UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

Similar documents
ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

An Investigation of Stimulus Pairing and Listener Training to Establish Emergent Intraverbals in Children with Autism

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

The Effects of Multiple-Tact and Receptive-Discrimination Training on the Acquisition of Intraverbal Behavior

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

A COMPARISON OF PROCEDURES TO ESTABLISH EMERGENT INTRAVERBALS IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS. Mary Vallinger

The Effects of Multiple Exemplar Instruction on Generating Functional Interdependence between Listener and Intraverbal Categorization Repertoires

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

Acquisition of Picture Exchange-Based vs. Signed Mands and Implications to Teach Functional Communication Skills to Children with Autism

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

TRANSFER OF MAND TOPOGRAPHIES TO TACT RELATIONS AND VICE VERSA IN TWO VOCAL-VERBAL CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Jill E. Castellani, B.A.

The Role of Joint Control in Teaching Complex Behavior to Children with Autism

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

A Role of Problem-Solving in Complex Intraverbal Repertoires

WESTERN MICHIGAN UNIVERSITY

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

Cultivating Motivation During Natural Environment Teaching and Group Instruction for Diverse Learners. Pam Salerno Aja Weston Michael Houck

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

The Effects of Alternating Mand and Tact Training on the Acquisition of Tacts

Teaching Language to Young Children. Functional Analysis and Intervention

TEACHING SIMPLE AUDITORY DISCRIMINATIONS TO STUDENTS WITH AUTISM. Kristine L. Marino, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

Verbal Operants as Predictors for Children with Autism in Inclusive Settings

The Effects of Precision Teaching With Textual or Tact Relations on Intraverbal Relations

That Define the Mand and the Tact

VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

Evaluating Picture Exchange and the ipad as a Speech Generating Device to Teach Communication to Young Children with Autism

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

A SORTING-TO-MATCHING METHOD TO TEACH COMPOUND MATCHING TO SAMPLE RACHEL S. FARBER

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010

The Core Elements of Applied Verbal Behaviour AVB. Dr. Chafica Mansour Gharbieh Education & ABA/VB Specialist

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

MAKING IT WORK AS AN ABA THERAPIST

A Suggested Methodology for the Application of Parent Training in Teaching Verbal Behavior to Children with Autism

Using a Behavioral Skills Approach to Train a Mother to Teach Basic Learning and Self-Help Skills to Her Daughter Diagnosed with Rett Syndrome

Acquisition and Generalization of Tacts across Stimulus Modes in Children Diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder

A TRANSLATIONAL EVALUATION OF THE INTERPOLATED REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURE WITH YOUNG CHILDREN

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

Generalization across Modalities of Expressive and Receptive Language. Acquisition through the Use of Multiple Exemplar Training and Errorless

J-01 State intervention goals in observable and measureable terms.

Association for Behavior Analysis-International

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

COMBINING INTERVENTION STRATEGIES TO ADDRESS INDIVIDUAL NEEDS OF CHILDREN WITH ASDS

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

Bidirectional Naming and Problem Solving Caio Miguel, Ph.D., BCBA-D California State University, Sacramento

Teaching the Language of Emotions

Effects of Conditioning Reinforcement for Print Stimuli on Match-to-Sample Responding in Preschoolers

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

A Behavioral Analysis of the DSM-5 Criteria for Autism: Failure of Normal Back-and-Forth Conversation

Emily A. Jones, PhD, BCBA 1

HELPING STUDENTS WITH AUTISM FIND THEIR VOICE FINAL EXAM

Contents. copyrighted material by PRO-ED, Inc.

DERIVED CONDITIONAL POSITION DISCRIMINATION IN INDIVIDUALS WITHOUT NAMI NG SKILLS

Empowering Families and Children with Autism through STEPS: Screening, Teaching, Evaluating, and Parenting for Success!

