Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation

Similar documents
Declarative memory includes semantic, episodic, and spatial memory, and

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

MARÍA ISABEL MIRANDA AND FEDERICO BERMÚDEZ-RATTONI*

Gustatory Mechanisms for Neural Signaling in Rats. Lucy Schermerhorn. Fall, 2012

The psychology department at Memorial University provided a supportive environment in which I was surrounded and encouraged by eminent scholars

Taste Neophobia and c-fos Expression in the Rat Brain

The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction

The highlight for January, 2010 is by Shadna Rana who is in the Department of Physiology and Pharmacology at University of Calgary in Alberta,

Conditioned Taste Aversion and Latent Inhibition Following Extensive Taste Preexposure in Rats with Insular Cortex Lesions

What is the Role of the Amygdala in Long Term Memory? Jack Pemment. University of Mississippi

Chapter 5: Learning and Behavior Learning How Learning is Studied Ivan Pavlov Edward Thorndike eliciting stimulus emitted

Sensory cortical processing is achieved in complex networks

Insular Cortex Lesions and Morphine-Induced Suppression of Conditioned Stimulus Intake in the Rat

C81ADD Psychology of Addiction. Alcohol. Ethyl alcohol (ethanol) School of Psychology. Tobias Bast.

Synaptic plasticity and hippocampal memory

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance

Lisa M. Giocomo & Michael E. Hasselmo

TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES ABSTRACT

The Neural Substrates of Incidental Sensory Experience. Senior Thesis. Presented to

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

BRAIN RESEARCH 1152 (2007) available at

Introduction the basics of psychological learning and memory theory. From Mechanisms of Memory by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

More dendritic spines, changes in shapes of dendritic spines More NT released by presynaptic membrane

Abstract. R. Roesler 1, J. Quevedo 1, C. Rodrigues 1, M. Madruga 1, M.R.M. Vianna 1 and M.B.C. Ferreira 2

Emotional Hormones and Memory Modulation

Following the seminal proposal of Hebb1 and many others, acquired learning

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Stress and Emotion. Stressors are things that challenge homeostasis -- these challenges may be real or merely anticipated

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

GABAergic Influences Increase Ingestion across All Taste Categories. Liz Miller. Molly McGinnis. Lindsey Richardson

ONTOGENY AND NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF THE CONTEXT PREEXPOSURE FACILITATION EFFECT ON CONTEXTUAL FEAR CONDITIONING. Felipe Schiffino

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2006 September 5.

Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala

The role of amygdala glutamate receptors in fear learning, fear-potentiated startle, and extinction

Neurotransmitters involved in Behavior. The Orchestra in the Brain

Sensory Gating Measures. Auditory P50 Response Prepulse Inhibition of Startle (PPI) Bruce Turetsky, M.D. IOM Workshop June 22, 2010

Emotion Explained. Edmund T. Rolls

Acquisition of new information can be stored into at least two

14 Taste. 14 Taste versus Flavor. Chapter 14

The Contextual Modulation of Conditioned Taste Aversions by the Physical Environment

Synap&c Plas&city. long-term plasticity (~30 min to lifetime) Long-term potentiation (LTP) / Long-term depression (LTD)

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Disruption of latent inhibition and perceptual learning

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 4 Nuts and Bolts of Conditioning (Mechanisms of Classical Conditioning)

Amygdala Strengthening of Cortical Memory Representations

MOLECULAR BIOLOGY OF DRUG ADDICTION. Sylvane Desrivières, SGDP Centre

Brain Stimulation in the Study and Treatment of Addiction. From Animal Models to Humans

Systems Neurobiology: Plasticity in the Auditory System. Jason Middleton -

Multiple Roles for Synaptic Plasticity in Pavlovian Fear Conditioning

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Effects of acute ketamine infusion on visual working memory encoding: a study using ERPs

Advanced Receptor Psychopharmacology

The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Contextual Conditioning with Lithium-Induced Nausea as the US: Evidence from a Blocking Procedure

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

GABAergic Influence on Taste Information in the Central Gustatory Pathway. Hannah Dinnen Fall 2007

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

biological psychology, p. 40 The study of the nervous system, especially the brain. neuroscience, p. 40

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

N-Methyl-D-Aspartate Receptors in the Basolateral Amygdala Are Required for Both Acquisition and Expression of Conditional Fear in Rats

