Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 1. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, in press

Similar documents
Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population

The role of Regulatory Focus in How Much We Care about Enemies: Cross-Cultural Comparison Between European Canadians and Hong Kong Chinese

International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation

Title. Author(s)Schug, Joanna; Yuki, Masaki; Maddux, William W. CitationPsychological Science, 21(10): Issue Date Doc URL.

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module

Basic Needs and Well-Being: A Self-Determination Theory View

A Comparison of Perceptions on the Investment Theory of Creativity between Chinese and American

The Rod and Fran Test : Relationship Priming Influences Cognitive-Perceptual Performance

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS (IR)

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes

Is the power of weak ties universal? A crosscultural comparison of social interaction in Argentina and Canada

SEEKING HELP FROM CLOSE, SAME-SEX FRIENDS 2. Seeking Help From Close, Same-Sex Friends:

Relational mobility: A socioecological approach to p. YukiSchug_IARRchapter Corrected_Sept12_2011 (for dis. Instructions for use

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

Value From Regulatory Fit E. Tory Higgins

SOC-SOCIOLOGY (SOC) SOC-SOCIOLOGY (SOC) 1

Keywords culture, social support seeking, self-esteem, relational concerns, influence and adjustment

PERSON PERCEPTION AND INTERPERSONAL ATTRACTION

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Motivational Affordances: Fundamental Reasons for ICT Design and Use

Culturally Construed Dual Sources of Well-being: Preliminary Psychometric Evidences

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

Understanding Emotions. How does this man feel in each of these photos?

Supplementary material. Cross-Cultural Differences and Similarities in Proneness to Shame: An Adaptationist and Ecological Approach

Running Head: SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 1

Understanding Social Norms, Enjoyment, and the Moderating Effect of Gender on E-Commerce Adoption

PYSC 224 Introduction to Experimental Psychology

SELF and CULTURE What type of change?

Poster Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. Toronto, August Running Head: CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE

The Role of the Self in Responses to Health Communications: A Cultural Perspective. David K. Sherman. University of California, Santa Barbara

ONE JUSTICE FITS ALL? EXAMINING CROSS-CULTURAL DIFFERENCES IN PERCEPTIONS OF JUSTICE

EDITORIAL: CURRENT AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS OF SOCIO-ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY

The role of the self in responses to health communications: A cultural perspective

2 Theoretical Frameworks

Changing the Graduate School Experience: Impacts on the Role Identity of Women

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

Culture & Psychology Mini Conference University of Kent 16 September 2016 KLT4, Keynes College Talks will be 20 min + 10 min for Q&A

ACTIVITY: Exploring Cultural Identity

Culture and Social Behavior, Self and Identity. Chapter 13. Defining the Concept of Self CULTURE AND THE CONCEPT OF SELF

Guide to the Focus in Mind, Brain, Behavior For History and Science Concentrators Science and Society Track Honors Eligible

ABSOLUTE AND RELATIVE JUDGMENTS IN RELATION TO STRENGTH OF BELIEF IN GOOD LUCK

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

PERSON PERCEPTION September 25th, 2009 : Lecture 5

Principles of Sociology

Definitions of Indicators

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 1. THE HUMANISTIC FRAMEWORK FOR INTERVIEWER SKILLS

Values-Based Leadership Why does it matter?

What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology?


A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan

The Cultural Congruency Effect: Culture, Regulatory Focus, and the Effectiveness of Gain- vs. Loss-Framed Health. Messages. Ayse K.

Applied Social Psychology Msc.

CALL FOR PAPERS: THE 8th MIDTERM CONFERENCE ON EMOTIONS, EDINBURGH, 2018

Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs

Counselling Psychology Qualifications Board. Qualification in Counselling Psychology

Analogical Representations. Symbolic Representations. Culture as Cognition. Abstract mental representations. Includes: 9/15/2012

Child Mental Health: A Review of the Scientific Discourse

Character Education Framework

From Codes to Conclusions: Strategies for Analyzing Qualitative Data

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological & social needs. Reading: Reeve (2009) Ch 6

Resilience in the RTW Context

CULTURE DIFFERENCES IN FEEDBACK EXCHANGE: SHOULD I SEND A OR? Xiaxin Zhong

Social Penetration Theory

Nonviolent Communication

Effects of Self-Construal Differences on Cognitive Dissonance Examined by Priming the Independent and Interdependent Self

Chapter 13. Social Psychology

Emotion regulation and well-being among Puerto Ricans and European Americans

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Goal of Cross-Cultural Research

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

Foundation Competencies CHILD WELFARE EPAS Core

Richard Nisbett is a Distinguished University Professor at the University of

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

The Experiences of Ultra-Orthodox Families. Rraising a Child with Autism

THE CHARACTERISTICS OF THE GRADUATE AT GRADUATION

Is Leisure Theory Needed For Leisure Studies?

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

Running Head: COGNITIVE VULNERABILITY AND ATTACHMENT. Cognitive Vulnerability and Attachment. Nathan L. Williams University of Arkansas

School of Social Work

Book Reviews. in developmental psychology, education, social work, and social policy.

VPS PRACTICUM STUDENT COMPETENCIES: SUPERVISOR EVALUATION VPS PRACTICUM STUDENT CLINICAL COMPETENCIES

Relational tendencies associated with broad personality dimensions

The Association between Free Will Beliefs and Stereotypes: People's Belief in Fatalism Promotes Gender Stereotypes

Department of Psychology

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

Hindsight Bias: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. STEVEN J. HEINE and DARRIN R. LEHMAN University of British Columbia, Canada

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

School Philosophy. Introduction

Interpersonal Communication in a Changing World: Culture and Social Networking 28

SHORT NOTE Which studies test whether self-enhancement is pancultural? Reply to Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea, 2007

Author's personal copy

Cultural Models and Implicit Theories of Self and World: A View from (Cross-)Cultural Psychology!