National University of Ireland, Maynooth

Using Pivotal Response Training with Peers in Special Education to Facilitate Play in Two Children with Autism

THE REFERENTIAL NATURE OF RULES AND INSTRUCTIONS: A RESPONSE TO INSTRUCTIONS, RULES, AND ABSTRACTION: A MISCONSTRUED RELATION BY EMILIO RIBES-IÑESTA

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

Teaching Children with ASD Intraverbal Responses About the Past

A Comparison of a Matrix Programming and Standard Discrete Trial Training Format to Teach Two-Component Tacts

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND KRIS M. KEENEY GWINNETT COUNTY PUBLIC SCHOOLS, GEORGIA

Paraprofessional Training Module

Using TAG to Increase Play Skills

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

The Effect of Varying Teacher Presentation Rates on Responding During Discrete Trial Training for Two Children With Autism

Derived Textual Control in Recreational Activity Schedules with Children with Autism

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

Analyzing and Removing Barriers that Impede Language Acquisition

Establishing Complex Derived Manding With Children With and Without a Diagnosis of Autism

Verbal Behavior 101: A Quick Introduction. Tashenna Gillmore, M.Ed., BCBA, LBA The Heart of Behavior LLC.

Effect of ABA Therapy on Language and Communication Skills among Children with Autism in. Kyrgyzstan. Anna Semikina

A Comparison of Two Variations of a Stimulus- Stimulus Pairing Procedure On Novel and Infrequent Vocalizations of Children with Autism

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Comparing the Teaching Interaction Procedure to Social Stories: A Replication Study

PECS and Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Andy Bondy, Ph.D. Lori Frost, MS, CCC-SLP

We are IntechOpen, the world s leading publisher of Open Access books Built by scientists, for scientists. International authors and editors

What is Autism? Katherine Lamb, Ph.D., CCC/SLP GSHA2018 1

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2008, 41, 411 415 NUMBER 3(FALL 2008) THE EFFECTS OF TACT AND LISTENER TRAINING ON THE EMERGENCE OF BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBAL RELATIONS ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR, ALMA RÚN ÓLAFSDÓTTIR, AND BERGLIND ARADÓTTIR UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI We evaluated the effects of 2 types of training on the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations with 4 typically developing children. Tact training involved reinforcing foreignlanguage vocalizations in the presence of visual stimuli, and listener training involved reinforcing selections of visual stimuli following vocal presentations of foreign-language words. Intraverbal relations were tested by instructing participants to vocalize native-language equivalents of foreign-language words or vice versa. Both types of training produced increases in intraverbal responding, but the emergent relations were not always bidirectional. DESCRIPTORS: emergent relations, foreign-language instruction, intraverbals, listener behavior, tacts Skinner (1957) described the intraverbal relation as a verbal response under the control of an antecedent verbal stimulus that does not have point-to-point correspondence with the response product. In the applied verbal behavior literature (e.g., Sundberg & Partington, 1998), this term has been used to describe a variety of educationally relevant outcomes of language training, such as answering social questions, reciting the alphabet, singing songs, telling stories, and describing stimuli in their absence. It has been argued that because academic and social behavior relies heavily on intraverbal repertoires, early childhood education should emphasize their establishment (Partington & Bailey, 1993; Sundberg & Michael, 2001). A few studies have investigated the extent to which young children may derive novel intraverbal relations from the reinforcement of other relations. Two studies (Miguel, Petursdottir, & Carr, 2005; Partington & Bailey, 1993) evaluated the the effects of training The second and third authors contribution should be considered equal. We thank Mark Sundberg for helpful comments. Address correspondence to Anna Ingeborg Petursdottir, who is now at the Department of Psychology, Texas Christian University, Box 298920, Fort Worth, Texas 76129 (e-mail: a.petursdottir@tcu.edu). doi: 10.1901/jaba.2008.41-411 multiple tact relations; that is, verbal responses in the presence of nonverbal stimuli (Skinner, 1957). Miguel et al. additionally trained the selection of nonverbal stimuli in the presence of verbal stimuli, sometimes termed listener relations, as did Petursdottir, Carr, Lechago, and Almason (2008). Across the three studies, tact and listener training had minimal effects on typically developing preschoolers vocal intraverbal responding, suggesting that for young children, activities such as labeling and identifying stimuli in picture books and videos may not always suffice to expand intraverbal repertoires. However, all three studies investigated training in the context of teaching categorization skills. Having acquired relations between objects and their object names and category names, the participants were tested for the ability to emit either multiple object names given a category name (Miguel et al.; Partington & Bailey) or the same category name given multiple object names (Petursdottir et al.). Thus, the relations were hierarchical (i.e., ordered groupings within a system, as in the case of utensils [category] and knife [object within the category]). The present study was intended to provide a preliminary evaluation of the effects of tact and listener training on the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations 411