Ligand-Gated Ion Channels

fuscata): Suppression of Key-Pressing 1

Short communication NORADRENERGIC STIMULATION OF THE LATERAL HYPOTHALAMUS AS A REINFORCEMENT IN T MAZE LEARNING IN RATS

PSY/NEU338: Animal learning and decision making: Psychological, computational and neural perspectives

SAMPLE EXAMINATION QUESTIONS

Lithium-induced context aversion in rats as a model of anticipatory nausea in humans

PHGY Physiology. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Sensory Receptors. Martin Paré

The Influence of Orexin Antagonist, SB , on Cognitive Flexibility

Emotional Memory, PTSD, and Clinical Intervention Updates

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF. Roger Neil Edson for the Master of Science

DRUG TREATMENT OF PARKINSON S DISEASE. Mr. D.Raju, M.pharm, Lecturer

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 9 Biological Influences on Learning

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

LAUREN C. ANDERSON Boston College, Department of Psychology Chestnut Hill, MA

Cogs 107b Systems Neuroscience lec9_ neuromodulators and drugs of abuse principle of the week: functional anatomy

Systemic administration of drugs, including adrenergic agonists

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien.

PHGY 210,2,4 - Physiology SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY. Martin Paré

Morphine-induced Suppression of Conditioned Stimulus Intake: Effects of Stimulus Type and Insular Cortex Lesions

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

Blocked and test-stimulus exposure effects in perceptual learning re-examined

Neural Communication. Central Nervous System Peripheral Nervous System. Communication in the Nervous System. 4 Common Components of a Neuron

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Network Dynamics of Basal Forebrain and Parietal Cortex Neurons. David Tingley 6/15/2012

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

Transcription:

The highlight for April is by Dr. Federico Bermudez-Rattoni from the Institute of Cellular Physiology, National University of Mexico in Mexico City, Mexico. Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni s work over the past 25 years has addressed the neuroanatomical and neurochemical mediation of taste aversion learning. Following his initial work with John Garcia on sensory gating in aversion learning in which he demonstrated a role for the basolateral amygdala in the gating of odors by tastes, he and his lab began exploring the neural mediation and integration of taste memory in aversion learning. Based on an impressive combination of behavioral, physiological, neurochemical and electrophysiological assessments, Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni and his colleagues have demonstrated that novel tastes cause the release of acetylcholine (in the insular cortex) that in turn establishes the memory trace of the gustatory event. Pre-training cholinergic deafferentation of the insular cortex disrupts aversion learning, indicating a role of ACH in its acquisition. When the animal is subsequently injected with an aversion-inducing agent, e.g., LiCl, glutamate is released (in the amygdala and insular cortex) that may be responsible for the formation of the memory for the aversive event. Through sophisticated work combining neurochemical and electrical activity in the amygdala and insular cortex, e.g., long-term potentiation induction and microinjections of glutamate, Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni demonstrated a convergence of activity in these two neural substrates (via acetylcholine and glutamate release) that results in the consolidation of taste aversion memory formation. Dr. Bermudez-Rattoni s work highlights an impressive integration of neuroscience techniques and approaches that illustrates the importance of a broad-based investigation into any area of interest. His work on taste aversion learning reveals the relative complexity of what appears to be a simple behavioral issue, i.e., the avoidance of a previously-poisoned solution. This integration and its importance are highlighted in this month s summary. Insular Cortex-Amygdala Dialogue During Taste Recognition Memory Formation Federico Bermudez-Rattoni Department of Neuroscience Institute of Cellular Physiology National University of Mexico Imagine having a very good meal or a good glass of wine, suddenly realizing that it is the best you ever had. To reach that conclusion, you should have had a lot of experience eating and drinking a variety of meals and wines in your life. Clearly, our ability to judge food and drinking in this way is rather sophisticated. In this regard, the Wall Street Journal recently reported a large scale smuggling of Coke bottles from Mexico to the US 1. A possible reason for that smuggling is that many people prefer the flavor of Mexican Coke to its US counterpart. Similar situations occur in nature in which it is not pleasure at stake, but survival. Taste recognition memory is the ability to identify a taste and relate it to past

