Two Experiments on Co-located Mobile Groupware

Similar documents
Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Empirical Research Methods for Human-Computer Interaction. I. Scott MacKenzie Steven J. Castellucci

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Topics. Experiment Terminology (Part 1)

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

Factors Affecting Speed and Accuracy of Response Selection in Operational Environments

Post-response stimulation and the Simon effect: Further evidence of action effect integration

Where No Interface Has Gone Before: What Can the Phaser Teach Us About Label Usage in HCI?

Getting the Design Right Daniel Luna, Mackenzie Miller, Saloni Parikh, Ben Tebbs

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Designing A User Study

Separating Cue Encoding From Target Processing in the Explicit Task- Cuing Procedure: Are There True Task Switch Effects?

Human Computer Interaction - An Introduction

Interpreting Instructional Cues in Task Switching Procedures: The Role of Mediator Retrieval

Predictive Model for Group Selection Performance on Touch Devices

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Intelligent Object Group Selection

Supplementary experiment: neutral faces. This supplementary experiment had originally served as a pilot test of whether participants

You can use this app to build a causal Bayesian network and experiment with inferences. We hope you ll find it interesting and helpful.

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Posner s Attention Test

LATENCY OF INSTRUMENTAL RESPONSES AS A FUNCTION OF COMPATIBILITY WITH THE MEANING OF ELICITING VERBAL SIGNS i

Designing Psychology Experiments: Data Analysis and Presentation

Visual perception on large displays

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

Joint Simon effects for non-human co-actors

Lecture 18: Controlled experiments. April 14

Quantitative Evaluation

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

Title: Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choices

You can use this app to build a causal Bayesian network and experiment with inferences. We hope you ll find it interesting and helpful.

Running head: How large denominators are leading to large errors 1

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

The Logic of Data Analysis Using Statistical Techniques M. E. Swisher, 2016

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

How Far Away Is That? It Depends on You: Perception Accounts for the Abilities of Others

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

The Gaze Cueing Paradigm with Eye Tracking Background Set-up Lab

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2

Instructor Guide to EHR Go

Lesson 9: Two Factor ANOVAS

Correlating Trust with Signal Detection Theory Measures in a Hybrid Inspection System

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Go4orienteering: a revolution in orienteering exercises

Invariant Effects of Working Memory Load in the Face of Competition

Behavioural Processes

What Solution-Focused Coaches Do: An Empirical Test of an Operationalization of Solution-Focused Coach Behaviors

20. Experiments. November 7,

Thrive Hearing Control Application

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

Amoeba on the Axiovert Standalone (with XYZ Control and Media) Joyce Ma and Jackie Wong. February 2003

Overview. Video calling today. Who we are 8/21/12. Video Quality and Interoperability across Videophones Today and in the Future

Layout Geometry in Encoding and Retrieval of Spatial Memory

Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences

Psychology Research Process

Short article The role of response selection in sequence learning

CROS System Initial Fit Protocol

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

Experimental Research in HCI. Alma Leora Culén University of Oslo, Department of Informatics, Design

How to Conduct Direct Preference Assessments for Persons with. Developmental Disabilities Using a Multiple-Stimulus Without Replacement

Improving Individual and Team Decisions Using Iconic Abstractions of Subjective Knowledge

Salient Feature Coding in Response Selection. Circularity?

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

Thrive Hearing Control Application

AC : USABILITY EVALUATION OF A PROBLEM SOLVING ENVIRONMENT FOR AUTOMATED SYSTEM INTEGRATION EDUCA- TION USING EYE-TRACKING

Evaluation of CBT for increasing threat detection performance in X-ray screening

Dissociation of S-R Compatibility and Simon Effects With Mixed Tasks and Mappings

Background Information on Simulation Created for

Examining differences between two sets of scores

Representing others actions: just like one s own?

