Functional MRI study of gender effects in brain activations during verbal working

Similar documents
Functional MRI Study of Gender Effects in Brain Activations During Verbal Working Memory Task

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Supplementary Online Content

FMRI Data Analysis. Introduction. Pre-Processing

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

GENDER-SPECIFIC SENSITVITY TO TIME-DISCREPANT TASK CONDITIONS OF REASONING DURING fmri

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Supporting Online Material for

Supplementary Online Content

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background:

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Brain diffusion tensor imaging changes in cerebrotendinous xanthomatosis reversed with

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Supporting Information

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Supporting Online Material for

A possible mechanism for impaired joint attention in autism

Common Neural Substrates for Ordinal Representation in Short-Term Memory, Numerical and Alphabetical Cognition

Primacy and recency effects as indices of the focus of attention

Supplementary Online Material Supplementary Table S1 to S5 Supplementary Figure S1 to S4

Voxel-based morphometry in clinical neurosciences

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

CEREBRUM. Dr. Jamila EL Medany

NeuroImage 45 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Supporting Information

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Title: Resting hyperperfusion of the hippocampus, midbrain and striatum

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

Regional and Lobe Parcellation Rhesus Monkey Brain Atlas. Manual Tracing for Parcellation Template

Text to brain: predicting the spatial distribution of neuroimaging observations from text reports (submitted to MICCAI 2018)

Botulinum toxin treatment of freezing of gait in Parkinson s disease patients as reflected in functional magnetic resonance imaging of leg movement

The neural correlates of conceptual and perceptual false recognition

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Differential contributions of subregions of medial temporal lobe to memory system in. amnestic mild cognitive impairment: insights from fmri study

Changes in Default Mode Network as Automaticity Develops in a Categorization Task

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Announcement. Danny to schedule a time if you are interested.

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

fmri and Voxel-based Morphometry in Detection of Early Stages of Alzheimer's Disease

CEREBRUM Dr. Jamila Elmedany Dr. Essam Eldin Salama

How to report my result using REST slice viewer?

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods


Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

The Effects of Music intervention on Functional connectivity. Supplemental Information

The functional neuroanatomy of human working memory revisited Evidence from 3-T fmri studies using classical domain-specific interference tasks

Andy C.H. Lee a,b,, Trevor W. Robbins b, Stephen Smith c, Gemma A. Calvert c, Irene Tracey c, Paul Matthews c, Adrian M. Owen a. 1.

Supplementary materials. Appendix A;

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Supplementary Online Content

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

AN fmri EXAMINATION OF VISUAL INTEGRATION IN SCHIZOPHRENIA

Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different

Face-specific resting functional connectivity between the fusiform gyrus and posterior superior temporal sulcus

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Confidence-based Ensemble for GBM brain tumor segmentation

Effect Assessment of Parkinson Disease on Default Mode Network of the Brain with ICA and SCA Methods in Resting State FMRI Data

Involvement of both prefrontal and inferior parietal cortex. in dual-task performance

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Reproducibility is necessary but insufficient for addressing brain science issues.

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

independent of comorbid disorders and exposure to maltreatment

Supplementary Material. Functional connectivity in multiple cortical networks is associated with performance. across cognitive domains in older adults

Topographical functional connectivity patterns exist in the congenitally, prelingually deaf

Anatomy and Physiology (Bio 220) The Brain Chapter 14 and select portions of Chapter 16

Reasoning and working memory: common and distinct neuronal processes

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

Funding: NIDCF UL1 DE019583, NIA RL1 AG032119, NINDS RL1 NS062412, NIDA TL1 DA

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Selective Abnormal Modulation of Hippocampal Activity During Memory Formation in First-Episode Psychosis

The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load

Training-Induced Functional Activation Changes in Dual-Task Processing: An fmri Study

Functional Connectivity Measures in Memory Networks Using Independent Component Analysis

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

Anatomy Lab (1) Theoretical Part. Page (2 A) Page (2B)

Chapter 3. Structure and Function of the Nervous System. Copyright (c) Allyn and Bacon 2004

Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Supplemental information online for

Theory of mind skills are related to gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia

Behavioural Brain Research

Auditory fmri correlates of loudness perception for monaural and diotic stimulation

Transcription:

