Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

Similar documents
Supporting Online Material for

Neural and Behavioral Evidence for an Intrinsic Cost of Self-Control

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Materials for

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Blame judgment task.

Joseph T. McGuire. Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences Boston University 677 Beacon St. Boston, MA

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Task timeline for Solo and Info trials.

TIME DISCOUNTING FOR PRIMARY AND MONETARY REWARDS *

The neural basis of value accumulation in intertemporal

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Supplementary Online Content

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Whose Choice is it Anyway? Montague's Experimental Results

Intertemporal Decision-Making Involves Prefrontal Control Mechanisms Associated with Working Memory

Instant Gratification and Self-Control in an Experiment with Children and Teenagers

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS: Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision-making

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

Delay discounting, food reward, and eating in the absence of hunger

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making in Psychopathy

The Neural Basis of Following Advice

Role of the ventral striatum in developing anorexia nervosa

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Double dissociation of value computations in orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate neurons

Distinguishing informational from value-related encoding of rewarding and punishing outcomes in the human brain

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions

Biosocial Science. David Laibson July 7, 2016

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Experimental evidence 1

Supplementary Material for The neural basis of rationalization: Cognitive dissonance reduction during decision-making. Johanna M.

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria

Hierarchical Control over Effortful Behavior by Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Clay Holroyd. Department of Psychology University of Victoria

RETRACTED. Motivated Cognitive Control: Reward Incentives Modulate Preparatory Neural Activity during Task-Switching

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

The Neural Signature of Social Norm Compliance Manfred Spitzer, Urs Fischbacher, Bärbel Herrnberger, Georg Grön, Ernst Fehr

The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability

NeuroImage 76 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

the body and the front interior of a sedan. It features a projected LCD instrument cluster controlled

Spatial Normalisation, Atlases, & Functional Variability

Focusing Attention on the Health Aspects of Foods Changes Value Signals in vmpfc and Improves Dietary Choice

PHONE: (719) HOME PAGE: Princeton University Postdoctoral Research Associate

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Medial Prefrontal Cortical Activity Reflects Dynamic Re-Evaluation During Voluntary Persistence

Classification of Alzheimer s disease subjects from MRI using the principle of consensus segmentation

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Working Memory Overload: Fronto-Limbic Interactions and Effects on Subsequent Working Memory Function

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Eur J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 August 10.

Embargoed until Oct. 16, 3:30 p.m. CST Contacts: Kat Snodgrass, (202) Press Room, Oct : (504) Todd Bentsen, (202)

Sensitivity of the nucleus accumbens to violations in expectation of reward

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

Neuroeconomics Lecture 1

Fibre orientation dispersion in the corpus callosum relates to interhemispheric functional connectivity

Functional connectivity in fmri

Supporting Information

Brian M Sandroff, Glenn R Wylie, Brad P Sutton, Curtis L Johnson, John DeLuca and Robert W Motl

Neuroeconomics Laibson Lecture 2

Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task

Supporting online material for: Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain

Testing the Reward Prediction Error Hypothesis with an Axiomatic Model

Prefrontal connections express individual differences in intrinsic resistance to trading off honesty values against economic benefits

The Anterior Cingulate and Error Avoidance

Mapping Neural circuitry of Risk and Resilience for Suicidal Behavior and Mood Disorders

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping

Performance Feedback Drives Caudate Activation in a Phonological Learning Task

Consumer Neuroscience Research beyond fmri: The Importance of Multi-Method Approaches for understanding Goal Value Computations

Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs

Figure Credits. Figure 4. Photo courtesy of the Mind Research Network.

Network-based pattern recognition models for neuroimaging

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Reference Dependence In the Brain

The Neural Basis of Economic Decision- Making in The Ultimatum Game

Is model fitting necessary for model-based fmri?