In search of a new paradigm for cultural psychology

CULTURAL CONSTRUCTIONS OF HAPPINESS: THEORY AND EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

Culture, Self-construal and Social Cognition: Evidence from Cross-Cultural and Priming Studies

Department of Psychological Sciences Learning Goals and Outcomes

Transcription:

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 1 Asian Journal of Social Psychology, in press Please visit the journal website for the updated version: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ajsp.12091/abstract Beware of Friends: The Cultural Psychology of Relational Mobility and Cautious Intimacy Liman Man Wai Li, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Glenn Adams, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas, USA Tuğçe Kurtiş, University of West Georgia, Carrollton, Georgia, USA Takeshi Hamamura, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China Keywords: Culture; Relational mobility; Interpersonal relationships CORRESPONDING AUTHOR: Glenn Adams University of Kansas Mailing address: Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66049 email address: adamsg@ku.edu

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 2 Abstract Previous research has contrasted patterns of cautious or prevention-oriented relationality in various West African settings with patterns of growth or promotion-oriented relationality in many North American settings. The present research draws upon the concept of relational mobility to test the hypothesis that different patterns of relationality have their source in respective affordances for embedded interdependence or abstracted independence. Study 1 investigated the relationship between cautious intimacy and perception of relational mobility among a sample of Hong Kong students. Study 2 compared students in Hong Kong and North American settings to test whether differences in perception of relational mobility mediated hypothesized differences in caution about friends. Study 3 used an experimental manipulation among a sample of Hong Kong students to test the hypothesis that increased perception of relational mobility reduces caution about friends. Results reveal broad support for hypotheses. Whether as a measured variable or as an experimental treatment, the perception of relational mobility was negatively related to caution about friends. Moreover, this relationship mediated hypothesized cross-national differences in caution about friendship. Discussion of results considers intersections of cultural and ecological approaches to psychology and implications for theoretical conceptions of interdependence. Keywords: Culture; Relational mobility; Interpersonal relationships

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 3 Beware of Friends: The Cultural Psychology of Relational Mobility and Cautious Intimacy The idea of cautious intimacy that one should beware of friends and others in intimate spaces is a common theme in social discourse and everyday practice in the various West African settings where Adams (2005) conducted research (Adams & Plaut, 2003). These features of everyday life suggest a normative construction of social relations that one might refer to as prevention-oriented relationality characterized by avoidance of such painful outcomes as rejection or conflict (see Gable, 2006; Gable & Impett, 2012; see also Hashimoto & Yamagishi, 2013; Higgins, 1987; 1996). Within these West African settings, this orientation finds expression in tendencies for people to report a relatively small number of friends (Adams & Plaut, 2003); to be vigilant for attacks from personal enemies in the context of close relationships (Adams, 2005); to emphasize silence and concealment rather than expressive disclosure and revelation in intimate spaces (e.g., Ferme, 2001; Shaw, 2000; see also Taylor et al., 2004); and to focus on careful management of obligations for material care (Coe, 2011) or shared participation in everyday concerns as the basis of social support (Adams & Plaut, 2003; see Mesquita, 2001). The implicit contrast is the more promotion-oriented relationality that constitutes a normative construction of social relations in North American settings. This orientation valorizes the relatively uninhibited pursuit of such pleasurable outcomes as intimacy or growth (see Gable, 2006; Gable & Impett, 2012; see also Higgins, 1987; 1996). It finds expression in tendencies for people to report a large number of friends (Adams & Plaut, 2003) and social interactions (Wheeler, Reis, & Bond, 1989); to report a sense of freedom from enemies (Adams, 2005) and high levels of general trust (Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994); to emphasize self-disclosure as a tool for production of intimacy (e.g., Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992); and to regard verbally

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 4 oriented, emotional understanding as the essence of social support (Kim, Sherman, & Taylor, 2008; Mesquita, 2001). In this way, comparative research in West African and North American settings has highlighted broad differences in goal orientations that cohere across a wide range of relationship phenomena. We use prevention-oriented relationality to refer to a constellation of tendencies that emphasize fulfillment of obligation and avoidance of disruption as strategies to maintain security within existing connections. These prevention-oriented tendencies include an emphasis on material support, vigilant concern about preventing harm from possible enemies, and caution about friendship. Conversely, we use promotion-oriented relationality to refer to a constellation of tendencies that emphasize emotional intimacy and pursuit of expansive happiness as strategies to enhance personal growth by creating more satisfying connections. These promotion-oriented tendencies include an emphasis on emotional support, a sense of freedom from concern about enemies, and an open approach to friendship.. Theoretical Framework: The Cultural Psychology of Relationship In their original formulation, Adams and his colleagues located the source of these different patterns in implicit constructions of relationality by which people in different settings practice and experience interpersonal connection (Adams, Anderson, & Adonu, 2004). In particular, responses of participants in West African settings linked a cautious approach to friendship, concern about enemies, and other prevention-oriented patterns to cultural realities of embedded interdependence (Adams, 2005). These everyday realities afford both a sense of rootedness in context and an experience of relationality as ontologically prior connection in dense or overlapping networks constituted by mutual obligations of material support (Adams, 2005; Adams & Plaut, 2003; Salter & Adams, 2012). These everyday realities afford "thick" or "sticky" forms of relationality that