consequences of its ingestion, an important survival skill that animals have developed throughout evolution 2. I have been involved in taste recognition memory for about 26 years, since I met John Garcia when he received the Distinguish Scientific Contribution Award from the American Psychological Association in 1979. Later, I continued my doctoral studies under his supervision at the University of California, Los Angeles. I remember that during my first interview with him, he proposed to me to find a brain and-gate that switched odors into aversive cues when they were presented together with tastes during taste aversion learning. Taste is an excellent conditioning stimulus for delayed illness, but odor is not. However, when odor and taste are combined into a compound stimulus and is followed by delayed illness, the odor becomes as aversive as taste when each is tested separately in extinction trials. Under these conditions odor aversions appear to be potentiated by taste, as if a sensory and-gate controlled by taste entered odors into food-related memory mechanisms 3-5. In a way, we succeeded in finding the and-gate in the basolateral amygdala (BLA), since the BLA, but not other brain areas, was responsible for switching odors into aversive cues when they where presented together with taste 6-9. These results have been corroborated by a number of laboratories around the world 10-15, and, furthermore, it was demonstrated that N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors in the BLA might play an important role in taste-potentiated odor aversions. That is, BLA injections of the NMDA receptor antagonist, d-2- amino-5-phosphonovalerate (APV), selectively blocked taste-potentiated odor aversions 11,13,15. It has also been found that depletion of dopaminergic terminals in the BLA produce a strong disruption of taste-potentiated odor aversions but leaves taste aversions unaltered 16,17. Therefore, it seems that the role of the amygdala in toxiphobia learning could be in the association of taste cues with visceral consequences and enabling potentiated odor conditioning by taste aversion learning. Later, I continued investigating the neural integration of taste memory. Together with students and colleagues, we were able to determine the cholinergic activity of the insular cortex (IC) and BLA during conditioned taste aversion (CTA) learning. Many of the studies at the time assessed the effects of different drugs on CTA by giving them before or after the presentation of the conditioned and the unconditioned stimuli (CS-US) pairing. Instead, we determined acetylcholine (ACh) release by using in vivo microdialysis to a CS separate from the US. Thus, we demonstrated that novel taste produced a strong release of ACh in the IC, whereas after several presentations of the stimulus the cortical ACh release decreased to the same levels as those produced by the familiar ones, indicating an inverse relationship between familiarity and cortical ACh release 18,19. Similarly, specific cholinergic deafferentation restricted to the cortex produced a significant disruption in the acquisition of CTA. However, the same animals were able to recall the taste aversion when the conditioning trial was established before deafferentation 20,21. These results suggest that cortical

cholinergic activity is involved in the recognition of taste novelty, but it is no longer necessary to recall aversive stimuli. From these results, we were interested in determining if ACh activity was involved in signaling CS novelty. Although, it was known that cortical administration of scopolamine, a muscarinic receptor antagonist, blocked CTA, it remained unknown if that blockade would interfere with the taste or novelty perception. To better understand the role of ACh activity on gustatory memory trace, we used a learning protocol described in the 70s 22. When an animal drinks a novel taste solution, it innately hesitates to drink it, reducing its consumption (neophobia) until its post-digestive consequence has been assessed. Unlike CTA, when taste is followed by absence of malaise, an incremental increase in its consumption is observed, which is called attenuation of neophobia. Therefore, we assessed the effects of muscarinic receptor antagonists by giving microinjections of scopolamine or the NMDA-receptor antagonist APV into the IC on neophobia and attenuation of neophobia. The neophobic response was induced by presentation of a strong novel saccharin solution (0.5%) that was not affected by the injection of either drug given before the taste presentation, ruling out any impairment in taste perception or novelty discrimination. However, attenuation of neophobia was impaired by microinjection of scopolamine given before or after the first taste experience 23,24. However, APV did not affect the attenuation of neophobia, suggesting that this learning is independent of NMDA receptor activity 23. These results indicate that ACh activity is involved in the formation of the gustatory trace and that post-digestive consequences will produce safe (attenuation of neophobia) or aversive (CTA) gustatory memory formation. Thus, we were interested in how a dialogue is established between the BLA and the IC during taste aversion memory formation. In this regard, we demonstrated that there is a functional communication between the BLA and IC by means of induced Long-Term Potentiation (LTP). We were able to demonstrate a significant increase of synaptic responses in the IC to low frequency stimulation after in vivo tetanic stimulation of the BLA in adult rats 25 and that intracortical administration of APV disrupted the inducted LTP 26,27. In addition, induction of LTP in the BLA-IC projection before CTA training enhanced the retention of this task 28. These results point to a cortical functional facilitation induced by BLA stimulation that seems to be regulated by glutamate (Glu) through activation of NMDA receptors. In conclusion, the induced LTP in the BLA-IC projection is a possible mechanism for memory-related functions performed by the cortex. Thus, we decided to further investigate the possible role of Glu and if the US and the CS could differentially activate it. We demonstrated by in vivo microdialysis that the US (i.p. injection of LiCl), but not the saccharin presentation, induced a dramatic increase in Glu release in the BLA and a modest but significant release in the

