Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Similar documents
Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging with Arterial Spin Labeling: Techniques and Potential Clinical and Research Applications

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Personal Space Regulation by the Human Amygdala. California Institute of Technology

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Group-Wise FMRI Activation Detection on Corresponding Cortical Landmarks

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Online Content

Supplementary Information

Repeatability of 2D FISP MR Fingerprinting in the Brain at 1.5T and 3.0T

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

International Journal of Innovative Research in Advanced Engineering (IJIRAE) Volume 1 Issue 10 (November 2014)

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

BRAIN STATE CHANGE DETECTION VIA FIBER-CENTERED FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY ANALYSIS

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Temporal preprocessing of fmri data

The Effects of Music intervention on Functional connectivity. Supplemental Information

Visualization strategies for major white matter tracts identified by diffusion tensor imaging for intraoperative use

Incorporation of Imaging-Based Functional Assessment Procedures into the DICOM Standard Draft version 0.1 7/27/2011

Functional Elements and Networks in fmri

A number form area in the blind

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Final Report 2017 Authors: Affiliations: Title of Project: Background:

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

Experimental Assessment of Infarct Lesion Growth in Mice using Time-Resolved T2* MR Image Sequences

ROC Analysis of Statistical Methods Used in Functional MRI: Individual Subjects

Supporting Online Material for

ASSESSING MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION WITH ADDITIVE FACTORS AND FUNCTIONAL MRI

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

High Field MR of the Spine

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Tissue-engineered medical products Evaluation of anisotropic structure of articular cartilage using DT (Diffusion Tensor)-MR Imaging

Inverse problems in functional brain imaging Identification of the hemodynamic response in fmri

Presence of AVA in High Frequency Oscillations of the Perfusion fmri Resting State Signal

Activated Fibers: Fiber-centered Activation Detection in Task-based FMRI

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Supporting Information

Tactile Auditory Shape Learning Engages the Lateral Occipital Complex

FREQUENCY DOMAIN HYBRID INDEPENDENT COMPONENT ANALYSIS OF FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING DATA

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

An Unified Approach for fmri-measurements used by a New Real-Time fmri Analysis System

MR QA/QC for MRgRT. Rick Layman, PhD, DABR Department of Radiology July 13, 2015

UK Biobank. Imaging modality Cardiovascular Magnetic Resonance (CMR) Version th Oct 2015

Temporal preprocessing of fmri data

Magnetic Resonance Angiography

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Supplementary Online Content

Daniel Bulte. Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain. University of Oxford

Supporting Information

RECENT ADVANCES IN CLINICAL MR OF ARTICULAR CARTILAGE

Supplementary Materials

What do you think of the following research? I m interested in whether a low glycemic index diet gives better control of diabetes than a high

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Q-Sense fmri in the MRI environment

Source Memory is not Properly Differentiated from Object Memory in Early Schizophrenia An fmri study using virtual reality

Reading Assignments: Lecture 18: Visual Pre-Processing. Chapters TMB Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

fmri Acquisition: Temporal Effects

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Supplemental Information

Seamless pre-surgical fmri and DTI mapping

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

5/2/2013. Real-time fmri: Methods and applications

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Neuroimaging vs. other methods

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

P2 Visual - Perception

Decoding the future from past experience: learning shapes predictions in early visual cortex

Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Stroke Patients After Upper Limb Robot-Assisted Therapy: A Pilot Study

doi: /brain/awq006 Brain 2010: 133; Imaging memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: predicting the effects of temporal lobe resection

Consonant Perception test

MR Advance Techniques. Vascular Imaging. Class II

Design Optimization. SBIRC/SINAPSE University of Edinburgh

Supporting Information

Predicting Perceptual Performance From Neural Activity

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

Task-specific reorganization of the auditory cortex in deaf humans

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

Prediction of Successful Memory Encoding from fmri Data

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Reporting Checklist for Nature Neuroscience

Category: Life sciences Name: Seul Lee SUNetID: seul

Seeing through the tongue: cross-modal plasticity in the congenitally blind

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Transcription:

