Physiological Responses in the Anticipation of an Auditory. and Visual Stimulus

Similar documents
Effects of Auditory Stimuli on Blood Pressure, Respiration Rate, and Heart Rate Changes While Watching a Suspenseful Video

Feed-forward response of three physiological factors in men and women due to a startle stimulus

The Effect of Induced Stress on Fine Motor Accuracy

The Effect of Sympathetic Nervous System Stimulation on Skeletal Muscle Reflex Response Time

Political Affiliation as a Predictor of Physiological Response to Political Statements

Effects of Different Auditory Stimulation on Physiological Stress Response

The Effects Virtual Reality Has on Physiological Responses as Compared to Two-Dimensional Video

Physiological Fear Response Amplitude in Visual Stimuli vs Audio-Visual Stimuli: A Comparative Gender Study

The Effect of Emotion on Cognitive Flexibility Using the Stroop Test While Monitoring Heart Rate, Respiration Rate, and Electrodermal Activity Levels

Visual Stimulus Effect on Jump Scares. Abigail Austin, Connor Butters, Brianna Juda, Hannah Mulford

Experimental Design Figure 1

Activity Vital Signs: Heart Rate and Blood Pressure

Short Term Physiological Effects of Yoga on Relieving Acute Stress Induced by Task-Anxiety

Effects of Residential Habituation on Physiological Response to Auditory Stimuli. University of Wisconsin - Madison

Power and Stature: Analysis of the. Human Physiological Response to Stress

Regulation of Human Heart Rate

Media Delivery Method Shows No Effect On Physiological Stress Response Factors of Heart Rate, Blood Pressure and Respiration

EMOTION. Joy, Sorrow, Anger, Fear, Love, Hate, Happiness

POLYGRAPH BIOFEEDBACK

Student Name: Pre-Lab: Homeostasis Notes, Body Temperature Activity + Video; Introduction in this Lab.

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

Pupil dilation accuracy in the prediction of random events. The name, affiliation, and address for the lead experimenter(s) for the study:

Learning Outcome: To what extent do cognitive and biological factors interact in emotion?

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

Learning and conditioning

Psychology for Nurses EMOTION. By Dr. Hayder Al-hadrawi University of Babylon, College of Nursing

Stress Response Associated with Finishing a Test Slower than One s Peers

FACTORS INFLUENCING EDA DURING NARRATIVE EXPERIENCES BY SAM SPAULDING & JULIE LEGAULT FOR AFFECTIVE COMPUTING, FALL 2013

Effects of an Auditory Stimulus on the Nervous System and Selective Attention

Effects of Gender Stereotype Threat on Physiological Stress Response. Joey Janz, Caitlin Kuckes, Linda Mei, Hasan Nadeem, Emerald Reeg, Rachel Smitz

Exposures, Flooding, & Desensitization. Anxiety Disorders. History 12/2/2009

Emotion Theory. Dr. Vijay Kumar

Introduction to Psychology. Lecture no: 27 EMOTIONS

Situation Modification. Taylor Vonk. Northeastern University

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Heart Rate and Blood Pressure as Vital Signs

The Effect of Aromatherapy on Reduction of Physical Stress

Signs of Acute Stress Disorder Symptom Behavioral Signs Support Needed

Learning theory provides the basis for behavioral interventions. The USMLE behavioral science section always contains questions relating to learning

Theories and Physiology of Emotion. AP Psychology Module 41

Effects of Music Tempo on the Stress Response to Short Term Memory and Problem Solving Tests

PSY402 Theories of Learning. Chapter 4 (Cont.) Indirect Conditioning Applications of Conditioning

Psychological preparation for natural disasters

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015

Relationship of Terror Feelings and Physiological Response During Watching Horror Movie

The Effects of Music Genre on Physiological Measures of Stress from Arithmetic Testing

science-u.org What affects blood Blood Pressure Directions You Will Need BEST FOR GRADES 5-8 ESTIMATED TIME Minutes

LOUDOUN ACADEMY PHYSICAL EDUCATION DEPARTMENT NATIONAL 5 PORTFOLIO PREPARATION

SLEEP THEORIES. Sleep Protects: Sleeping in the darkness when predators loomed

Positive emotion expands visual attention...or maybe not...

Chapter 8. What Is Emotion? What Do Our Emotions Do For Us? Emotion and Motivation

Linking Color and Emotions. Erika Harper, Sierra Wilkie, Keith Kilpatrick, Emily Pederson & Kari Smith. Broome Community College

April 9, 2015 Keywords: audio, auditory, baby, crying, emotion, EDA, EEG, pulse oximetry, sex, stress

Learning and Anxiety 1

Last month I reviewed the behavior-based principles of safety self-management.