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 5 limit people's capacity to create connections or to insulate themselves from the interpersonal friction. Within these cultural realities of embedded interdependence, the prevention-oriented emphasis on careful management of existing ties is an optimal strategy to ensure everyday wellbeing (Hamamura & Heine, 2008; Hashimoto & Yamagishi, 2013; Oishi & Kesebir, 2012). In contrast, responses of participants in North American settings linked an open approach to friendship, minimal concern about enemies, and other promotion-oriented forms of relationality to cultural realities of voluntaristic or abstracted independence (Adams, 2005; Adams & Plaut, 2003; Anderson, Adams, & Plaut, 2008). These everyday realities afford both a sense of separation from context and an experience of social connection as the tenuous creation of ontologically prior individuals (Adams, Anderson, & Adonu, 2004). Resonating with what Fiske (1991) called a market pricing (MP) model of relationship, these realities promote an experience of the social world as a relatively frictionless "free market" populated by unfettered "free agents" who are at liberty (and compelled) to arrange their own connections. They afford the sense of possibility not only to construct a broad network of friends, but also to avoid negative consequences of connection (including personal enemies). Within these cultural realities of abstracted independence, the promotion-oriented emphasis on expansion and open pursuit of pleasurable ties is an optimal strategy to enhance well-being (Hamamura & Heine, 2008; Oishi & Kesebir, 2012). Ecological Intersections: Relational Mobility In their original formulation, Adams and his colleagues (2004) referred to implicit constructions of relationship in a manner consistent with an ecological (versus entity) conception of culture as explicit and implicit patterns of historically derived and selected ideas and the material manifestations [of these ideas] in institutions, practices, and artifacts (Adams

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 6 & Markus, 2004; p. 11). In other words, they used implicit constructions to refer to both person-inscribed patterns of mind-in-brain associated with different understandings of relationship (Markus & Kitayama, 1991) and ecologically inscribed patterns of mind in society (Vygotsky, 1978) that afford and constrain different experiences of relationship (Barker, 1968; Cole, 1995). This ecological conception of culture provides a point of contact with the renewed interest in ecological perspectives across the field of social psychology. Although a broad review is beyond the scope of the present work (see Oishi & Graham, 2010, for a review), a recurring theme of this work has been the impact of mobility on psychological experience (e.g., Chen, Chiu, & Chan, 2009; Oishi, 2010; Yamagishi, Hashimoto, Li, & Schug, 2012; Yuki & Schug, 2012). Particularly relevant for the present work is the idea of relational mobility: the extent to which local realities afford opportunities to meet new people and choose new relationships (Yuki & Schug, 2012). This concept provides an explicit link between the experience of mobility and different constructions of relationship. Almost by definition, everyday settings that promote high relational mobility afford a voluntaristic-independent (Adams & Plaut, 2003) sense of freedom to pursue and choose satisfying connections, to disinvest in relationships that no longer satisfy, and other patterns of promotion-oriented relationality. Conversely, everyday settings that promote low relational mobility afford an embedded-interdependent sense of caution, avoidance of conflict, and other patterns of prevention-oriented relationality (also see Hashimoto & Yamagishi, 2013; Yamagishi, Hashimoto, Li, & Schug, 2012). Initial evidence for these ideas comes from research comparing relational mobility and tendencies of self-disclosure among students in Japan and the U.S. (Schug, Yuki, & Maddux, 2010). Consistent with research on sociocultural differences at the level of national society, the

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 7 promotion-oriented pattern of greater willingness to self-disclose was more evident among U.S. than Japanese students. Consistent with research on sociocultural differences at the level of local ecology, the promotion-oriented pattern of greater willingness to self-disclose was positively associated with students perceptions about relational mobility in their local environment. Moreover, perceptions of relational mobility were greater among U.S. than Japanese students, and this difference in perceptions of relational mobility mediated corresponding differences in promotion-oriented tendencies of open self-disclosure. Overview of the Current Research The present research extends previous work on cultural-psychological foundations of relationship (e.g., Adams et al., 2004) in two important ways that make synergistic contact with research on relational mobility (e.g., Yuki & Schug, 2012). First, the present research extends the comparison of cautious intimacy in West African and North American settings to an East Asian setting (Hong Kong) which researchers have associated with varieties of (embedded) interdependence (e.g., Shen, Wan, & Wyer, 2011; Zou et al., 2009). Second, the present work extends previous research on cultural-psychological foundations of relationality by investigating the links between cautious intimacy and perceptions of relational mobility. The resonance between discussions of relationship constructions (e.g., Adams et al., 2004) and relational mobility (e.g., Yuki & Schug, 2012) suggests the hypothesis that promotion-oriented patterns of relationality that is, an open approach to friendship, sense of freedom from concern about enemyship, and tendency to emphasize emotional over material aspects of support (Adams, 2005; Adams & Plaut, 2003) will be positively associated with relational mobility. We tested this hypothesis with multiple methods across three studies. Study 1

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 8 Study 1 tested the hypothesized relationship between promotion-oriented patterns of relationality and perceptions of relational mobility. In all three studies, we presented all measures in English, which is a language of instruction at all research sites. Method Participants. We recruited 93 students (40 men, 53 women; M age = 20.72, SD = 2.02), all of whom were born in Hong Kong, from a university in Hong Kong. Procedure. Participants responded to items in an online survey using a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The survey included the 12-item Relational Mobility Scale (RMob; Yuki et al., 2007; e.g., For the most part, people are able to choose those who they interact with ; α =.70). High scores indicate that people perceive greater relational mobility that is, meeting new people, choosing connections, and creating relationships in their local environment. The survey also included measures that we developed to assess distinct, but conceptually related, components of relationality. Our primary outcome was a 7-item measure of caution about friends (e.g., A person who is a friend today might become an enemy tomorrow ; see Appendix A). We computed the mean of these items to create a composite indicator of the cautious approach to friendship associated with prevention-oriented relationality (α =.83). Besides the primary outcome of caution about friends, we also included briefer, more exploratory measures for two other components of relationality. First, we modified interview prompts from earlier work (Adams, 2005) to create two items that tapped concern about enemyship: People who claim they do not have enemies are naïve and I am the target of enemies: that is, there are people who want my downfall and are trying to sabotage me, r(93) =.50, p <.001.The mean of these items provided an indicator of the prominence of concern about