IC 29. In addition, reliable and robust taste aversions could be elicited by intraamygdala microinjections of Glu. That is, when Glu is administered in the BLA just before the presentation of a weak US (low LiCl dose), a clear CTA is produced 29,30. Furthermore, blockade of cortical NMDA receptors by APV applied before the novel taste impaired the induced taste aversions by the combined Glu injections and low doses of LiCl in the BLA 30. Altogether, these results suggest that cholinergic activity is involved in the taste memory trace, whereas glutamatergic activity participates in aversive memory formation. In addition, they indicate that Glu has the function of signaling at least partly the visceral input (US) that would eventually converge with the CS signal during consolidation of taste aversivion memory formation. Altogether, these experiments demonstrate that behavioral enhancement of taste aversion could be induced by electrical (high frequency stimulation, LTP) or pharmacological (applications of Glu) stimulation of the BLA. The facilitations of CTA induced by BLA-LTP or by BLA- Glu infusions can be reversed by cortical infusions of NMDA receptor antagonists. In summary, we are proposing that there are at least two forms of taste memory, i.e., safe and aversive, and that an active functional communication between the BLA and IC is established during taste memory formation. Thus, safe taste memory seems to involve cholinergic neurotransmission and its downstream signaling pathways. However, if a taste cue is followed by induction of visceral malaise it produces a release of Glu in the BLA and in the IC activating the NMDA receptors. If the US signal activation takes place during the few hours following consumption of the new taste, it interferes with the safe taste consolidation transforming the taste representation from safe to aversive trace and produces a long-lasting aversive memory trace 31. References 1 Terhune, C. Taste Test: U.S. Thirst for Mexican Cola Poses Sticky Problem for Coke. Wall Street Journal. (Eastern edition), A1 (2006). 2 Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Molecular mechanisms of taste-recognition memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 5, 209-17 (2004). 3 Bermudez-Rattoni, F. & Yamamoto, T. in Conditioned Taste Aversion. Memory of a Special Kind (eds. Bures, J., Bemudez-Rattoni, F. & Yamamoto, T.) 28-44 (Oxford University Press, New York, 1998). 4 Garcia, J., Lasiter, P. S., Bermudez-Rattoni, F. & Deems, D. A. A general theory of aversion learning. Ann N Y Acad Sci 443, 8-21 (1985). 5 Palmerino, C. C., Rusiniak, K. W. & Garcia, J. Flavor-illness aversions: the peculiar roles of odor and taste in memory for poison. Science 208, 753-5 (1980). 6 Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Rusiniak, K. W. & Garcia, J. Flavor-illness aversions: potentiation of odor by taste is disrupted by application of novocaine into amygdala. Behav Neural Biol 37, 61-75 (1983). 7 Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Grijalva, C. V., Kiefer, S. W. & Garcia, J. Flavor-illness aversions: the role of the amygdala in the acquisition of taste-potentiated odor aversions. Physiol Behav 38, 5038 (1986).