The Complexity of Fear Are you experiencing anxiety, or is it fear?

Stress and its vegetative symptoms can be greatly reduced through biofeedback training, where you intentionally work on lowering your skin response.

RESEARCH ARTICLE. Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, Italy

Tips for Writing a Research Paper in APA format:

acquisition associative learning behaviorism B. F. Skinner biofeedback

Can Stress Make You Sick?

Psychology in Your Life

PATIENT INFORMATION. Medicine To Treat: H ypertension. Patient Awareness and Self-Care

The role of music characteristics on drivers anger regulation

Chapter 29 1/8/2018. Vital Signs. Measuring and Reporting Vital Signs. Key Terms

The Brave Child How to Help Your Child Adapt, Move Forward, and Thrive (Even When They Are Scared)

iworx Sample Lab Experiment HP-7: Hypothesis-driven Biofeedback Lab/Research Study

Dude, calm down! De-escalating driving anger using in-car information

AFFORDABLE TECHNOLOGY

Short Term Physiological Effects of Meditation on Induced. Task-Related Stress

Learning. 3. Which of the following is an example of a generalized reinforcer? (A) chocolate cake (B) water (C) money (D) applause (E) high grades

Emotion. Cannon-Brad Theory. Display Rules. Schacter s Two-Factor Theory. Cognitive Appraisal Theory. Lateralization of Emotion. James-Lange Theory

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

In each of us are heroes; Speak to them and they will come forth. Anonymous

Analysis of the Stressful Event. her bed. She soon saw there was a man creeping along side her bed. When she began to scream,

Response to Commentaries on Multiple Arousal. Theory and Daily-Life Electrodermal Activity Asymmetry

Drive-reducing behaviors (eating, drinking) Drive (hunger, thirst) Need (food, water)

The Effects of Rating Accuracy on Treatment Fidelity

Sensory Processing Disorder

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder

Fear of faces: a psychophysiological investigation of facial affect processing in social phobia

Learning Theories Reviewed

Lesson 2 The Experimental Method

THE EFFECT OF LIFESTYLE AND GENDER ON CO 2 OUTPUT BEFORE AND AFTER PHYSICAL ACTIVITY JULY 22, 2016

PhysioPilot C2 Plus STRESS PROFILE APPLICATIONS

2. Measure a subject's blood pressure and heart rate both at rest and during exercise.

Evaluating the Evidence for Paranormal Phenomena

Raw BP and Eight Hemodynamic Measurements Displayed in Real Time

Psychology in Your Life

Protocol 4: Measuring Blood Pressure

Overview. Basic concepts Theories of emotion Universality of emotions Brain basis of emotions Applied research: microexpressions

Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions

Treatment of Anxiety as a Cooccurring Disorder

Guided Reading Activity 15-1 Sources of Stress

Chapter 13: From Social Cognition to Affect

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

Psychology and ME/CFS: Not all in the mind... Alex Howard Dr. Megan Arroll

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

The only thing we have to fear is fear itself. These. famous words were spoken by Franklin Delanor Roosevelt

LAB 9: Metabolic Rates

Transcription:

Physiological Responses in the Anticipation of an Auditory and Visual Stimulus John Bruckbauer, Audrey Entwistle, Bilal Malas, Shauna Patin University of Wisconsin-Madison, Department of Physiology Physiology 435 Lab 601, Group 14 Key Words: Electrodermal activity (EDA), heart rate, blood pressure, anticipation, stimulus Word Count: 2941