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Accidental Inversion During 3d Rotation With 2-dof Input Devices

Sanako Lab 100 STS USER GUIDE

Response preparation and code overlap in dual tasks

Effect of Positive and Negative Instances on Rule Discovery: Investigation Using Eye Tracking

Do you have to look where you go? Gaze behaviour during spatial decision making

Piloting the use of the Modified Attention Network Task in Children

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

Supporting the Mobile Notification Process through Tactile Cues Selected using a Paired Comparison Task

Analogical Inference

Unit 3: EXPLORING YOUR LIMITING BELIEFS

Behavioural Brain Research

Evaluating Tactile Feedback in Graphical User Interfaces

Perceptual Load in Different Regions of the Visual Field and its Effect on Attentional Selectivity. Hadas Marciano Advisor: Yaffa Yeshurun

Tapping on the Move: Fitts Law Under Mobile Conditions

myphonak app User Guide

QuickTIPS REMOTE CONTROL TRULINK FOR APPLE DEVICES VOLUME CHANGES MEMORY CHANGES. PRODUCT AVAILABILITY: Halo iq, Halo 2, and Halo Devices

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Last week: Introduction, types of empirical studies. Also: scribe assignments, project details

The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making

Perception Lie Paradox: Mathematically Proved Uncertainty about Humans Perception Similarity

User Manual. RaySafe i2 dose viewer

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K.

EVALUATION OF DRUG LABEL DESIGNS USING EYE TRACKING. Agnieszka Bojko, Catherine Gaddy, Gavin Lew, Amy Quinn User Centric, Inc. Oakbrook Terrace, IL

Transcription:

Two Experiments on Co-located Mobile Groupware Miguel A. Nacenta*, Dzimitri Aliakseyeu**, Tadeuz Stach*, Sriram Subramanian* and Carl Gutwin* *Computer Science Department University of Saskatchewan http://hci.usask.ca Canada **Department of Industrial Design Technische Universiteit Eindhoven http://w3.id.tue.nl/nl/ The Netherlands INTRODUCTION The Dimensional Overlap Model (D.O.) [3] and Stimulus- Response Compatibility (SRC) [1,2,4] are two interrelated areas of study from experimental psychology that investigate the relationships between human response performance and the nature of the stimuli that elicits these responses. Decades of research in these areas have shown that the relationship between the stimuli and the required responses greatly affect the speed and accuracy with which users can perform simple tasks. These results have clear implications for the design of computer systems and, in particular, of groupware environments. Surprisingly enough, the field of Human- Computer Interaction has rarely tried to use or replicate the results, regardless of the fact that D.O. and SRC deal with the exact same problems that lie at the heart of interaction in the most basic and common computer tasks. Applying the lessons learnt from experimental psychology to HCI and Groupware can lead to a better understanding of basic interaction tasks (e.g. passing digital objects) and, in turn, to more efficient interfaces. The application of results from the psychological literature to human-computer application requires, however, that these results are replicated for situations and environments that are ecologically valid with respect to common computer tasks. In this report we describe two experiments on basic groupware tasks and interpret the results in terms of dimensional overlap and stimulus-response compatibility. In both experiments the task consists of selecting a particular user among the four co-present participants by tapping on a particular part of the screen assigned to that participant. The differences between the two experiments and the different conditions in the experiments lie in the way that the possible tapping targets (representing other users) are aligned on the screen with respect to the actual physical position of the users and in the way that the stimuli which indicates the desired destination is presented. From a first experiment in which the stimuli was presented in a non-spatial fashion, we found that the HCI-USASK Tech. Report 2007-1 relationship between the spatial distribution of the responses and the positions of the actual users has statistically significant effects on completion time, at least in the first 12 trials of each block. In a second experiment the stimuli were presented in a spatial fashion, and we also found differences depending on whether the responses required corresponded in position with the source of the stimuli. In this case, however, the differences were consistent across all trials. Our results fit nicely with predictions from the SRC and D.O. literatures. Although these experiments cover only a small part of the possible applications, they encourage the application of D.O. and SRC results to HCI problems, and the further pursue of experimental studies that will help predict performance of interface designs. CO-LOCATED MOBILE GROUPWARE This report is focused on situations in which several users want to collaborate while present in the same general space (e.g. the same room) through their portable devices (a co-located mobile groupware situation see figure 1). Real-life examples of such a situation would be colleagues casually meeting outside of the meeting room, friends interchanging music or files in the street, or work meetings in places where there is no access to large common displays. Figure 1. An example of a co-located mobile situation. One of the most common tasks for collaborators in this kind of situations is to exchange objects. These exchanges 1