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 9 enemyship associated with prevention-oriented relationality. Second, we modified interview prompts from earlier work (Adams & Plaut, 2003) to create two items about social support in friendship: I regard emotional support [practical or material assistance] as a defining feature of friendship. The difference in responses to these items provided an indicator of the promotionoriented tendency to emphasize emotional over material support in friendship. Results and Discussion Means, standard deviations, and correlations between variables appear in Table 1. Results revealed hypothesized relationships. Perceptions of relational mobility were negatively associated with caution about friendship, r(93) = -.47, p <.001; negatively associated with concern about enemyship, r(93) = -.29, p =.005; and positively associated with an emphasis on emotional intimacy over material assistance, r(93) =.25, p =.016. Besides hypothesized associations, we also found gender differences on all variables such that patterns of promotion-oriented relationality were stronger among women than men. Specifically, women (M = 4.12, SD =.53) scored higher than men (M = 3.86, SD =.42) on the 2 RMob scale, F(1, 91) = 6.56, p =.01, η p =.07; and women (M = 1.23, SD = 1.22) showed a greater tendency than men (M = 0.63, SD =.90) to emphasize emotional rather than material aspects of social support, F(1, 91) = 6.91, p =.01, η 2 p =.07. Conversely, men (M = 3.64, SD = 1.23) expressed stronger concern about enemyship than women did (M = 3.07, SD = 1.14), F(1, 91) = 5.33, p =.02, η 2 p =.06); and men (M = 3.36, SD =.79) tended to express greater caution about friendship than women did (M = 3.05, SD =.90), F(1, 91) = 3.14, p =.08, η 2 p =.03. Gender did not moderate associations between RMob scores and dependent variables. For this reason, and because we did not observe these effects consistently across Studies 2 and 3, we do not

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 10 attempt to interpret them here. However, the pattern does have interesting theoretical implications that we consider in the General Discussion section. Previous work has established that RMob scores predict tendencies of self-disclosure (Schug et al., 2010) and the relationship of similarity and attraction in friendship (Schug, Yuki, Horikawa, & Takemura, 2009), both of which are specific manifestations of general patterns that we refer to as promotion-oriented relationality (see Adams, Kurtiş, Salter, & Anderson, 2012). The present study extends research by demonstrating a link between RMob scores and other patterns of promotion-oriented relationality: an open (i.e., less cautious) approach to friendship, a sense of freedom from concern about enemyship, and an emphasis on emotional rather than material aspects of social support. Study 2 Results of Study 1 confirm an association between perceptions of relational mobility and promotion-oriented patterns of relationality. Study 2 considers whether this association can explain differences across cultural settings (Adams, 2005; Adams & Plaut, 2003). We administered the same instruments as in Study 1 to samples of students from universities in Hong Kong and the U.S. Extending previous comparisons of participants in North American and East Asian contexts (e.g., Schug et al., 2009; 2010), we anticipated that students in the U.S. setting would show greater tendencies toward promotion-oriented relationality than would participants in a Hong Kong setting. The guiding hypothesis of the study is that perception of relational mobility, measured by scores on the RMob scale, will account for observed differences in relationship tendencies. This implies that (a) RMob scores will be significantly correlated with relationship tendencies (as in Study 1), (b) RMob scores will be greater among participants in the

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 11 US setting than the Hong Kong setting, and (c) the difference in RMob scores will mediate differences in relationship tendencies across settings. Method Participants. Participants were 165 students (71 men, 80 women, and 14 who did not report gender; M age = 20.05, SD = 1.40) from a university in Hong Kong and 178 students (86 men, 75 women, and 17 who did not report gender; M age = 19.30, SD = 1.61) from a university in the U.S. Procedure. Participants completed the same online survey as in Study 1, including measures of RMob (Hong Kong: α =.77, US: α =.80), caution about friends (Hong Kong: α =.75, US: α =.74), concern about enemyship experience (Hong Kong: r(165) =.18, p =.02; US: r(170) =.26, p =.001), and emphasis on emotional support. Results and Discussion Means, standard deviations, and correlations between variables appear in Table 2. Results again reveal broad support for hypotheses. Differences across settings. We conducted 2 (Setting) x 2 (Gender) ANOVAs to test for hypothesized differences across cultural settings. Results revealed the hypothesized effect of 2 cultural setting on relational mobility, F(1,308) = 64.84, p <.001, η p =.17, such that RMob scores were higher in the US (M = 4.53, SD =.65) than Hong Kong setting (M = 3.97, SD =.58). A marginally significant effect of gender, F(1,308) = 3.46, p =.06, η 2 p =.01, suggested that Rmob scores were higher among women (M = 4.31, SD =.71) than men (M = 4.21, SD =.64). The interaction was not significant, F< 1, p =.81. Results revealed the hypothesized effect of cultural setting on caution about friendship such that caution scores were greater among participants in the Hong Kong (M = 3.13, SD =.76)