Supplementary Online Content

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

Altruistic Behavior: Lessons from Neuroeconomics. Kei Yoshida Postdoctoral Research Fellow University of Tokyo Center for Philosophy (UTCP)

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Experimental Design I

Moving the brain: Neuroimaging motivational changes of deep brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder Figee, M.

Restricting Temptations: Neural Mechanisms of Precommitment

Inferential Brain Mapping

Neural Circuitry of Emotional and Cognitive Conflict Revealed through Facial Expressions

Tools of the trade: psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity

White matter integrity of the cerebellar peduncles as a mediator of effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on eyeblink conditioning

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Sun, D; Chan, CCH; Hu, Y; Wang, Z; Lee, TMC

Distinct Brain Systems Mediate the Effects of Nociceptive Input and Self-Regulation on Pain

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

What do you think of the following research? I m interested in whether a low glycemic index diet gives better control of diabetes than a high

Psychological Science

How Self-Determined Choice Facilitates Performance: A Key Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

Supporting Information

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Transcription:

Supporting Information Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc In the fmri study on effort discounting by Botvinick, Huffstettler, and McGuire [1], described in detail in the original publication, participants completed task blocks with either low or high demands for task switching. After each block, participants either received or did not receive a reward presented as payment for the task (independent of accuracy). In the analysis originally reported by Botvinick and colleagues [1], single-subject general linear models (GLM) analyses estimated a trial-by-trial beta coefficient for each task block and reward delivery event. There were 72 trials in the experiment, each consisting of a task block followed by a reward. This initial analysis therefore yielded, for each participant, whole-brain maps of 72 task-block coefficients and 72 corresponding rewardevent coefficients. Trials were approximately evenly distributed across the cells of a 2x2 design, crossing task demand (high vs. low) with reward magnitude (high vs. low). For the central analysis of the previous report [1], these single-event coefficients were spatially averaged in two anatomical regions of interest (ROIs), located in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and nucleus accumbens (NAc). A regression model then tested the lagged functional connectivity between ACC (in the task phase) and NAc (in the subsequent reward phase) across trials. The reward-phase NAc response was the outcome

variable in the regression model, and there were four predictors: demand level, reward magnitude, task-phase ACC response, and task-phase NAc response. (Task-phase NAc response was included to account for baseline fluctuations.) The key finding was that, even controlling for the other predictors, task-phase activity in ACC was negatively related to subsequent reward-phase activity in NAc. These results suggested an effort discounting effect: an increase in cognitive costs, as registered by the ACC, decreased the reward response in the NAc. The objective of the present reanalysis was to test, in a voxelwise manner across the entire brain, whether other areas would show the same effect that this analysis detected in ACC. Our interest was particularly centered on dlpfc. In our original study [1], we reported a region-of-interest (ROI) analysis within this region, which failed to reveal the effect of interest, a finding that led us to conclude that dlpfc is not involved in the registration of effort costs. However, a subsequent study [2] found that activity in both ACC and a region of dlpfc correlated with demand avoidance. Importantly, the dlpfc area identified in this study differed slightly from the region targeted in the ROI analysis in Botvinick et al. [1]. Based on this, we speculated that the present whole-brain approach might reveal a dlpfc effect in the Botvinick et al. [1] dataset. The new analysis tests the relationship between task-phase activity in each voxel and the subsequent reward response in the NAc ROI. In other words, we tested each voxel's lagged functional connectivity, across trials, with a seed region in NAc.