412 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al. in the context of a less complex, nonhierarchical task structure. To this effect, 4 children were taught a small foreign-language vocabulary in which a one-to-one relation existed between native-language and foreign-language nouns. METHOD Participants, Setting, and Materials Ida, Emil, Lena, and Karen were 5 years old, spoke Icelandic as a native language, did not speak a second language, and participated in the study with their parents permission. Sessions lasted 20 to 30 min and were conducted three to five times a week over a period of 2 to 4 weeks. Sessions took place in an empty preschool classroom, where the child and experimenter sat across from each other at a child-sized table. Visual stimuli consisted of laminated color photographs (8 cm by 10 cm) of six animals pictured in a natural environment and six fruits pictured against a white background. Prior to the experiment, the children could name (i.e., tact) all of the visual stimuli in Icelandic and vocally imitate their Spanish names. Procedure Tact and listener training. Emil and Ida received tact training, and Lena and Karen received listener training. Each child received training sequentially on two six-stimulus sets (fruits and animals). Training was conducted in six-trial blocks. On a tact training trial, the experimenter placed a visual stimulus on the table in front of the child and asked the Icelandic equivalent of What is this [animal or fruit] called in Spanish? A correct response was defined as vocalizing the Spanish name of the animal or fruit displayed on the picture. On a listener training trial, the experimenter placed three visual stimuli (all animals or all fruits) on the table and asked the Icelandic equivalent of Which [animal or fruit] is called [Spanish name]? A correct response was defined as touching the target stimulus. The experimenter delivered social praise contingent on correct responses in both types of training. If the child made an incorrect response, the experimenter prompted a correct response vocally (tact training) or by pointing (listener training). If the child did not make a response, the experimenter waited 10 s and then prompted a correct response. Training continued until 100% correct responding was achieved in three consecutive trial blocks. Intraverbal testing. We used a multiple baseline design across stimulus sets to evaluate the effects of tact training (Ida and Emil) or listener training (Karen and Lena) on the acquisition of two types of intraverbal responding: Icelandic-Spanish intraverbals and Spanish- Icelandic intraverbals. Intraverbals were tested in baseline and following each training phase. Each trial block consisted of six Icelandic- Spanish and six Spanish-Icelandic trials. Trial presentation order varied across blocks. The experimenter initiated a Spanish-Icelandic trial by presenting the Icelandic equivalent of What does [Spanish name] mean? A correct response was defined as vocalizing the Icelandic equivalent of the Spanish name. The experimenter initiated an Icelandic-Spanish trial by presenting the Icelandic equivalent of What is [Icelandic name] in Spanish? A correct response was defined as vocalizing the Spanish equivalent of the Icelandic name. The experimenter did not present visual stimuli during test trials and did not deliver any consequences following correct or incorrect responses. Data collection and interobserver agreement. During testing and training, the experimenter recorded correct and incorrect responses on data sheets. The experimenter recorded a correct response when the child emitted the target vocalization or selected the target visual stimulus and recorded an incorrect response if the child vocalized the name of or selected a different stimulus or did not respond within 10 s of trial initiation. The experimenter scored the child s first response if the child vocalized

BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBALS 413 multiple fruit or animal names. A second observer independently collected data on at least 40% of all testing and training trials for each child. An agreement was defined as both observers recording a correct response or both recording an incorrect response. Otherwise, a disagreement was scored. Point-by-point interobserver agreement for each trial block was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the sum of agreements and disagreements and converting this ratio to a percentage. During testing, mean interobserver agreement was 100% for Emil, Ida, and Lena and 99% (range, 92% to 100%) for Karen. Mean agreement during training exceeded 97% for each child. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Figure 1 shows intraverbal testing data for all participants. Correct intraverbals rarely occurred in baseline, but stable increases were observed following both types of training. The tact-trained participants (Emil and Ida) performed with high accuracy (overall.90% correct) in Icelandic-Spanish trials on both stimulus sets. In addition, their performance in Spanish-Icelandic trials was highly accurate (overall.90% correct) on the animals set, but Emil s performance was less accurate on the fruits set. The listener-trained participants (Lena and Karen) did not reach.90% overall levels of accuracy in either testing condition; however, Lena responded with 100% accuracy in Spanish-Icelandic trials during the last two blocks of testing on the animals set. Lena s performance in Icelandic-Spanish trials was consistently less accurate than her performance in Spanish-Icelandic trials, whereas Karen performed with similar accuracy in both conditions. In sum, tact training reliably resulted in the acquisition of an Icelandic- Spanish intraverbal repertoire and somewhat less reliably in the acquisition of Spanish- Icelandic intraverbals. The effects of listener training were more variable and typically resulted in only partial acquisition of the intraverbal relations. Both types of training appeared to produce greater effects on intraverbal responding than those observed in prior research on emergent vocal intraverbal relations (Miguel et al., 2005; Partington & Bailey, 1993; Petursdottir et al., 2008). This might suggest that young children readily derive vocal intraverbal relations when those relations are not hierarchically structured, whereas a hierarchical structure might interfere with their emergence. However, the participants in the present study were slightly older or in the upper age range of those in earlier studies, and as a result, developmental differences cannot be ruled out. Although tact training appeared to have a stronger effect on the intraverbal repertoire than did listener training, these data should be interpreted with caution, because no within-subject evaluation was conducted to compare the effects of the two types of training. In addition, the tact-trained children were exposed to more training trials (M 5 53.7 trial blocks) than the listener-trained children (M 5 33.8 trial blocks) before they reached the training criterion (acquisition data are available from the first author). One finding of interest, however, is that training did not necessarily result in equal accuracy on both of the tested relations. When Lena was vocally presented with Spanish words following listener training, she emitted their Icelandic equivalents with fairly high accuracy, but when she was vocally presented with those same Icelandic words, she rarely emitted correct Spanish responses. Also, following tact training on his first stimulus set, Emil s intraverbal responding was more accurate in Spanish than in his native Icelandic language. These findings suggest that emergent intraverbal relations are not necessarily bidirectional, along with the possibility that the training of different relations (e.g., tacts vs. listener relations) may differentially affect which type of unidirectional relation emerges. This finding may be related to the role

414 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al. Figure 1. The number of correct responses on Icelandic-Spanish and Spanish-Icelandic intraverbal test trials in baseline and following the completion of tact training (top two panels) and listener training (bottom two panels).

BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBALS 415 of the novel Spanish words as either responses or discriminative stimuli (S D s) during training and testing. Tact training directly established the Spanish vocalizations required on Icelandic- Spanish test trials but did not establish stimulus control over any kind of responding by the Spanish names presented in Spanish-Icelandic trials. Listener training, by contrast, established stimulus control by Spanish words over selection responses, but no Spanish vocalizations were required. Keeping this in mind, a verbal behavior analysis (Skinner, 1957) might suggest that if during training, the children were to tact the visual stimuli in Icelandic prior to selecting them or tacting them in Spanish, those tacts alone could suffice to establish the intraverbal relation that shared a common response form or a common S D with the trained relation. By contrast, additional processes would be necessary for the emergence of the opposite relation. Although we did not monitor the occurrence of Icelandic tacts, it is possible that they occurred either overtly or covertly. Future research might examine further the role of existing tacts in the indirect establishment of novel intraverbal relations and explore practical and conceptual (cf. Horne & Lowe, 1996) implications of unidirectional versus bidirectional intraverbals. It should be noted that the similarity of the instruction used on tact training trials and Icelandic-Spanish test trials may have facilitated responding on test trials as a result of the verbal stimulus Spanish acquiring some degree of control over Spanish-language vocalizations. However, because this stimulus was present on all tact and Icelandic-Spanish trials, the specific form of the response must have been controlled by the visual stimulus on tact trials and by the relevant vocal stimulus (Icelandic fruit or animal name) on Icelandic-Spanish trials. Exclusive intraverbal control by Spanish would result in one of the six responses being correct by chance (assuming a repertoire of the six Spanish responses in the set) and therefore cannot explain why tacts were acquired to criterion and Icelandic-Spanish intraverbals were typically emitted with 100% accuracy. More likely, instructions on test trials functioned as contextual stimuli that prompted the child to vocalize some name in either Spanish or Icelandic. REFERENCES Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185 241. Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2005). The effects of multiple-tact and receptive-discrimination training on the acquisition of intraverbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 27 41. Partington, J. W., & Bailey, J. S. (1993). Teaching intraverbal behavior to preschool children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 11, 9 18. Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., Lechago, S. A., & Almason, S. M. (2008). An evaluation of intraverbal training and listener training for teaching categorization skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 53 68. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley. Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The benefits of Skinner s analysis of verbal behavior for children with autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698 724. Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching language to children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Pleasant Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc. Received March 13, 2007 Final acceptance July 24, 2007 Action Editor, Bridget Taylor