Introduction Fear and anxiety are basic emotions seen amongst humans as a means of signaling danger, threat, or motivational conflict and to trigger appropriate adaptive responses. Several different stimuli may induce fear in people. One example is a sudden and unexpected sound or visual stimulus, which are abundant in horror films and other scary videos (Steimer, 2002). Therefore, watching a short horror video with such stimuli should prompt fear in those watching it. According to a study involving fear responses in humans, audiovisual stimuli from a scary video clip caused an increase in heart rate and eye movement, with EDA showing an upward trend, although not significant (Diehl et al., 2017). This experiment shows that audiovisual stimuli are capable of producing fear or fear-like emotions, and that several physiological changes are associated with the emotion. These physiological changes may be caused by activation of the sympathetic nervous system, which is induced by fear or in response to a threat (Steimer, 2002). The body also contains mechanisms that allow us to anticipate and prepare for certain stimuli and the future effects they will have on the body. These mechanisms are mediated by feedforward regulation. The mechanisms lead to changes in physiological variables before the stimulus occurs which improves the speed of homeostatic responses and minimizes fluctuations in the regulated variable (Widmaier et al., 2016). A common example of feedforward regulation involves alterations in blood pressure and respiration when a person anticipates physical activity (Widmaier et al., 2016). In this example, these physiological variables change in advance due to feedforward regulation so that the body will not have to rapidly work to maintain homeostasis during the physical activity. A study on the impact of anticipation in response to arousing stimuli showed that several physiological variables, such as EDA and heart rate, significantly increased (Mossbridge et al., 2012). This demonstrates the body s ability to experience physiological

changes, likely as a result of feedforward regulation to an arousing stimulus, which can include fear. The purpose of our study is to attempt to find the relationship between the anticipation of fear-inducing stimuli and any corresponding physiological changes that result prior to the showing of the stimuli. The significance of our study is that it gives more insight into the quick response time of the sympathetic nervous system and how the body reacts to a short-term fearful situation, which is common in everyday life. It will give us more information on deviance from homeostasis in response to external stimuli and the complex link between emotion and physiology. It will also reveal if knowing about a scary situation is beneficial to maintaining homeostasis. Our hypothesis is that anticipation of fear will induce statistically significant physiological changes in blood pressure, heart rate, and EDA before the frightening stimulus occurs. Heart rate, blood pressure, and EDA are measurements that can be used to demonstrate that the anticipation of fear leads to the activation of the sympathetic nervous system. We hope to learn whether warning subjects of a potential emotional reaction will lead to measurable changes in these variables as part of a physiological response. We chose to use a scary video containing abrupt visual and auditory stimuli that have been shown to induce fear and cause certain elevated physiological variables due to activation of the startle reflex (Quevedo et al., 2010). Blood pressure, heart rate, and EDA are all physiological measurements commonly used in studies to measure the physiological response to an emotional stimulus. Each of these variables have been shown to increase in response to fear. EDA is widely used to measure emotional activation and processing in physiological studies (Braithwaite et al., 2015). Also, the sympathetic nervous system has been shown to elicit an increases in arterial pressure, heart rate, and respirations when activated (Funkenstein, 1955). The results from the experiments mentioned above, especially the study regarding the physiological changes caused by scary

audiovisual stimuli and anticipation with arousing stimuli, reinforce our decision to measure EDA, heart rate, and blood pressure, as they have all been shown to increase due to fear. Materials The variables tested for physiological change include heart rate, blood pressure, and electrodermal activity (EDA). Heart rate and blood pressure were obtained using an automatic blood pressure monitor (Model BP791IT, Omron Healthcare Inc. Lake Forest, IL) to measure beats per minute and systolic and diastolic pressures in mmhg. EDA was tested using finger plethysmography (Model: SS3LA, Biopac Systems Inc., Goleta, CA) measured in microsiemens. We used a computer with working internet capable of playing a scary video in order to invoke an emotional response via auditory and visual stimuli. The video was a jump scare video that starts with a car driving and is interrupted with a screaming face. It was taken from YouTube. Methods Procedure This study was conducted throughout the spring semester of 2018 for the Physiology 435 course at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. Participants were students of the university ranging from age 20-22. The study took place in the Medical Sciences building on the University of Wisconsin-Madison campus. Participants sat down at a table and filled out a consent form. The door to the room was closed. Measurement devices (EDA and blood pressure cuff) were connected to the subject. For each subject, the EDA electrodes were placed on the index and middle fingers of the right hand. A conducting gel was used to ensure proper data collection. The blood pressure cuff was placed on the upper left arm. A group member asked the subject a set of pre-study survey