can be implemented simply by generating portholes in the interface that link one person s device with another person s. This is analogous to having network folders in common operating systems: by dropping an object into the folder, the object is transmitted or made available to another device. In the design of these interfaces the question arises of whether the spatial arrangement of the portholes affects the efficiency of the interface. On one side, one might think that having the portholes aligned with the physical positions of the users will result in faster interactions and fewer errors; on the other side, providing this alignment might be expensive to implement in the devices (these have to be able to physically locate other devices), or it might require manual configuration efforts by the users. STIMULI AND RESPONSES The problem that we just presented above can be formulated in terms of stimuli and response. This reformulation will help us later to interpret the results of experiments through SRC and D.O. In an interactive system as the one described above, the stimulus is whatever triggers the subsequent action (also called response). For example, imagine an impromptu meeting in which participant A asks participant B for a copy of a document. Normally this request will trigger an action of user B in which she will have to select the document and then somehow indicate her computer to transfer the file to A s device. If there are more than 2 users, B will have to select the destination device among several possible destinations: a response must be chosen that corresponds to the stimulus. Stimuli and responses can be spatial or non-spatial. In the scenario described in the previous section, responses are mostly spatial because they consist of a spatial gesture on the screen of the device (perhaps a drag-and-drop action onto the corresponding network folder). However, stimuli can be spatial or non-spatial. An example of a spatial stimulus is an oral request by another user (e.g. pass me the spreadsheet file ). The request is spatial because it comes from a certain direction and probably has also some associated spatial cues, as lip movements or other gestures. An example of non-spatial stimulus is a situation in which user B knows that all spreadsheet files have to be passed to A, all text documents to C and so on. According to the Dimensional Overlap Model, stimulusresponse sets are said to be highly overlapping if they have attributes in common. In our examples, a spatial stimuli with a spatial response will have a high overlap, whereas a non-spatial stimuli with a spatial response will make for a poor dimensional overlap. COMPATIBLE AND INCOMPATIBLE MAPPINGS If the stimuli and response sets have a high dimensional overlap (e.g. if both are spatial) then the location of the responses might or might not correspond to the location of the stimuli. Sets in which the arrangement of the responses corresponds to the arrangement of the stimuli are said to be SR-compatible, and sets in which the arrangements don t correspond for stimulus and response are called SR-incompatible (see figure 2). Figure 2. A classic Stimulus-Response experiment setting. A) compatible mapping, B) and C) incompatible mappings Note that SR sets can only be compatible or incompatible if the sets have some degree of dimensional overlap. EMPIRICAL STUDIES We designed two experiments that test two of the situations already sketched in our scenario. Each of the experiments tests a single main hypothesis for which the SRC and D.O. models already have predictions. Experiment 1 The first experiment tests a situation in which the response is spatial but the response is not. The SR set has therefore low or nonexistent dimensional overlap. Our main interest is to find out if in this kind of situations, the physical arrangement of the responses matters. It should be noted that even though there is low dimensional overlap (and therefore there is no SRcompatibility), a co-located situation is always inherently spatial. Although the physical environment surrounding the task is, in this case, irrelevant for the completion of the task (see the description of the task below), we do not know whether it is possible for participants to ignore the physical environment around them even though the task does not require using or referencing it. In other words, can the spatial world interfere in tasks that are intrinsically non-spatial? Predictions and Hypothesis From the SRC and D.O. literature we know that irrelevant attributes of the response can have an effect in completion time (see, for example the Simon effect [5]). Therefore we hypothesize that: H1: Completion time of a task with non-overlapping stimulus response sets is higher if the arrangement of the responses corresponds to the physical arrangement of destinations (even though the physical arrangement of the destinations is not relevant to complete the task).

