Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats

Similar documents
Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

Fronto-executive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates

Chapter 4. Role of the nucleus accumbens core and shell in Pavlovian instrumental transfer

Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Disrupt Selective Aspects of Reinforcer Representation in Rats

The Biology of Addiction

The orbitofrontal cortex, predicted value, and choice

The Contribution of the Amygdala to Reward-Related Learning and Extinction

Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion

ISIS NeuroSTIC. Un modèle computationnel de l amygdale pour l apprentissage pavlovien.

Ratio and time requirements on operant schedules: effortrelated effects of nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions

John A. Parkinson, 1 Mary C. Olmstead, 2 Lindsay H. Burns, 3 Trevor W. Robbins, 1 and Barry J. Everitt 1

Complexity Made Simple

Contrasting Roles of Basolateral Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex in Impulsive Choice

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

Succumbing to instant gratification without the nucleus accumbens

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

nucleus accumbens septi hier-259 Nucleus+Accumbens birnlex_727

Enhancement of Latent Inhibition in Rats With Electrolytic Lesions of the Hippocampus

ECHO Presentation Addiction & Brain Function

Still at the Choice-Point

Basic definition and Classification of Anhedonia. Preclinical and Clinical assessment of anhedonia.

The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour

LESIONS OF THE MESOLIMBIC DOPAMINE SYSTEM DISRUPT SIGNALLED ESCAPE RESPONSES IN THE RAT

Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function

Reinforcement learning and the brain: the problems we face all day. Reinforcement Learning in the brain

Instrumental Conditioning VI: There is more than one kind of learning

Nucleus accumbens neurons encode Pavlovian approach behaviors: evidence from an autoshaping paradigm

The attribution of incentive salience to a stimulus that signals an intravenous injection of cocaine

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Nucleus accumbens lesions impair context, but not cue, conditioning in rats

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6

Associative learning

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

THE AMYGDALA AND REWARD

Anatomy of the basal ganglia. Dana Cohen Gonda Brain Research Center, room 410

The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in reward related behavior in addiction and alcoholism in particular

Behavioral Neuroscience

The Contribution of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Orbitofrontal Cortex, and Dorsomedial Striatum to Behavioral Flexibility

THE PERSISTENCE OF MALADAPTIVE MEMORY: ADDICTION, DRUG

TO BE MOTIVATED IS TO HAVE AN INCREASE IN DOPAMINE. The statement to be motivated is to have an increase in dopamine implies that an increase in

MeCP2 and psychostimulantinduced behavioral adaptations. Anne E. West, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurobiology Duke University Medical Center

NUCLEUS ACCUMBENS NEURONAL ACTIVITY DURING A SENSORY PRECONDITIONING TASK. Domenic Hayden Cerri

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

processes in the central nervous system (CNS), affecting many of the during the course of ethanol treatment. Ethanol stimulates the release of

A visual safety signal improves learning of an auditory avoidance task

Appetitive overshadowing is disrupted by systemic amphetamine but not by electrolytic lesions to the nucleus accumbens shell

Abstract. Introduction. Keywords: appetitive conditioning, amygdala, context, cue, hippocampus

Eighth Edition. Part I: Current Science of Addiction, Relapse & Recovery: Dispelling The Stigmas

Reward Systems: Human

Teach-SHEET Basal Ganglia

Ventral Striatum and Orbitofrontal Cortex Are Both Required for Model-Based, But Not Model-Free, Reinforcement Learning

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia

Chapter 2: Studies of Human Learning and Memory. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

BRAIN MECHANISMS OF REWARD AND ADDICTION

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

Draft 2 Neuroscience DRAFT work in progress

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Neurobiology of Addiction

The Brain, Behavior and Addiction National Family Dialogue January 27, 2010 Presenter: Flo Hilliard, MSH University of Wisconsin-Madison

Dissociable Roles of Dopamine Within the Core and Medial Shell of the Nucleus Accumbens in Memory for Objects and Place

The effects of amygdala lesions on conditioned stimulus-potentiated eating in rats

REVIEW Neural and psychological mechanisms underlying compulsive drug seeking habits and drug memories indications for novel treatments of addiction*