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods Subjects The experiment included twelve subjects: ten sighted subjects and two blind. Five of the ten sighted subjects were expert users of a visual-to-auditory SSD called The voice, and the other five had learned associations between voice sounds and object names without interpreting the shape information in the sounds. All sighted subjects beside one were females, and all were aged 18 to 25. The two blind subjects were also expert users of The voice and employed this SSD in their daily lives. Subject A.S. is a 37 year old male, congenitally blind due to retinopathy of prematurity. At the time of testing, he was able to see faint light but not forms, shapes or colors. P.F. is a 45 year old female who became profoundly blind at the age of 21 secondary to a chemical accident. Her residual vision was limited to faint light perception in her right eye (the left eye had been enucleated). All subjects were right-handed as assessed by the Oldfield questionnaire had normal hearing, and had no neurological or psychiatric conditions. Sighted experts training procedures: Subjects were trained for 20 consecutive weekdays. Firstly, subjects were played a voice sound (soundscape) and asked to select from four choices the picture that it corresponded to. When they chose incorrectly, they were shown the correct answer and given feedback by an investigator. These multiple choice questions increased in difficulty as the training progressed. Lines and simple shapes were used for the easy stimuli, capital letters and more complex shapes for more difficult stimuli, and pictures of real objects for the most difficult stimuli. We created a library of hundreds of object related soundscapes so that each stimulus was effectively novel and subjects were not able to learn by simple pair association. Instead, they had to interpret the shape information contained in the soundscape to pick the correct answer. Secondly, subjects were blindfolded and set up with video-camera glasses, a laptop running The voice software, and headphones. They were then asked to use The voice to see and locate various objects placed in front of them. 1

During training, subjects were tested with multiple choice questions to assess their ability to interpret novel voice soundscapes. The same tests were used each week to monitor progress, and subjects were never offered feedback on these tests. All subjects achieved a minimum level of 50% success on these multiple choice tests by the end of the training period; since each question offered four possible choices, this represented twice the level of success expected by chance. Our subjects achieved an average level of 69.5% correct by the end of training (range 53% to 86%). Sighted association subjects training procedures: Each subject had 3-5 training sessions of 2 hours with the 8 associations required for the fmri experiment (see general experimental design). During these sessions subjects were played the voice soundscapes and told the names of the objects represented by the sounds. This process was repeated until the subjects were able to accurately recognize and name the 8 sounds required. The variable length of training represents the variable time it took the control subjects to learn the associations. The criterion for success was 100% accurate recognition in two complete sets of the 8 stimuli (16/16 associations), repeated on two consecutive days. These subjects were not told how the sounds were created instead they were told that the sounds had been randomly generated. After the scanning session subjects were debriefed. They were questioned about how they had learned the associations to exclude subjects that they had developed a strategy involving shape. No subjects had to be excluded on these grounds. To demonstrate a behavioral difference between our two groups we also asked these subjects to attempt the multiple choices testing (on novel soundscapes) that our sighted experts took. This was carried out after the scanning so that any learning that might have occurred during the testing did not impact on the fmri data. The association subjects achieved an average of 30.8% success (range 20%-36%), which, as expected, was significantly worse than our experts (P=0.004, Wilcoxon rank sum test). No association subject reached our success threshold for voice expert subjects, which was 50% success. 2

General Experiment design: Plastic toys were obtained representing the following objects: Horse, Cow, Elephant, Rooster, Jeep, Saw, Hammer and Whistle. These objects were chosen because they are easily recognizable both by their shape and by the sounds they make. Photographs of the objects were taken and converted into soundscapes using The voice visuo-auditory conversion algorithm (Suppl Fig 2). Short recordings were obtained of the sounds made by these objects (e.g. the banging of a hammer). Testing Protocol During the scanning, subjects were first given auditory instructions through a pair of MRI compatible headphones. During voiceobj, AudObj and TacObj subjects were then played two sounds of the same type (e.g. voice, scramble, animal/object noise etc) each repeated 3 times. Each sound lasted 2 seconds. Each pair of trials lasted a total of 12 seconds and was terminated with the auditory instruction stop. An 8 second rest period followed, before the next trial. Subjects had to recognize each object and covertly name the object s identity. Data acquisition began only when subjects were able to correctly identify all 8 objects inside the scanner (overtly). During half of the runs, subjects were asked to identify the stimuli as man-made or animal, and to respond via a two-button response box. This allowed verification that the subjects were correctly identifying objects during the scanning. To control for hand movements, subjects had to press the response buttons randomly also during the control conditions after each cue for a stimulus (AudScr, voicescr, SenMot). A sample trial is shown in the schematic figure below: Instruction listen stop Stimulus Hammer Soundscape X 3 Elephant Soundscape X 3 Time (s) 1 6 6 8 3