Functional MRI study of gender effects in brain activations during verbal working memory task Zbyněk Tüdös 1, Pavel Hok 2, Petr Hluštík 2, Aleš Grambal 3 1 Department of Radiology, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, Palacky University and University Hospital, Olomouc, Czech Republic 2 Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, Palacky University and University Hospital, Olomouc, Czech Republic 3 Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Dentistry, Palacky University and University Hospital, Olomouc, Czech Republic Corresponding author: Zbyněk Tüdös, Department of Radiology, University Hospital, I.P. Pavlova 6, 77900, Olomouc, Czech Republic. Tel. +420 588 445 995, Fax. +420 588 442 508, E-mail zbynek.tudos@seznam.cz Short title: Searching for gender effects in verbal working memory networks Summary: Neuroimaging methods have been used to study differences of brain function between males and females. Differences in working memory have been also investigated, but results of such 1

studies are mixed with respect to behavioral data, reaction times and activated brain areas. We tried to analyze functional MRI data acquired during the working memory task and search for differences of brain activation between genders. 20 healthy right-handed volunteers (10 male and 10 females) participated in the study. All of them were university students or fresh graduates. Subjects underwent block designed verbal working memory task (Item Recognition Task) inside the MRI scanner. Standard single-subject pre-processing and group fmri analyses were performed using the FEAT software from FSL library. In the behavioral data, there was no statistically significant difference in the number of correct responses during the task. The task activated similar bilateral regions of frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital lobes, basal ganglia, the brainstem and in the cerebellum, which corresponds to the previous verbal working memory neuroimaging research. In direct comparison, there was no statistically significant difference in brain activation between small samples of male and female young healthy volunteers. Keywords: fmri; verbal working memory; brain mapping; gender differences; sex differences Main body of the text Research of differences in higher cognitive functions between men and women using imaging methods is a dynamically evolving area. Frequently searched features include working 2

memory. Although there are several neuroimaging studies on the subject, the results are not consistent. Some of the studies report differences in behavioral data (Speck et al., 2000; Zilles et al., 2016), some differences in laterality of activations or different activation areas (Bell et al., 2006; Goldstein et al., 2005; Speck et al., 2000; Zilles et al., 2016), and some papers have not shown significant differences in either aspect (Haut et Barch, 2006; Schmidt et al., 2009). In order to contribute to this topic, we decided to re-analyze previously published data and to directly compare activations in males and females during the verbal memory task using functional MRI (Tüdös et al., 2011). Twenty volunteers (ten women, ten men with a mean age of 22.5 ± 1.7, resp. 24.1 ± 2.9 years, p = 0.38, Mann-Whitney U test) participated in this study. All participants were university students or recent graduates, all were right-handed. Local ethical committee approved research on healthy volunteers and all subjects participated after providing written informed consent. Modified item-recognition task in block-design was used to engage the verbal working memory (E. E. Smith et Jonides, 1998). In our setting, we used rear projection and a mirror mounted on the top of head coil to first display a slide with four target words; participants had to remember the words and their position on the screen. The target words were displayed for 4 seconds, then there was 7-second blank delay interval followed by single probe word in one of four possible positions displayed for 3 seconds. Subject had to respond, whether the probe word corresponded to one of the target words and whether it had the same position. A control condition with the same timing was used to subtract visual processing and to keep subject aware. The control task had the same timing but instead of target words we projected ordinal numbers in four positions ("first", "second", "third", "fourth"), which subjects did not have to remember and the probe word ("first" or "second") was always correct. Each single trial lasted 15 seconds. Two memory trials alternated with two control trials, thus providing 30-seconds 3

timing of block design. Six such blocks were one 6-minutes functional run, subjects underwent 2 such runs. MRI data were acquired on 1.5-Tesla scanner (Siemens Avanto, Erlangen, Germany) with a standard head coil. The subject s head was immobilized with cushions to assure maximum comfort and minimize head motion. The MR imaging protocol covered the whole brain with 30 axial slices parallel to the anterior commissure - posterior commissure (AC-PC) line, 5-mm thick, including anatomical T1-weighted images to provide an immediate overlay with functional data, fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images to screen for unsuspected brain lesions and functional T2*-weighted BOLD images during task performance and control state. BOLD images were acquired with gradient echo EPI (TR/TE = 2500/41 ms, flip angle 80, FOV = 220 mm, matrix 64 64) to provide 3.4-mm 3.4-mm 5.0-mm resolution. In total, 144 images were acquired per each 6-minute functional run. For better anatomical reference, a high-resolution 3-dimensional scan (magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo, MPRAGE) was performed as well. Numbers of correct responses in males and females was compared using two-tailed t- test. FMRI data processing was carried out using FEAT (6.00 version, FMRI Expert Analysis Tool), part of FSL (FMRIB's Software Library) (S. M. Smith et al., 2004). Standard preprocessing was applied: motion correction; slice-timing correction; non-brain tissue removal; spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 8.0 mm; and high-pass temporal filtering with sigma=30.0s. Time-series statistical analysis was carried out using FMRIB's Improved Linear Model (FILM) with local autocorrelation correction. Registration to standard space images was carried out using FMRIB's Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT). Higher-level analyses of average group activation and between-gender differences were carried out using one-sample t-tests and a two-sample t-test as implemented in FMRIB's Local Analysis of Mixed Effects (FLAME) stage 1 and 2 (Woolrich, 2008). Z (Gaussianised 4

