005). Efficacy of individual auditory training in adults: A systematic review of the evidence, J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 16, 494-504. Wagener, K., Josvassen, J. L., and Ardenkjær, R. (2003), Design, evaluation and optimization of a Danish sentence test in noise, Int. J. Audiol. 42, 10-17. Wightman, F. L., and Kistler, D. J. (1992). The dominant role of low-frequency interaural time differences in sound localization, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91, 1648-1661. 522