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 12 than the U.S. setting (M = 2.37, SD =.76), F(1,308) = 75.78, p <.001, η 2 p =.20. The main effect of gender and its interaction with cultural setting were not significant, Fs < 1, ps >.47. Results revealed the hypothesized effect of cultural setting on concern about enemyship, such that concern about enemyship was higher among participants in the Hong Kong (M = 3.42, SD = 1.02) than the U.S. setting (M = 2.87, SD =.97), F(1,308) = 25.56, p <.001, η 2 p =.08. Results also revealed a main effect of gender, such that concern about enemyship was higher among men (M = 3.23, SD = 1.02) than women (M = 3.03, SD = 1.04), F(1,308) = 4.72, p =.03, η 2 p =.02. The Setting x Gender interaction did not reach conventional levels of statistical significance, F(1,308) = 3.39, p =.07 1. Finally, results revealed only a main effect of gender such that the emphasis on emotional over material support was greater among women (M = 1.90, SD = 1.77) than men (M = 1.34, SD = 1.75), F(1,308) = 7.79, p <.01, η 2 p =.03. Neither the effect of cultural setting nor its interaction with gender were significant, Fs < 2, ps >.31. Mediation analysis. We next tested the hypothesis that observed differences in perceptions of relational mobility would account for (i.e., mediate) observed differences in relational orientations across settings. In particular, we tested this hypothesis for the relationship tendencies, caution about friends and concern about enemyship, for which we observed differences across settings; we did not observe a difference across settings in valorization of emotional support, so we did not test the mediation hypothesis for this outcome. Mediation analyses with 5,000 bootstrapping resamples using the procedure of Preacher and Hayes (2008) revealed that the indirect effect of cultural setting on relationship tendencies through its effect on RMob scores was significant for both caution about friends and concern about enemyship. A summary of these analyses appears in Figure 1.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 13 Summary. Results of Study 2 extend research on cultural-psychological foundations of relationality in 2 ways. First, we replicated patterns of cultural differences in friendship and concern about enemyship (Adams, 2005; Adams & Plaut, 2003) in a comparison of North American and East Asian (Hong Kong) settings. Second, we observed support for the hypothesis linking differences in relationality across settings to corresponding differences in perception of relational mobility. Study 3 The hypothesis that perceptions of relational mobility mediate cultural differences in relationship tendencies implies the hypothesis that different perceptions of relational mobility can elicit different relationship tendencies. In Study 3, we conducted an experiment to test this hypothesis in more explicit fashion. Method Participants. Participants were 55 students (24 men, 27 women, and 1 who did not report gender; M age = 20.67, SD = 2.07) from a university in Hong Kong. All of participants were born in Hong Kong.. Procedure and Materials. Participants arrived in groups of 5 to 10. The experimenter led each participant to an individual workstation to complete all procedures. Participants read a brief passage about a company that we portrayed as either high or low in relational mobility (see Appendix B; cf. Chen et al., 2009; Oishi, Miao, Koo, Kisling, & Ratliff, 2012; Yuki, Sato, Takemura, & Oishi, 2013). Participants had 10 minutes to imagine working for the company and to write about the relationships they would have with their coworkers. After the writing task, participants completed the same measures of caution about friends (α =.70), concern about enemyship [r(52) =.12, p =.38], and emphasis on emotional support.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 14 Results and Discussion To test the hypothesis, we conducted 2 (Condition: stable vs. mobile) x 2 (Gender: male vs. female) ANOVAs on outcomes of interest. Results for the primary outcome of caution about friendship showed only the hypothesized, main effect of condition, F(1,47) = 5.13, p =.03, η 2 p =.10. Participants in the high mobility condition (M = 2.91, SD =.59) reported less caution about friends than did those in the low mobility condition (M = 3.32, SD =.60). The main effect of gender and its interaction with condition were not significant, Fs < 1, ps >.36. In contrast to results for caution about friends, results revealed no effects for concern about enemyship, Fs< 2, ps >.23. A plausible explanation for this pattern concerns the relative sensitivity of different items to experimental manipulation. Definitive statements about enemyship concern are presumably less open to experimental manipulation than are more fluid judgments about the advisability of caution in intimate relations. If so, then it is not surprising that we observed effects of the relational mobility manipulation for our seven-item measure of fluid beliefs about friendship but not for our two-item measure of concern about enemyship. Although plausible, a definitive test of this speculation awaits future research. Similarly, results revealed no main effects on the measure of social support, Fs < 3, ps >.10. Instead, the Condition x Gender interaction was significant, F(1,47) = 4.24, p =.05, η 2 p =.08. To decompose the interaction, we examined the simple effect of condition within gender. The hypothesized effect of the manipulation was significant among women, F (1,25) = 9.61, p <.01, η 2 p =.28, such that the emphasis on emotional over material support was greater in the high mobility (M = 2.50, SD = 1.41) than the low mobility condition (M =.91, SD = 1.14). The effect of the manipulation was not significant among men, F < 1, p >.67.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 15 A speculative explanation for this interaction pattern may be broader gender differences in responsiveness to situational variation. Theory and research suggest that a defining feature of independent constructions of self is an abstraction of experience from social context and corresponding insensitivity to situational variation relative to more interdependent constructions (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Suh, 2002). If affordances for independence are more prominent among men than women (e.g., Markus & Oyserman, 1989), then the associated insensitivity to situational variation may make men less responsive (than women) to experimental manipulation. Alternatively, the pattern of results may reflect gender differences not in sensitivity to experimental manipulation, but instead in flexibility of the response domain. That is, response repertoires for valorization of emotional support may be more flexible and therefore more responsive to experimental manipulation among women than among men. Again, a definitive test of these possibilities awaits future research. In the meantime, we emphasize that the experimental manipulation of relational mobility did produce hypothesized effects on our primary measure of caution about friendship. Despite the relatively minimalist character of (or participant insensitivities to) the experimental manipulation, participants who imagined a high mobility future expressed less caution about friendship than did participants who imagined a low mobility future. Of course, as with any investigation, Study 3 is not without limitations. First, the sample was small, which raises questions about replicability that are best answered by conducting more research. Second, we did not include a control condition, so it is impossible to judge whether one or both conditions (low and high relational mobility) were the primary force of change in relationship tendencies.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 16 Even so, the study adds to the growing body of literature which demonstrates that experimental manipulations of anticipated mobility can be sufficient to produce differences in psychological outcomes that correlational research has associated with different affordances for mobility (cf. Chen et al., 2009; Oishi, et al., 2012; Yuki et al., 2013) Previous work has applied correlational techniques (including quasi-experimental comparisons across contexts) to document a link between measures of relational mobility and relationship tendencies (e.g. Schug et al., 2009; 2010). Extending previous research, the present study applies experimental techniques to document a causal link between perceptions of relational mobility and relationship tendencies. Specifically, results support the hypothesis that a temporary manipulation of perceptions of relational mobility can be sufficient to produce hypothesized differences in caution about friendship that parallel both the differences across context that we observed in Study 2 and the associations with RMob that we observed in Studies 1 and 2. General Discussion The guiding hypothesis of this research is that the promotion-oriented relationality that constitutes the normative standard of psychological science is not a just-natural baseline, but instead reflects affordances for what Yuki and his colleagues (e.g., Yuki et al., 2007) have called relational mobility. Support for the hypothesis was greatest for our primary outcome of interest: caution about friendship. Whether as an experimental treatment (Study 3) or measured variable (Studies 1 and 2), the perception of relational mobility was negatively related to caution about friends, and this negative relationship mediated hypothesized differences in caution about friendship between Hong Kong and U.S. settings (Study 2). Besides the primary outcome of caution about friendship, support for the hypothesis was also evident for concern about enemyship. Perceptions of relational mobility were negatively