questions to collect their demographics. The subject sat for a total of five minutes, starting at the point when they sat down at the table, and then baseline measurements were collected. EDA was continuously recorded throughout the duration of the experiment. Heart rate and blood pressure were measured three separate times during the experiment as stated below. At six minutes from the point when the participant sat down at the table, subjects were either informed that they were going to be watching a video or that they were going to be watching a video that was meant to scare them. Groups were randomly assigned. Odd numbered participants were told it was a video meant to scare them and even numbered participants were told it was a video. As they were being told, a second blood pressure and heart rate measurement was initiated. At six minutes and 45 seconds, lights were turned off and all subjects were shown the scary video on the computer. Immediately after the video was over, a third blood pressure and heart rate measurement was collected. EDA measurements stopped when the final blood pressure was finished. The subject then had all measurement devices removed. All subjects were given a post-study survey to determine how much the video scared them and their mindset after being told about the video but before the video was shown. They were then debriefed. A flow chart of the methods can be seen in Figure 1. Data Analysis Percent change was calculated for each participant from their baseline to the point when they were either told they would be watching a video that was meant to scare them (experimental) or a video (control). An unpaired t-test with a 95% confidence interval was used to determine any statistical significance between the experimental group and the control group for each category (heart rate, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and EDA). Averages for heart rate, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and EDA at different

time points in the experiment between the experimental and control groups were also compared to look at trends in the data. Positive Control Our positive controls were the subjects that were not informed that it was a scary video. They should not have shown an increase in heart rate, blood pressure, or EDA before the video was shown since they did not anticipate it to be scary. After the video, their heart rate, blood pressure, and EDA should have increased. The results indicated that a physiological change could be obtained from the stimulus using the specified equipment. Negative Control We conducted baseline physiological tests for each subject. The subject sat relaxed for a total of five minutes before the baseline measurements were collected. These measurements were the basis for analyzing physiological change as a result of our experiment. Results There were 30 participants involved in the study, including 19 females and 11 males ranging from ages 20 to 22, as seen in Figures 2 and 3. 60% of the participants reported having seen the video prior to our experiment, as seen in Figure 4. Our post-study survey revealed that between being told about the video and being shown the video, the most common mental state of the experimental group was anxious/nervous and the most common mental state of the control group was calm/relaxed (Figure 5). Our post-study survey also revealed that the video invoked a range of fear levels in the participants and invoked a higher level of fear in the experimental group than the control group (Figure 6). The results of the t-tests are summarized in Table 1. Heart Rate

As shown in Figure 7, the experimental group experienced a 3.697% increase in heart rate on average between their baseline and being told about the video, while the control group had a 2.625% decrease in heart rate on average. The results of the t-test concluded that this difference was statistically significant (t value = 2.2923). A comparison of the average heart rate at each time point for the experimental and control groups can be seen in Figure 8. There was an increase in average heart rate for the experimental group after being told it was a scary video, and then a decrease in average heart rate after finishing the video. In the control group, there was a decrease in average heart rate after being told about the video and a slight increase in average heart rate after completing the video. Blood Pressure As shown in Figure 9, the average percent change for the systolic reading from baseline to being told about the video was a 2.799% increase for the experimental group and a 1.092% increase for the control group. The results of the unpaired t-test for the systolic reading were not statistically significant (t value = 0.8411). Figure 10 shows the average systolic blood pressure at all three time points for the experimental and control groups. There was a larger increase in average systolic blood pressure between baseline and being told about the video for the experimental group, but there is also a slight increase for the control group as well. The experimental group exhibited a decrease in average systolic blood pressure after the scare, but the control group continued to show an increase. As seen in Figure 11, the average percent change for the diastolic reading from baseline to being told about the video was a 2.518% increase for the experimental group and a 2.748% decrease for the control group. Just like the systolic measurements, the unpaired t-test showed the diastolic results were not statistically significant (t value = 1.3115). Figure 12 shows the average diastolic blood pressure at each of the three time points for the experimental and

control groups. There is an increase in average diastolic blood pressure between baseline and being told about the video for the experimental group, and a decrease in average diastolic blood pressure for the control group. The average diastolic blood pressure then goes down after the scare in the experimental group and increases in the control group after the scare. Electrodermal Activity (EDA) Figure 13 contains the average percent change calculations for the experimental and control groups between the baseline measurement at time point one and right after the subjects were informed of the video at time point two. For the experimental group, there was a 36.206% increase in EDA between time points one and two. The control group had a 18.109% increase in EDA. The unpaired t-test showed that these results were not statistically significant (t value = 0.6674). The average EDA reading for each group at various time points can be seen in Figure 14. The trends are fairly similar in both the experimental group and control group. There is a slightly larger increase in average EDA between baseline and being told about the video for the experimental group. Both groups continue to increase in average EDA during the scare, and then decrease in average EDA after the scare. Discussion The experimental group showed increases in all three physiological measurements after being informed of the scary video. Only the increase in heart rate was found to be statistically significant. Nevertheless, blood pressure and EDA still both showed increasing trends, suggesting that they may be physiologically significant and may have been statistically significant if more subjects were tested. The control group, on the other hand, exhibited either reduced increases in systolic blood pressure and EDA, as well as decreases in heart rate and diastolic blood pressure after being informed about the video. These results suggest that the anticipation involved with the experimental group was sufficient to cause changes in the body,