Participants We recruited 16 participants, 8 males and 8 females, in groups of 4. Groups were same-sex and knew each other from before the experiment. Ages ranged from 18 to 39. top circle would be the name of the person in front, and so on (see Figures 3 and 4). In the W-R incompatible condition none of the names matched the positions of the fellow participants. Apparatus Each participant in the group was assigned one pen-based computer at the beginning of the session. Several different computers were used for the study: one HP and one Toshiba tablet PC, and two Sony Vaio ultra-portable computers. The screen resolutions were 800x600 pixels for the two Sony machines, 1024x768 for the HP, and 1280x1024 for the Toshiba. Each computer ran software developed for the study in C#. To compensate differences in size and resolution of the four devices, the application was normalized to appear equal in size to all participants. Task The participants had to carry out a simple matching task with either a World-Response compatible mapping or with a WR-incompatible 1 mapping, while seated in four chairs facing each other. The task consisted of dropping one of four different shapes onto one of three circles that contained the other participant s names (see Figure 4). The relationship between the shapes and the names was established at the beginning of each group of trials; for each trial group, each shape corresponded to only one participant name. For example, if the names of the participants were Paul, Eva, John and Akira, Paul might be assigned a triangle, Eva a star and so on. Before each trial, the software let participants learn the correspondence between participants and shapes by showing a larger version of the shape assigned to each one in that participant s screen. They had to show each other their screens to learn the correspondences. For each trial, a shape appeared on the screen; users had to remember whose shape it was, touch on the shape and then on the corresponding named circle. A participant never got to match his or her own shape. The system provided individual visual feedback of the correctness of the user action. The locations of the circles themselves were always the same, but the names in the circles would be in different positions depending on the condition. In the W-R compatible condition, the name in the left circle would be the name of the person seating to the left, the name in the 1 Note that World-Response compatibility is different from Stimulus-Response compatibility (described in the Compatible and Incompatible mappings section). W-R compatible refers to the compatibility between the physical location of participants and the possible responses. W-R compatibility is irrelevant for this task because the stimuli are presented in a non-spatial form. Figure 3. The experimental setting Figure 4. A screenshot of the device-device task. The layout is compatible according to Figure 3. For training we used a variant of this task in which, instead of seating facing each other, participants sat by themselves. In the training task the names were not those of the other participants, but randomly chosen names of five letters instead. The associations between shapes and names were done at the beginning in a popup window. Procedure After filling in a demographics form and signing for consent, every participant was instructed in the training task using a demo. Then, each participants trained by themselves in two blocks of 75 correct trials in which the same names were used. The order in which the different shapes appeared did not follow any recognizable pattern. For each block the shapes were different, as were the positions. Note that the training task is neither W-R compatible nor W-R incompatible because the names do not represent actual people, and the users carry out the task isolated from anyone else. 3