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 17 related to enemyship concern (Studies 1 and 2), and this negative relationship mediated hypothesized differences in enemyship concern between Hong Kong and U.S. settings (Study 2). However, our experimental manipulation of perceptions about relational mobility did not produce parallel effects on enemyship concern (Study 3). As we noted, a speculative explanation for this pattern is that definitive statements about enemyship concern may be less sensitive to experimental manipulation than more fluidly constructed judgments about caution in friendship. Support for the hypothesis was weakest for valorization of emotional (versus material) support. On one hand, valorization of emotional support did not vary across cultural settings in the same manner as caution about friends and concern about enemyship (Study 2). Moreover, our relational mobility treatment produced effects on valorization of emotional support only among women, but not among men (Study 3). On the other hand, we did observe the hypothesized relationship between measured perceptions of relational mobility and valorization of emotional support in Study 1. Moreover, we replicated this pattern within both Hong Kong and US samples in Study 2. In summary, results provide consistent support for the hypothesis that the perception of relational mobility is negatively associated with a cautious approach to friendship. In addition, results provide some support for the hypothesis that the perception of relational mobility is associated with a broader tendency of promotion-oriented relationality that includes a sense of freedom from enemyship and valorization of emotional over material support. Variation in consistency of results across these latter measures (especially in response to experimental manipulation) is a limitation of the present work that provides fertile ground for further work. Without downplaying this limitation, we turn attention to the implications of the present work for theory and research.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 18 Perspectives on Interdependence and Relationality Besides evidence for the hypothesized association of relationship experience with relational mobility, results revealed inconsistent patterns of gender differences. Replicating wellestablished patterns (e.g., Burda, Vaux, & Schill, 1984; Trobst, Collins, & Embree, 1994), we observed in all three studies that valorization of emotional (relative to material) support was greater among women than men, although this gender difference was evident in Study 3 only among participants in the high-mobility condition. We also observed gender differences in relational mobility, such that RMob scores were higher for women than men in both Studies 1 and 2. Finally, men scored higher than women on measures of caution about friends (Study 1, but not Studies 2 and 3) and enemyship concern (Studies 1 and 2, but not Study 3). Although one must exercise caution about interpretation of these results given their inconsistency across studies, the general pattern suggests that women perceived greater relational mobility and indicated stronger tendencies of promotion-oriented relationality than did men. This pattern exposes inconsistencies in theoretical understandings of interdependence and relationality across dimensions of cultural setting and gendered difference. Research on gender differences reveals that women tend to show more promotion-oriented patterns of disclosure and emotional support than men, and it explains this by referring to women s greater interdependence (Cross & Madson, 1997). In contrast, the theoretical framework that underlies the present studies derives from comparisons across cultural settings that associate interdependent selfways with prevention-oriented patterns of caution and material care rather than promotion-oriented patterns of disclosure and emotional care. The question then arises: is interdependence related to prevention-oriented patterns of relationality (as a cultural psychological framework suggests) or promotion-oriented patterns of relationality (as research on gender differences suggests)? As a