resulting in increases in each of these physiological factors. When we finished conducting the study with each subject they were asked not to share with others what our study was about, as it could skew our results. Sampling from only people in our lab left a strong possibility that participants informed other potential participants about the nature of the experiment or what the video contained after leaving the room the experiment was in. Any knowledge regarding the nature of the experiment could have mitigated the effects of anticipation on the physiological measurements. Data from the third time point occurring immediately after the subject watched the video was not heavily analyzed as part of this experiment for several reasons. Our study focused on the impact of anticipation on physiological variables in response to being told about the scary video, instead of physiological changes in response to the video itself. Post-study surveys showed that 60% of the participants had seen that specific video before. Due to the fact that a majority of the subjects had seen the video, results from the third time point are likely to be unreliable. A future study should be sure to choose a less well known video or create their own jumpscare video to generate results that are not influenced by previous experiences. This left results vulnerable to other confounding variables, such as the subject returning to their initial measurement faster than participants who had not seen it before. Furthermore, the reaction of each subject to the video was very subjective and determined by factors that could not be controlled for in this experiment. Subjects who are particularly sensitive to startling may have had a sharper increase in their vitals than a subject who was not startled. A study involving 2 groups of people, 1 that claimed to be afraid of snakes and the other of public speaking, both showed significant physiological changes in response to being in a room with snakes or speaking in public. However, neither group showed significant physiological reactions when presented with fear imagery (Lang et al. 1983). This could partially

explain why our experiment did not achieve the statistically significant physiological changes that we expected: the participants anticipation of fear involved in seeing a scary video might not have induced dramatic physiological changes. This also suggests that if the participants were told that they would experience a fear-inducing stimulus in real life instead of through a video, they would be more likely to exhibit significant physiological responses in heart rate, EDA, and blood pressure. The inability to collect constant data for blood pressure and heart rate may have impacted our overall results. We collected data at three time points for each measurement, which may have prevented us from collecting data that showed the maximum percent change due to anticipation. In order to compensate for this, data was collected at the same time point in the experiment for each subject. EDA data was collected constantly throughout the experiment but analyzed at the same time points as heart rate and blood pressure. Despite these caveats, the increase in heart rate compared to the control group was found to be statistically significant, however our results on EDA and blood pressure might have been significant if we were able to collect continuous data. In this experiment we hypothesized that the effects of anticipation would lead to a statistically significant increase in heart rate, blood pressure, and EDA when compared to a group who did not experience anticipation. Analysis of the data revealed that only the increase in heart rate was a statistically significant change. The lack of statistically significant data for blood pressure and EDA may have been due to a combination of the experimental design, population used, and the sample size. In a future study, we would sample from a larger population and use more subjects to obtain consistent results that could potentially produce statistically significant data for all three measurements. Collecting physiological data throughout the duration of the experiment rather than at specific time points may provide more conclusive

data on the effects of anticipation. An additional study involving anticipation of actually being in a scary event rather than watching a video may yield significant physiological changes and reveal that feedforward may induce these changes due to fear. Nevertheless, further research is needed to develop a more comprehensive understanding of feed forward mechanisms, the impact of anticipation on homeostasis, and the relationship between emotion and physiology.

Appendix Figure 1. This figure displays the process of this study and what a participant should expect to experience. Figure 2 Figure 3 Figures 2-3. These figures display the demographic results of a pre-study survey completed by participants.

Figure 4. Participants completed a post-study survey where they were asked if they had seen the video prior to this study. 60% had seen the video before. Figure 5. Participants were asked to complete a post-study survey where they were asked to self-assess their mental state between being told about the video and being shown the video. They were given five emotions to choose from. This revealed that the most common category selected in the experimental group was anxious/nervous and the most common category selected in the control group was calm/relaxed.

Figure 6. Participants completed a post-study survey that asked them to rate their level of fear elicited by the video on a scale of 1-10. No participants rated their fear as at a level of 10. This revealed that the video invoked a range of fear levels in the participants and invoked a higher level of fear in the experimental group than the control group. Critical Value t Value Statistical Significance Heart Rate 2.048 2.2923 Significant Systolic Blood Pressure Diastolic Blood Pressure 2.048 0.8411 Not Significant 2.048 1.3115 Not Significant EDA 2.048 0.6674 Not Significant Table 1. Percent change was calculated for each participant from their baseline to the point when they were either told they would be watching a video that was meant to scare them (experimental) or a video (control). Unpaired t-tests with a 95% confidence interval were then performed for heart rate, systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and EDA. This table displays the critical value, t value, and whether or not the means of the experimental and control groups were significantly different from each other. The results showed that the only category with a significant difference was heart rate.