8 Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Forthman, D. L., Sanchez, M. A., Perez, J. L. & Garcia, J. Odor and taste aversions conditioned in anesthetized rats. Behav Neurosci 102, 726-32 (1988). 9 Bermudez-Rattoni, F., Coburn, K. L., Fernandez, J., Chavez, A. F. & Garcia, J. Potentiation of odor by taste and odor aversions in rats are regulated by cholinergic activity of dorsal hippocampus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 26, 553-9 (1987). 10 Ferry, B., Sandres G. & Di Scala, G. Neuroanatomical and Functional Specificity of the Basolateral Amygdaloid Nucleus in Taste-Potenatiated Odor Aversion. Neurobiol Learn Mem 84, 169-180 (1995). 11 Ferry, B. & Di Scala, G. Basolateral amygdala NMDA receptors are selectively involved in the acquisition of taste-potentiated odor aversion in the rat. Behav Neurosci 114, 1005-10 (2000). 12 Hatfield, T., Graham, P. W. & Gallagher, M. Taste-potentiated odor aversion learning: role of the amygdaloid basolateral complex and central nucleus. Behav Neurosci 106, 286-93 (1992). 13 Hatfield, T. & Gallagher, M. Taste-potentiated odor conditioning: impairment produced by infusion of an N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist into basolateral amygdala. Behav Neurosci 109, 663-8 (1995). 14 Sakai, N. & Yamamoto, T. Effects of excitotoxic brain lesions on taste-mediated odor learning in the rat. Neurobiol Learn Mem 75, 128-39 (2001). 15 Willner, J., Gallagher, M., Graham, P. W. & Crooks, G. B., Jr. N-methyl-Daspartate antagonist D-APV selectively disrupts taste-potentiated odor aversion learning. Behav Neurosci 106, 315-23 (1992). 16 Fernandez-Ruiz, J. et al. Adrenal medullary grafts restore olfactory deficits and catecholamine levels of 6-OHDA amygdala lesioned animals. J Neural Transplant Plast 4, 289-97 (1993). 17 Fernandez-Ruiz, J., Miranda, M. I., Bermudez-Rattoni, F. & Drucker-Colin, R. Effects of catecholaminergic depletion of the amygdala and insular cortex on the potentiation of odor by taste aversions. Behav Neural Biol 60, 189-91 (1993). 18 Miranda, M. I. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Reversible inactivation of the nucleus basalis magnocellularis induces disruption of cortical acetylcholine release and acquisition, but not retrieval, of aversive memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96, 6478-82 (1999). 19 Miranda, M. I., Ramirez-Lugo, L. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Cortical cholinergic activity is related to the novelty of the stimulus. Brain Res 882, 230-5 (2000). 20 Gutierrez, H., Miranda, M. I. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Learning impairment and cholinergic deafferentation after cortical nerve growth factor deprivation. J Neurosci 17, 3796-803 (1997). 21 Gutierrez, H. et al. Differential effects of 192IgG-saporin and NMDA-induced lesions into the basal forebrain on cholinergic activity and taste aversion memory formation. Brain Res 834, 136-41 (1999). 22 Domjan, M. Determinants of the enhancement of flavored-water intake by prior exposure. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2, 17-27 (1976). 23 Gutierrez, R., Tellez, L. A. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Blockade of cortical muscarinic but not NMDA receptors prevents a novel taste from becoming familiar. Eur J Neurosci 17, 1556-62 (2003). 24 Gutierrez, R., Rodriguez-Ortiz, C., De la Cruz, V., Nunez-Jaramillo, L. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Cholinergic dependence of taste memory formation: Evidence of two distinct processes. Neurobiol Learn Mem 80, 323-331 (2003). 25 Escobar, M. L., Chao, V. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. In vivo long-term potentiation in the insular cortex: NMDA receptor dependence. Brain Res 779, 314-9 (1998).

26 Escobar, M. L., Alcocer, I. & Chao, V. The NMDA receptor antagonist CPP impairs conditioned taste aversion and insular cortex long-term potentiation in vivo. Brain Res 812, 246-51 (1998). 27 Escobar, M. L., Alcocer, I. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. In vivo effects of intracortical administration of NMDA and metabotropic glutamate receptors antagonists on neocortical long-term potentiation and conditioned taste aversion. Behav Brain Res 129, 101-6 (2002). 28 Escobar, M. L. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Long-term potentiation in the insular cortex enhances conditioned taste aversion retention. Brain Res 852, 208-12 (2000). 29 Miranda, M. I., Ferreira, G., Ramirez-Lugo, L. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Glutamatergic activity in the amygdala signals visceral input during taste memory formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99, 11417-22 (2002). 30 Ferreira, G., Miranda M.I., De la Cruz, V., Rodriguez-Ortiz, C.J. and Bermudez- Rattoni, F. Basolateral amygdala glutamatergic activation enhances taste aversion through NMDA receptor activation in the insular cortex. Eur J Neurosci 22, 2596-2604 (2005). 31 Ferreira, G., Gutierrez, R., De La Cruz, V. & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. Differential involvement of cortical muscarinic and NMDA receptors in short-and long-term taste aversion memory. Eur J Neurosci 16, 1139-45 (2002).