After training, participants were asked to sit in the four chairs facing each other. Each group was assigned an order condition; half the groups started with compatible trials and half started with incompatible trials. In each condition, each group performed a training block of 15 trials and two blocks of 75 trials (counting only correct trials), for a total of 30 training trials and 150 correct test trials. For each block the participants changed their positions relative to each other, and they were assigned new shapes. If a participant made a mistake, that trial was replaced for another at the end of the block, assuring that all participants performed exactly 75 correct trials in each block, and a variable number of wrong trials. Once all blocks were completed by all four participants, they were asked to complete a post-task questionnaire. The questionnaire required the users to rate the W-R incompatible and W-R compatible conditions of the group task using the standard TLX performance measures. They also had to select which condition they preferred. Results We took four measures: completion time, errors, subjective workload assessments from the participants, and preference. A one-way ANOVA revealed that completion time was shorter for W-R compatible mappings than for W-R incompatible (F 1,15 = 6.88, p <.02). The participant variable was modeled as a random factor. Completion time was log-transformed in order to comply with the normality assumption of the ANOVA test. The average completion time for W-R compatible mappings was 780ms and for W-R incompatible mappings 827ms, a difference on average of 47ms (or 6%). Inspection of the data reveals that there is a strong learning effect which favors whichever condition was performed last (see Figure 5). This effect shows up as a significant interaction between order and mapping in a repeated-measures ANOVA (F 1,14 = 6.87, p <.02). Although we distributed the different orders across conditions evenly, we cannot discard the existence of asymmetric order effects across conditions (e.g. an incompatible mapping might be a much better training for a compatible mapping than the reverse). Therefore, we decided to analyze only the first condition for each participant, effectively transforming the design into a between-subjects comparison. This design type is, however, drastically less powerful than the previous one, and cannot show any statistically significant differences due to the huge variations in individual performance. Completion Time (ms) 880.0000 840.0000 800.0000 760.0000 720.0000 Incompatible Incompatible first Compatible first Compatible Figure 5. Order effects in the first experiment. Nevertheless, we decided to carry out the comparisons that we had planned before we ran the experiment. In particular, from a pilot study we discovered that the first 20 trials for each position showed a learning curve and interesting differences between conditions. We performed a repeated-measures ANOVA in which the condition (W- R compatible or incompatible) was modeled as a between-subjects variable and the trial order was the within-subjects variable. To reduce noise, trials were collapsed into three different groups of six trials: trials 2 to 7 in the first group, trials 8 to 13 in the second group and trials 14 to 19 in the third group (the first trial was eliminated because it is subject to external factors such as setup delay). The analysis reveals a significant effect of condition (F 1,14 = 4.23, single-sided p <.03), and trial group (F 2,28 = 14.02, p <.001). A first look at Figure 6 reveals the expected result that completion time is reduced with the number of trials performed. The Figure also shows that the difference between conditions is larger during the initial trials. In particular, the differences in average are very important in the first 12 trials (first and second trial groups), reaching a difference in means of 294ms in the first group and of 122ms in the second group. These findings indicate that the differences in completion time for W-R compatible and W-R incompatible mappings are relevant in the first trials with a particular mapping, but the difference is reduced with more experience until it is indistinguishable due to the noise of the measure. The error data was analyzed using a repeated-measures Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test that showed no difference between the W-R compatible and incompatible mappings (z = -.352, p <.72). The error data was analyzed also using only the first condition, yielding also nonsignificant results.

Completion Time (ms) 1,500.000 1,300.000 1,100.000 900.000 700.000 1 2 3 Group (6 trials) incompatible compatible Figure 6. Average completion time for the different conditions after each position change. Blocks aggregate data from 6 contiguous trials. Error bars represent Standard Deviation. The TLX workload assessment was analyzed for differences between the W-R compatible and incompatible mappings. A Wilcoxon Signed Ranks test revealed no statistically significant difference between the two conditions (z = -1.470, p >.14). It should be noted that the subjects answered this questionnaire only after doing all the trials, which means that the data reflects the subject s perceptions across all trials, not only the initial ones where the log data finds statistical differences. For the question of which mapping they preferred, 7 participants chose the compatible mapping, 3 chose the incompatible, and 6 stated no preference. These differences were analyzed using a Chi-square test that reveals no significant difference (χ 2 (2) = 1.62, p >.44). Experiment 2 The second experiment tests a situation in which response and stimulus are both spatial. The SR set has therefore high dimensional overlap and different mappings can be S-R compatible or S-R incompatible. Our main interest is to find out whether the differences in performance and errors shown in the SR Compatibility theory are present in a groupware setting and to which extent. Unlike for experiment 1, the conditions of experiment 2 are indeed compatible or incompatible in the classic SRC sense because the stimulus and response sets are both spatial, and therefore, have a high dimensional overlap. Predictions and Hypothesis The advantages of SR compatible mappings in one of the most reliable phenomena in experimental psychology. We therefore hypothesize that: H2: Completion time of a task with a stimulus-response compatible mapping is faster than with a stimulusresponse incompatible mapping. Task In this task the participants had to touch the circle corresponding to the person of the group that said Me!. Participants were instructed to say me! when they got a special screen in their devices with a button on it that they had to press. Other participants then had to touch on the center of the screen and in the corresponding named circle. The task was synchronized so that only one person at a time said me!, and a new trial only started after all participants had finished the previous one. The order in which the participants said me! did not follow any noticeable pattern. As in the first experiment, the names in the circles were in different locations depending on the condition. In the compatible condition the names matched the positions in the real world; in the incompatible, none matched. The system also provided visual feedback of correctness. Procedure For each condition (compatible and incompatible) participants performed a short demo of the task (3 correct trials per person) and then two blocks of 45 correct trials per person; this meant that the group performed 60 correct trials per block, since a trial in which a user had to say me! was not counted or measured for that user. The order of the conditions was the same as in the first experiment, and again participants changed positions after each block. If any participant made a mistake, that trial was replaced for another at the end of the block, assuring that all participants performed at least 45 correct trials each in each block, and a variable number of wrong trials. Once all blocks were completed, they were asked again to complete a post-task questionnaire as in the first study. Results We took four measures: completion time, errors, subjective workload assessments from the participants, and preference. A one-way ANOVA shows that completion time with compatible mappings is significantly different than with incompatible mappings (F 1,15 = 39.49, p <.001). The completion time variable was log-transformed in order to comply with the normality assumption of the ANOVA test. Participants were modeled as a random factor. The average completion time was 1115ms for the incompatible condition and 927ms for the compatible condition, a difference of 188ms (i.e., the task took 20% more time to complete with incompatible mappings). Unlike in the first experiment, this time there were no signs of a strong learning effect. This is corroborated by a repeated-measures ANOVA in which order was included as a between-subjects factor. In this test, the order was 5