Figure 7. This graph displays the average percent change in heart rate from the baseline measurement to the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them (experimental) or a video (control). The unpaired t-test on this data set resulted in a t value of 2.2923 which was greater than the critical value of 2.048, meaning the means of the experimental and control groups were significantly different from each other. Figure 8. This graph shows the average heart rate differences between the experimental and control groups at baseline, the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them or a video, and after the jump scare video. There is an increase in average heart rate for the experimental group after being told it was a scary video, and then a decrease in average heart rate after the scare. In the control group, there was a decrease in average heart rate after being told about the video and a slight increase in average heart rate after the scare.

Figure 9. This graph displays the average percent change in systolic blood pressure from the baseline measurement to the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them (experimental) or a video (control). The unpaired t-test on this data set resulted in a t value of 0.8411 which was less than the critical value of 2.048, meaning the means of the experimental and control groups were not significantly different from each other. Figure 10. This graph shows the average systolic blood pressure differences between the experimental and control groups at baseline, the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them or a video, and after the jump scare video. There is a larger increase in average systolic blood pressure between baseline and being told about the video for the experimental group, but there is also a slight increase for the control

group as well. The experimental group shows a decrease in average systolic blood pressure after the scare, but the control group continues to show an increase. Figure 11. This graph displays the average percent change in diastolic blood pressure from the baseline measurement to the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them (experimental) or a video (control). The unpaired t-test on this data set resulted in a t value of 1.3115 which was less than the critical value of 2.048, meaning the means of the experimental and control groups were not significantly different from each other. Figure 12. This graph shows the average diastolic blood pressure differences between the experimental and control groups at baseline, the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them or a video, and after the jump scare video.

There is an increase in average diastolic blood pressure between baseline and being told about the video for the experimental group, and a decrease in average diastolic blood pressure for the control group. The average diastolic blood pressure then goes down after the scare in the experimental group and increases in the control group after the scare. Figure 13. This graph displays the average percent change in EDA from the baseline measurement to the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them (experimental) or a video (control). The unpaired t-test on this data set resulted in a t value of 0.6674 which was less than the critical value of 2.048, meaning the means of the experimental and control groups were not significantly different from each other. Figure 14. This graph shows the average EDA differences between the experimental and control groups at baseline, the point when participants were told they d either be watching a video that was meant to scare them or a video, and after the jump scare video. The trends are

fairly similar in both the experimental group and control group. There is a slightly larger increase in average EDA between baseline and being told about the video for the experimental group. Both groups continue to increase in average EDA during the scare, and then decrease in average EDA after the scare.

References Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R., & Rowe Biopac, M. (2015). A Guide for Analysing Electrodermal Activity (EDA) & Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs) for Psychological Experiments {via the Biopac MP36R & AcqKnowledge software}. Psychophysiology Boucsein The Neuroscientist Psychophysiology Psychophysiology, 49 (8), 1017 1034. Diehl, C., Centanni, R., Rehfeldt, E., Blada, S., & Heath, J. (2017). The Effect of Audiovisual vs. Visual Startle Stimuli on Eliciting the Fear Response. Jass. Funkenstein, D. H. (1955). The Physiology of Fear and Anger. In C. F. Reed (Ed.), Psychopathology: A Source Book (pp. 223-233). Harvard University Press. Lang, P. J., Levin, D. N., Miller, G. A., & Kozak, M. J. (1983). Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: The problem of affective response integration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92(3), 276-306. Mossbridge, J., Tressoldi, P., & Utts, J. (2012). Predictive physiological anticipation preceding seemingly unpredictable stimuli: a meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 390. Quevedo, K., Smith, T., Donzella, B., Schunk, E., & Gunnar, M. (2010). The startle response: developmental effects and a paradigm for children and adults. Developmental Psychobiology, 52 (1), 78 89. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20415 Steimer, T. (2002). The biology of fear- and anxiety-related behaviors. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 4 (3), 231 49. Widmaier, E. P., Raff, H., Strang, K. T. (2016). Vander s human physiology: The mechanisms of body function. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education.