T/F) statistic images were thresholded using clusters determined by Z>3.5 and a (corrected) cluster significance threshold of P=0.05 (Worsley K.J., 2001). Performance of the task was good in all subjects, mean number of correct responses out of the 24 possible was 22.5±1.6 in males and 22.4±1.3 in females, this difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.88). Such a result on behavioral level is consistent with most previous studies on neuroimaging of verbal working memory (Bell et al., 2006; Goldstein et al., 2005; Haut et Barch, 2006; Schmidt et al., 2009), studies that have found differences in behavioral domain are not consistent, one reporting a better score for males (Zilles et al., 2016), another for females (Speck et al., 2000). The results of fmri analyses by gender subgroups revealed similar bilateral regions of activation in the frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital lobes, basal ganglia, the brainstem and in the cerebellum (Fig.1) which are areas usually activated during verbal working memory tasks (E. E. Smith et Jonides, 1998; Tüdös et al., 2014; Wager et Smith, 2003). Local maxima were localized to the left BA 9 and 46, in the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44 and 47), in the left putamen and head of caudate bilaterally. Activations were also clearly visible in the left BA 44, BA13 and BA6. All of these areas were activated also in the right hemisphere, but left-sided dominance was obvious. Bilateral symmetrical activations were in the anterior cingulate cortex (BA 32) and the medial portion of the premotor area BA 6 (SMA). Parietal regions of activation involved bilateral intraparietal sulcus and superior parietal lobule (BA 7/40) with a predominance in the left hemisphere. The temporal lobes showed apparent activity in the posterior division of superior temporal gyrus (BA 21/22) and in both hippocampi. The occipital lobes were activated within Brodmann areas 18 and 19. Areas of activation in both cerebellar hemispheres and vermis were large and symmetric. Betweengender analysis revealed no statistically significant differences. 5

Traditionally, there are assumptions about differences between cognitive functions between males and females. Some previous neuroimaging studies report statistically significant differences between males and females during the verbal working memory task, however, these results are inconsistent there are reports of increased activation in occipital and parietal areas in 23 males compared to 10 females during the Sternberg item recognition task (Bell et al., 2006), increased bilateral activation in frontal areas in 7 females compared to 7 males during the auditory continuous performance test (similar to the 3-back task) (Goldstein et al., 2005), differences in the laterality index and overall increased activation in 9 males compared to 8 females during the 2-back task (Speck et al., 2000), and finally increased activation in the midbrain and right-sided Broca area in 139 females compared to 92 males (Zilles et al., 2016). This broad range of findings, therefore, does not indicate the existence of convincing gender differences; in general, it can also be concluded that in direct comparison the differences between the genders were significant in smaller or gender unbalanced study groups, and their power is therefore questionable. Published meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of working memory between the genders also reports the differences; according to their results, females consistently activate more limbic and prefrontal structures and males activate a distributed network including more parietal regions (Hill et al., 2014). Among the drawbacks of the meta-analysis is unbalanced number of subjects (701 male subjects in 44 studies and 200 female subjects in 15 studies). The analysis also pooled results of working memory studies regardless of the nature of the material (e.g., verbal, spatial, etc.), modality of the stimuli (e.g., visual, auditory, cross-modal, other) and difficulty of tasks (e.g., 2-back, 3- back, task timing, etc.), which can lead to misleading results. Contrary to the above-mentioned inter-gender reports, in our group, there was no statistically significant difference in brain tissue activation between males and females. Schmidt et al (Schmidt et al., 2009) have also come to the same result using 1-, 2- and 3-back task in 25 6

males and 25 females. Similarly, another team failed to replicate the statistically significant differences in activation between the genders in two different study groups (the first with 49 subjects and the second with 61 subjects) using verbal 2-back task (Haut et Barch, 2006). Our negative result can be further supported by a study that did not show any significant difference in behavioral performance between men and women in both the verbal and spatial working memory task (Kaufman, 2007). In a recently published study, gender differences in behavioral data were observed only at very high loads of working memory tasks, i.e., with increasing number of objects to maintain (Reed et al., 2017). In this perspective, working memory load in our task with four objects to remember in each trial was relatively low, which should also be considered when interpreting our negative result. Our work has several potential limitations. The main limiting factor of our study is the low number of subjects which reduces the statistical power to detect significant differences. Nevertheless, even the above cited previous studies with larger sample sizes (Haut et Barch, 2006; Schmidt et al., 2009) either did not detect any reliable gender effects, or reported them only at an uncorrected statistical threshold (Zilles et al., 2016). Second, we acknowledge that the current standard for functional neuroimaging studies is the 3T MRI scanner, whereas the 1.5T field strength used in our study provides a lower spatial and temporal resolution and lower signal-to-noise ratio compared to 3T field. To compensate for this, we acquired two functional runs per subject. Finally, the recruited subjects had a narrow range of age and uniform education level, so our results cannot be generalized to the general population. In conclusion, our data obtained on a small sample of young healthy volunteers does not indicate a significantly different brain function in the verbal working memory task between the genders. Whereas the brain networks implementing other cognitive functions 7