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 19 preliminary answer to this question, the present work suggests that interdependence in the form of affordances for relational embeddedness versus relational mobility is associated with prevention-oriented forms of relationality. How does one square this conclusion with research derived from theoretical perspectives on gender that associate women s interdependence with promotion-oriented relationality? A clue comes from discussions in historical sociology that link the valorization of promotionoriented relationality evident in contemporary North American settings to the relatively recent rise of affective individualism: constructions of self characterized not only by abstracted independence and relational mobility, but also a value emphasis on exploration and expression of personal feelings (Bellah et al., 1985; Giddens, 1991; Oliker, 1998). If this account is correct, then women s greater tendencies toward promotion-oriented relationality evident in the present studies and other research may indeed be a manifestation of interdependence, understood specifically in terms of socio-emotional values. However, this increased focus on interdependence in the sense of socio-emotional values may ironically be a consequence of independence in the sense of relational mobility. Although definitive evidence for this account awaits future research, an important contribution of the present studies is to illuminate these theoretical issues for deeper consideration. Affordances for Regulatory Focus. In his foundational articulations, Higgins (1987; 1996) discussed features of early childhood environments that afford different forms of regulatory focus. [C]hildren socialized by bolstering and love-withdrawal modes of caretaker-child interaction are likely to acquire a promotion focus in which survival in the world means attaining accomplishments and fulfilling aspirations. In contrast, children socialized by

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 20 some combination of prudent and punitive/critical modes of caretaker-child interaction are likely to acquire a prevention focus in which survival in the world means attaining safety and meeting obligations (Higgins, 1996; p. 1066). Subsequent research has illuminated variation in regulatory focus, linking prevention orientation with realities of embedded interdependence and promotion-orientation with realities of abstracted independence (Elliot, Chirkov, Kim, & Sheldon, 2001; Hamamura, Meijer, Heine, Kamaya, & Hori, 2009; Lee, Aaker, & Gardner, 2000). Extending this work, we proposed, tested, and observed that the perception of relational mobility (and its flipside, embedded interdependence) fosters promotion-oriented (and prevention-oriented) forms of relationality. This bridge between research areas suggests directions for additional research on affordances for variation in regulatory focus beyond differences in relational orientation. For example, one might examine variation in promotion and prevention motivations as a function of measured or manipulated differences in perceptions of relational mobility (e.g., Hamamura, Meijer, Heine, Kamaya, & Hori, 2009). Conclusion: Bridging Cultural and Ecological Perspectives A similar conceptual bridge is implicit in our intersection of cultural-psychological and social-ecological perspectives. Other work has fruitfully elaborated the distinction between cultural-psychological and social-ecological approaches to call for renewed attention to ecological perspectives on psychological science (e.g., Oishi & Graham, 2010). In the present work, we have taken inspiration from an ecologically oriented understanding of culture to emphasize a point of contact between (a) work that locates the cultural grounding of personal relationship in affordances for embedded interdependence and voluntaristic independence (Adams et al., 2004) and (b) work that associates contextual variation in relationship tendencies with relational mobility (Schug et al.,

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 21 2009, 2010; Yuki & Schug, 2012). Again, the bridge between research areas suggests directions for research that investigates variation in relational mobility and its consequences for variation in psychological tendencies that previous research has examined within a cultural psychology framework. For example, one might examine variation in self-enhancement tendencies (e.g., Falk, Heine, Yuki, & Takemura, 2009) or the association between self-esteem and happiness (Yuki et al., 2013) as a function of measured or manipulated differences in perceptions of relational mobility. We offer this paper as a contribution toward such theoretical integration.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 22 Note 1 A probe of this interaction indicated that the simple effect of gender was significant only among Hong Kong participants, F(1, 149) = 7.64, p <.01, η 2 p =.05, where reports of enemyship concern were stronger among men (M = 3.65, SD =.93) than women (M = 3.21, SD = 1.05). There was no gender difference in reports of enemyship concern among American participants, F<1, p =.81.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 23 References Adams, G. (2005). The cultural grounding of personal relationship: Enemyship in North American and West African worlds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 948-968. Adams, G., Anderson, S. L., & Adonu, J. K. (2004). The cultural grounding of closeness and intimacy. In D. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), The handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 321-339). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Adams G., Kurtiș, T., Salter, P.S., & Anderson, S.L. (2012). A cultural psychology of relationship: Decolonizing science and practice. In O. Gillath, G. Adams, & A.D. Kunkel, (Eds.), Relationship science: Integrating evolutionary, neuroscience, and sociocultural approaches (pp. 49-70). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Adams, G. & Markus, H. R. (2004). Toward a conception of culture suitable for a Social Psychology of Culture. In M. Schaller & C. S. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 335-360). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Adams, G., & Plaut, V. C. (2003). The cultural grounding of personal relationship: Friendship in North American and West African worlds. Personal Relationships, 10, 333 347. Anderson, S. L., Adams, G., & Plaut, V. C. (2008). The cultural grounding of personal relationship: The importance of attractiveness in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 352-368. Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596-612 Bellah, R., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W., Swindler, A., & Tipton, S. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life. New York: Harper & Row