Differential Effects of 6-OHDA Lesions of the Frontal Cortex and Caudate Nucleus on the Ability to Acquire an Attentional Set

CURRICULUM VITAE. Robert W Sikes PhD. Address:

Michael A. McDannald January 9, 2018

Deficits in Attentional Orienting Following Damage to the Perirhinal or Postrhinal Cortices

Ch. 18. Brain Mechanisms of Emotion. Bear et al., Neuroscience: Exploring the Brain, 3 rd ed., Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2006, pp

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

Fornix Lesions Impair Context-Related Cingulothalamic Neuronal Patterns and Concurrent Discrimination Learning in Rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus)

Class 16 Emotions (10/19/17) Chapter 10

LESSON 3.4 WORKBOOK. Can you become addicted to food?

Neurotransmitter Functioning In Major Depressive Disorder

Council on Chemical Abuse Annual Conference November 2, The Science of Addiction: Rewiring the Brain

Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex Lesions Differentially Influence Choices during Object Reversal Learning

Limbic system. Dr Devendra Save

Intact negative patterning in rats with fornix or combined perirhinal and postrhinal cortex lesions

Thalamic--Prefrontal Cortical--Ventral Striatal Circuitry Mediates Dissociable Components of Strategy Set Shifting

Organizing behavior in time is a key feature of goal-directed behavior and humans

Basal Ganglia. Introduction. Basal Ganglia at a Glance. Role of the BG

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

ZNZ Advanced Course in Neuroscience Mon Limbic System II. David P. Wolfer MD

Contextual Control of Conditioned Responding in Rats With Dorsal Hippocampal Lesions

Psychology & Neuroscience ISSN: Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro. Brasil

Cognition in Parkinson's Disease and the Effect of Dopaminergic Therapy

Encoding Specific Associative Memory: Evidence From Behavioral and Neural Manipulations

Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine JD Salamone, M Correa, SM Mingote and SM Weber

Basolateral amygdala lesions in the rat produce an abnormally persistent latent inhibition with weak preexposure but not with context shift

Brain Stimulation in the Study and Treatment of Addiction. From Animal Models to Humans

Impaired Outcome-Specific Devaluation of Instrumental Responding in Mice with a Targeted Deletion of the AMPA Receptor Glutamate Receptor 1 Subunit

Animals engaging in operant testing were moved to a 9a-9p reversed light cycle to permit testing during

Orbitofrontal cortex. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. Approximate location of the OFC shown on a sagittal MRI

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Nicholas W. Simon Ph.D. Curriculum Vitae

Effects of Drugs on the Brain and Behavior in Adolescents

Thalamic Cortical Striatal Circuitry Subserves Working Memory during Delayed Responding on a Radial Arm Maze

Contingent Versus Incidental Context Processing During Conditioning: Dissociation After Excitotoxic Hippocampal Plus Dentate Gyrus Lesions

Behavioural Brain Research

Transcription:

Effects of limbic corticostriatal lesions on a u t o shaping performance in rats BJ Everitt, JA Parkinson, G Lachenal, KM Halkerston, N Rudarakanchana, RN Cardinal, J Hall, CH Morrison, JW Dalley, SR Howes, TW Robbins Departments of Experimental Psychology and Anatomy, University of Cambridge, UK Abstract. It has previously been shown that acquisition of Pavlovian conditioned approach behaviour (autoshaping) in rats depends on a circuit involving the anterior cingulate cortex (Ant Cing), the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC), accumbens dopamine (DA) innervation, and the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeN). This study sought to determine which elements of this circuit are required for performance of the task in well-trained animals. Rats were first trained that one visual stimulus, the CS+, was always followed by food, while another, the CS, never predicted food. As a consequence, they came to approach the CS+ selectively. Subjects that reached a performance criterion subsequently received excitotoxic lesions of the Ant Cing (lesion n = 11 / sham ), AcbC (ns = 9 / 7), or CeN ( / 7), or -OHDA-induced dopamine depletion of the entire nucleus accumbens (7 / 7). Subjects were then re-tested. Ant Cing lesions and AcbC lesions both impaired autoshaping performance. Acb DA depletion also impaired performance and the impairment was correlated with the degree of dopamine depletion, though the deficit was mild compared to that observed for the acquisition of autoshaping (Everitt et al., 1999). In contrast, rats with CeN lesions were unimpaired. These results support the view that the limbic corticostriatal circuit involving Ant Cing and AcbC is involved in the storage and/or expression of appetitive stimulus reward associations, but suggest a critical role for CeN in early learning, which may reflect its role as a controller of attentional resources (Holland & Gallagher, 1999). SFN autoshaping 1 of 9