may be different in males and females, verbal working memory seems to be localized similarly in both genders. Acknowledgements Supported by Palacky University grant IGA_LF_2018_002 and Ministry of Health, Czech Republic - conceptual development of research organization (LF UP 61989592). References BELL, E. C., WILLSON, M. C., WILMAN, A. H., DAVE, S., SILVERSTONE, P. H. 2006. Males and females differ in brain activation during cognitive tasks. NeuroImage, 30(2):529 538. GOLDSTEIN, J. M., POLDRACK, R., BREITER, H. C., MAKRIS, N., GOODMAN, J. M., JERRAM, M., ANAGNOSON, R., TSUANG, M. T., SEIDMAN, L. J. 2005. Sex differences in prefrontal cortical brain activity during fmri of auditory verbal working memory. Neuropsychology, 19(4):509 519. HAUT, K. M., BARCH, D. M. 2006. Sex influences on material-sensitive functional lateralization in working and episodic memory: Men and women are not all that different. NeuroImage, 32(1):411 422. HILL, A. C., LAIRD, A. R., ROBINSON, J. L. 2014. Gender differences in working memory networks: A BrainMap meta-analysis. Biological Psychology, 102:18 29. KAUFMAN, S. B. 2007. Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? Intelligence, 35(3):211 223. REED, J. L., GALLAGHER, N. M., SULLIVAN, M., CALLICOTT, J. H., GREEN, A. E. 2017. Sex differences in verbal working memory performance emerge at very high loads of common neuroimaging tasks. Brain and Cognition, 113:56 64. 8

SCHMIDT, H., JOGIA, J., FAST, K., CHRISTODOULOU, T., HALDANE, M., KUMARI, V., FRANGOU, S. 2009. No gender differences in brain activation during the N-back task: an fmri study in healthy individuals. Human Brain Mapping, 30(11):3609 3615. SMITH, E. E., JONIDES, J. 1998. Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(20):12061 12068. SMITH, S. M., JENKINSON, M., WOOLRICH, M. W., BECKMANN, C. F., BEHRENS, T. E. J., JOHANSEN-BERG, H., BANNISTER, P. R., DE LUCA, M., DROBNJAK, I., FLITNEY, D. E., NIAZY, R. K., SAUNDERS, J., VICKERS, J., ZHANG, Y., DE STEFANO, N., BRADY, J. M., MATTHEWS, P. M. 2004. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23:S208 S219. SPECK, O., ERNST, T., BRAUN, J., KOCH, C., MILLER, E., CHANG, L. 2000. Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory. NeuroReport, 11(11):2581 2585. TÜDÖS, Z., HLUŠTÍK, P., HOK, P. 2011. Verbal working memory investigation using functional MRI. Ceska Radiologie, 65(1):14 18. TÜDÖS, Z., HOK, P., HRDINA, L., HLUŠTÍK, P. 2014. Modality effects in paced serial addition task: Differential responses to auditory and visual stimuli. Neuroscience, 272:10 20. WAGER, T. D., SMITH, E. E. 2003. Neuroimaging studies of working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(4):255 274. WOOLRICH, M. 2008. Robust group analysis using outlier inference. NeuroImage, 41(2):286 301. WORSLEY K.J. 2001. Statistical analysis of activation images. In: Jezzard, P., Matthews P.M., Smith S.M. (Eds.). Functional MRI: An Introduction to Methods, Oxford University Press 9

ZILLES, D., LEWANDOWSKI, M., VIEKER, H., HENSELER, I., DIEKHOF, E., MELCHER, T., KEIL, M., GRUBER, O. 2016. Gender Differences in Verbal and Visuospatial Working Memory Performance and Networks. Neuropsychobiology, 73(1):52 63. Figure 1 Group analysis for females (top part) and males (bottom part) reveals similar bilateral regions of activation in the frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital neural networks, basal ganglia, the brainstem and in the cerebellum. Between-gender analysis revealed no statistically significant differences. 10

11

12