found to be non-significant (F 1,14 = 0.083, p >.70) as well as the interaction of order and mapping (F 1,14 = 0.011, p >.90). For the effectiveness measure we performed a repeatedmeasures Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test that shows that the number of errors is higher for the incompatible mapping than for the compatible one (z = -3.078, p <.002). On average, a participant would make 2.75 errors per set with the incompatible mapping and 0.81 with the compatible mapping. A set is defined as the group of trials that take place without a change of position of the participants relative to each other and the mapping in their devices. The TLX workload assessment was analyzed for differences between the mappings. A Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test revealed a significant difference between conditions (z = -3.498, p <.001), with the incompatible mapping having a higher workload assessment than the compatible (M = 34.3% vs. M = 24.0). When asked which mapping they preferred, 10 chose the compatible mapping, 4 had no preference, and 2 preferred the incompatible. A Chi-square test shows significant differences between conditions (χ 2 (2) = 6.5, p <.04). Discussion The results from the second experiment clearly support hypothesis 2: correspondence between the arrangement of stimulus and response makes interaction more efficient, less error-prone and preferred by the participants. This is consistent with the psychological research on SRC and the Simon effect. Hypothesis 1 is also confirmed by the results of the first experiment, but the results are less conclusive than for the second hypothesis. First, the advantage of the W-R compatible setting is only noticeable during the first 12 to 16 trials; second, the improvement is only in completion time, not in number of errors. We believe that this difference is caused because users have to explicitly focus on blocking out the world when using the W-R incompatible mapping. After a while, the associations of shape with their correct destinations in the device are strengthened so that the physical position of others can be ignored. This study was limited in that we only used groups of four users. We cannot be sure of how larger groups will affect the results, but we suspect that the differences will only increase as existing SRC literature shows a consistent increase of the differences between mappings when the number of possible responses increases. Another factor that these experiments do not test is the accuracy of the symbolic representation. We would expect that with less accurate maps of the participants, the advantages of a compatible mapping will fade away, especially with large numbers of users. These issues deserve attention in further research. REFERENCES 1. Fitts, P. M., & Deininger, R. L. S-R compatibility: Correspondence among paired elements within stimulus and response codes. J. Experimental Psychology, 48 (1954), 483-492. 2. Fitts, P.M., Seeger C.M. S-R compatibility: Spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. J. Experimental Psychology, 46, (1953) 199-210. 3. Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., and Osman, A. Dimensional Overlap: Cognitive Basis for Stimulus- Response Compatibility-A model and Taxonomy. Psychological Review (1990), Vol 97, No 2, 253-270 4. Proctor, R.W., Reeve, T.G. eds. Stimulus-Response Compatibility. (1990). 5. Simon, R. and Rudell, A. Auditory S-R Compatibility: The Effect of an Irrelevant Cue on Information Processing. J. Appl. Psychology, 51, 3 (1967), 300-304.