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 24 Burda, P. C., Vaux, A., & Schill, T. (1984). Social support resources: Variation across sex and sex role. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 119-126. Chen, J., Chiu, C-y, & Chan, S. F. (2009). The cultural effects of job mobility and the belief in a fixed world: Evidence from performance forecast. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 851-865. Coe, C. (2011). What is love? The materiality of care in Ghanaian transnational families. International Migration. Special Issue: Affecting Global Movement: The Emotional Terrain of Transnationality,49, 7-24. Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self: Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37. Elliot, A. J., Chirkov, V. I., Kim, Y., & Sheldon, K. M. (2001). A cross-cultural analysis of avoidance (relative to approach) personal goals. Psychological Science, 12, 505-510. Ferme, M. C. (2001). The underneath of things: Violence, history, and the everyday in Sierra Leone. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., Yuki, M., & Takemura, K. (2009). Why do Westerners self-enhance more than East Asians? European Journal of Personality, 23, 183-203. Fiske, A.P. (1991). Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of social relations: communal sharing, authority ranking, equality matching and market pricing. New York: Free Press. Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and goals. Journal of Personality, 71, 175 222. Gable, S. L., & Impett, E. A. (2012). Approach and avoidance motives and close relationships. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6, 95-108.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 25 Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity: Self and society in the late modern age. Cambridge, U.K.: Polity. Hamamura, T., & Heine, S. J. (2008). Approach and avoidance motivation across cultures. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 557-570). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Hamamura, T., Meijer, Z., Heine, S. J., Kamaya, K., & Hori, I. (2009). Approach-avoidance motivation and information processing: cross-cultural analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 454-462. Hashimoto, H., & Yamagishi, T. (2013). Two faces of interdependence: Harmony seeking and rejection avoidance. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 142-151. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300. Higgins, E. T. (1996). The self digest : Self-knowledge serving self-regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1062-1083. Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Culture and social support. American Psychologist, 63, 518 526. Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures and pains of distinct selfconstruals: The role of interdependence in regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1122-1134. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224 253. Markus, H.R., & Oyserman, D. (1989). Gender and thought: The role of the self-concept. In M. Crawford & M. Hamilton (Eds.), Gender and thought (pp. 100-127). New York: Springer- Verlag.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 26 Mesquita, B. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 68 74. Oishi, S. (2010). The psychology of residential mobility: Implications for self, social relationships, and well-being. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 5-21. Oishi, S., & Graham, J. (2010). Social ecology: Lost and found in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 356-377. Oishi & S., & Kesebir, S. (2012). Optimal social-networking strategy is a function of socioeconomic conditions. Psychological Science, 23, 1542 1548. Oishi, S., Miao, F. F., Koo, M., Kisling, J., & Ratliff, K. A. (2012). Residential mobility breeds familiarity-seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 149-162. Oliker, S. J. (1998). The modernization of friendship: Individualism, intimacy, and gender in the nineteenth century. In R. G. Adams & G. Allan (Eds.), Placing friendship in context: Structural analysis in the social sciences (pp. 18-42). New York: Cambridge University Press. Preacher, K., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879-891. Salter, P.S., & Adams, G. (2012). Mother or wife? An African dilemma tale and the psychological dynamics of sociocultural change. Social Psychology, 42(4), 232-242. Schug, J., Yuki, M., & Maddux, W. (2010). Relational mobility explains between- and withinculture differences in self-disclosure to close friends. Psychological Science, 21, 1471 1478.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 27 Schug, J., Yuki, M., Horikawa, H., & Takemura, K. (2009). Similarity attraction and actually selecting similar others: How cross-societal differences in relational mobility affect interpersonal similarity in Japan and the USA. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 95 103. Shaw, R. (2000). "Tok af, lef af': A political economy of Temne techniques of secrecy and self. In I. Karp & D. A. Masolo (Eds.), African philosophy as cultural inquiry (pp. 25-49). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Shen, H., Wan, F., & Wyer, R. S. (2011). Cross-cultural differences in the refusal to accept a small gift: The differential influence of reciprocity norms on Asians and North Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 271-281. Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1378-1391. Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Kim, H. S., Jarcho, J., Takagi, K., & Dunagan, M. S. (2004). Culture and social support: Who seeks it and why? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 354 362. Trobst, K. K., Collins, R. L., & Embree, J. J. (1994). The role of emotion in social support provision: Gender, empathy, and expressions of distress. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 45-62. Wheeler, L., Reis, H. T., & Bond, M. H. (1989). Collectivism and individualism in everyday life: The middle kingdom and the melting pot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 79 86. Yamagishi, T., Hashimoto, H., Li, Y., & Schug, J. (2012). Stadtluft Macht Frei (City air brings freedom). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43, 38-45.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 28 Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129-166. Yuki, M., Schug, J., Horikawa, H., Takemura, K., Sato, K., Yokota, K., & Kamaya, K. (2007). Development of a scale to measure perceptions of relational mobility in society. CERSS Working Paper 75, Center for Experimental Research in Social Sciences, Hokkaido University. Yuki, M., & Schug, J. (2012). Relational mobility: A socio-ecological approach to personal relationships. In O. Gillath, G.E. Adams, & A.D. Kunkel (Eds.), Relationship science: Integrating evolutionary, neuroscience, and sociocultural approaches (pp. 137-152). Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association. Yuki, M., Sato, K., Takemura, K., & Oishi, S. (2013). Social ecology moderates the association between self-esteem and happiness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 741-746. Zou, X., Tam, K.P., Morris, M.W., Lee, S.-L., Lau, I.Y.-M., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus versus personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 579 597.

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 29 Table 1 Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations in Study 1. M SD 1 2 3 4 1. Relation mobility 4.00.50 - -.47*** -.29**.25* 2. Caution about friendship 3.18.87 - -.69*** -.29** 3. Enemyship experience 3.31 1.21 - - - -.29** 4. Emphasis on Emotional Support.97 1.13 - - - - * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001

Relational Mobility and Caution about Friends 30 Table 2 Means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations in Study 2. Hong Kong US M SD M SD 1 2 3 4 1. Relational mobility 3.97 a.58 4.53 b.65 - -.42*** -.11.38*** 2. Caution about friendship 3.13 a.76 2.37 b.76 -.33*** -.53*** -.40*** 3. Enemyship experience 3.42 a 1.02 2.87 b.97 -.22**.53*** - -.20** 4. Emphasis on Emotional Support 1.60 a 1.78 1.65 a 1.79.33*** -.28*** -.26** - Note. Different subscripts within each row indicate a significant cultural difference at p <.05. Correlations above and below the diagonal are for US and Hong Kong Chinese participants, respectively. * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001