The terms in the regression model were rearranged to facilitate whole-brain exploratory analysis, but their relationships remained the same. Whole-brain maps of task-phase beta coefficients (72 per participant, each corresponding to one trial) served as the outcome data (see the original report for details of the estimation of these trial-wise coefficients). The predictor of interest was the reward-phase NAc response. Additional predictors were demand level, reward magnitude, and task-phase NAc response. A voxelwise regression analysis for each participant was conducted using AFNI software [3]. The relationship between each voxel's task-phase response and the reward-phase NAc response (controlling for the other three variables) served as an index of the effort discounting effect at that location. A permutation test (the FSL function Randomise ) was used to produce a whole-brain map of clusters (controlled for family-wise error rate) that correlated with the effort discounting effect. Pertinent to the current study, this new analysis revealed an area in bilateral dlpfc (depicted in Figure 1 of the main text) that negatively correlated with reward response in the NAc, implicating a role for this area in effort discounting. A comment is warranted given the relationship between this dlpfc effect, which provides the focus of the present work, and the ACC effect originally reported. In McGuire et al. [2], we offered some speculations concerning potential detailed differences between the roles played by dlpfc and ACC in effort-cost evaluation. While such fine-grained differences remain tenable, it should be noted that they are not directly relevant to the hypotheses at stake in the present work. The key assumption, for present

purposes, is simply that dlpfc engagement is relevant to effort-cost registration and avoidance. Neuroimaging meta-analysis To compare the regions of dlpfc recruited during self-control and the representation of cognitive costs, we leveraged data from five published fmri studies, two focusing on effort cost representations and three on self-control. The first neuroimaging dataset on cognitive costs came from the effort discounting study reported above. In the second study, performed by McGuire and Botvinick [2], participants completed an version of the DST in which participants performed blocks of task-switching trials, rating after each the degree to which they wished to avoid similar task blocks in future. As described in detail in the original report, these avoidance ratings were correlated with the degree to which the dlpfc was recruited during task performance, even after controlling for reaction times and error rates. The experiments on self-control comprised of two intertemporal choice studies performed by McClure and colleagues [4,5] and one study on self-control in self-described dieters by Hare, Camerer and Rangel [6]. In the intertemporal choice experiments, participants performed an ITC task similar to the one described above in conjunction with our behavioral study. In one experiment,

participants chose between Amazon gifts card with different dollar values and delays, whereas in the other, participants the rewards were squirts of juice or water administered during the scanning session. As described in the original reports, the authors discovered a network of brain regions, including dlpfc, which increased activation when the delayed larger reward was selected (see Figure 1 of main text). Hare and colleagues [6] presented dieters with choices between healthy and unhealthy foods. They found that when participants showed failures in self-control, i.e., when they selected the unhealthy option, activity in a region of the dlpfc (see Figure 1 in main text) decreased. Bolstering the idea that this region was implicated in self-control, task-related fluctuations in this region were functionally related with goal values represented in the ventromedial PFC. As a first step in our analyses, we created functional masks based on the dlpfc regions identified in each of the five studies. Next, we used AFNI s 3dAllineate function [3] to warp the datasets that were originally in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI; all datasets, except for the McGuire and Botvinick demand avoidance experiment) space to Talairach space. The resulting dlpfc masks are the ones displayed in Figure 1 of the main text. In order to perform our region-of-interest (ROI) analyses, we took the whole-brain maps of correlations between task-phase activity and subsequent avoidance ratings in the McGuire and Botvinick [2] demand avoidance study (n = 10) and of correlations

between task-phase activity and the subsequent reward-related response in the NAc in the Botvinick and colleagues [1] effort discounting study (n = 23). For each participant, we computed the average correlation in each of the three dlpfc clusters from the self-control datasets (Talaraich-transformed for the McGuire and Botvinick dataset). For both studies and all three dlpfc clusters, group-level t-tests revealed that the task-phase signal showed a significant correlation with avoidance ratings (Table 1, top row) and activity in the NAc (Table 1, bottom row). References 1. Botvinick MM, Huffstetler S, McGuire JT (2009) Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 9: 16-27. 2. McGuire JT, Botvinick MM (2010) Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107: 7922 7926. 3. Cox R (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research 29: 162-173. 4. McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2007) Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience 27: 5796. 5. McClure SM, Laibson D, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD (2004) Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306: 503-507. 6. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A (2009) Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmpfc valuation system. Science 324: 646-648.