Data analysis and MRI acquisition. Data analysis was performed using the Brain Voyager QX 1.8 software package (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, Netherlands). Preprocessing included head motion correction, slice scan time correction and high-pass temporal smoothing in the frequency domain to remove drifts and to improve signal to noise ratio. To compute statistical parametric maps we applied a general linear model (GLM) using predictors convoluted with a typical hemodynamic response function (Boynton, 1996). We used a statistical threshold criterion of p < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons using a cluster-size threshold adjustment. This was done based on the Forman et al. Monte Carlo stimulation approach, extended into 3D data sets using the threshold size plug-in BrainVoyager QX. (For the original algorithm see Forman, S.D. et al. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn. Reson. Med. 33, 636-647 (1995)). The Cluster Threshold plug-in implements a randomization technique to estimate a cluster-level confidence on the current overlaid 3D maps (VMR/VMP) given the current voxel-level confidence level. The intrinsic smoothness of the map is estimated (4mm kernel) and a user-specified number of simulations (the recommended 1000 iterations in our case) is performed to assign an alpha-value to each active cluster. Based on the simulations, a minimum cluster size threshold is set for the current map to achieve a corrected p of 0.05. The resulting 3D volume (VMR/VMP) maps were then projected on the individual cortex reconstruction. For the group results (Fig. 1c, Fig. 2a,b and Suppl. Fig. 3) we used a random-effects GLM analysis with correction for multiple comparisons. 3D recording and cortex reconstruction Separate 3D recordings were used for surface reconstruction. High resolution 3D anatomical volumes were collected using high-resolution T1-weighted images using a 3D-turbo field echo (TFE) T1-weighted sequence (equivalent to MP-RAGE). Typical parameters were: Field of View (FOV) 23cm (RL) x 23cm (VD) x 17cm (AP); Foldover-axis: RL, data matrix: 160x160x144 zero-filled to 256 in all directions (approx 1mm isovoxel native data), TR/TE=9ms/6ms, flip angle = 8deg. Acquisition was segmented x 3 in order to enhance gray/white matter contrast. NEX = 2 in separate acquisitions. The 4

parallel imaging head coil (SENSE head) was used to reduce scan time. Reduction factor = 2.5 (total acquisition time: 5 minutes). fmri recording parameters The BOLD fmri measurements were performed in a whole-body 3-T, Philips scanner equipped with 22 mt/m field gradients with a slew rate of 120 T/m/s (Echospeed). The pulse sequence used was the gradient-echo echo planar imaging EPI sequence. We used 30-33 slices of 3mm thickness, with an interslice gap of 1 mm. Data in-plane matrix size was 128x128, field of view (FOV) 24cm x 24cm, time to repetition (TR) = 3000ms and time to echo (TE) = 35ms. Each experiment had 180 data point with four repetitions. The first five images (during the first baseline rest condition) were excluded from the analysis because of non-steady state magnetization. Percent signal change analysis For the LO/LOtv signal magnitude analysis, activation was sampled from the peak occipito-temporal voxel of the left hemisphere (Fig. 2c) and the right hemisphere (Suppl. Fig. 4) for the tactile objects versus sensory-motor control contrast and averaged across the four runs (using in each subject separately the peak voxel in a smoothed volume, after convolution with a Gaussian kernel of 4 mm full width at half maximum). The averaged percent signal change and standard errors were then calculated for each condition. 5