Introduction Autoshaping is a phenomenon in which subjects approach a conditioned stimulus (CS) that predicts an appetitive outcome (such as food), even though this approach behaviour has no effect on food delivery (Brown & Jenkins, 19). Autoshaping reflects Pavlovian conditioned approach (Williams & Williams, 199), and has been suggested to correlate with vulnerability to drug addiction (Tomie, 199). In the autoshaping task used in our laboratory, two stimuli are used. A CS+ is presented, and followed by food delivery at a different spatial location. A second stimulus (CS ) is also presented, but never followed by food. Normal rats develop a discriminated approach response in which they approach the CS+ before returning to the food hopper to collect the reward. It has previously been shown that the acquisition of autoshaping depends upon the Ant Cing (Bussey et al., 1997a; Parkinson et al., b), the AcbC (Parkinson et al., b), the CeN (Parkinson et al., a), and the dopaminergic innervation of the nucleus accumbens (see Everitt et al., 1999). However, it is not known whether these structures are simply required to learn the task, or whether they contribute to performance in a well-trained subject. The present study sought to address this issue. Rats were trained to a criterion on this task, received lesions of one of these structures, and were re-tested. A simplified schematic of part of the limbic loop of the basal ganglia. Grey shading indicates the regions lesioned in the present study. (Abbreviations: Acb nucleus accumbens; Ant Cing anterior cingulate cortex; mpfc medial prefrontal cortex; BLA basolateral amygdala; CeN central nucleus of the amygdala; VTA ventral tegmental area; VP ventral pallidum.) SFN autoshaping of 9

Methods Autoshaping task Autoshaping was assessed in a testing chamber with a computer monitor on one wall and a centrally-located pellet dispenser. Subjects were trained to associate stimuli with the delivery of 5-mg sucrose pellets. The stimuli were 1 cm white vertical rectangles displayed on the left and right of the screen for 1 s. One was designated the CS+ and the other the CS, counterbalanced across subjects. When the rat was located centrally at the rear of the chamber, a stimulus was presented. The CS+ was always followed by delivery of one food pellet; the CS was never followed by food. Activation of a pressure-sensitive floor panel in front of a stimulus was scored as approach. A trial consisted of a presentation of the CS+ and one of the CS, separated by a variable interval of at least 1 s. Rats were trained with 5 trials per day. Experimental design Male hooded Lister rats were maintained at 9% of their free-feeding body weight. They were trained for 1 trials. Subjects that failed to approach the CS+ on 7% of the last 3 trials were given a further 5 remedial trials. Subjects that failed to meet this criterion on the last 3 remedial trials were excluded. (For the AcbC study, the criterion was approach to the CS+ twice as often as the CS on the last 3 trials of training, with 3 or remedial trials.) Successful subjects were assigned to groups for surgery. Following surgery, they were retested for a further 5 trials. Excitotoxic lesions Excitotoxic lesions were made with.9 M quinolinic acid or. M ibotenic acid; sham-operated control rats were injected with vehicle only. Coordinates were taken from Paxinos & Watson (199) with the incisor bar at 3.3 mm. For Ant Cing lesions,.5 µl quinolinic acid was injected at the following coordinates relative to bregma: AP +1., ML ±.5, DV 3. and.; AP +.5, ML ±.5, DV. and.; AP., ML ±.5, DV.5 and.. For AcbC lesions,.5 µl quinolinic acid was injected at the following coordinates: AP +1., ML ±1., DV 7.1. For CeN lesions,. µl ibotenic acid was injected at the following coordinates: AP., ML ±., DV 7. from dura; AP.7, ML ±., DV 7. from dura. Neurochemical lesions and assessment Coordinates were taken from the atlas of Pellegrino et al. (1979), with the incisor bar set at +5. mm. Dopamine-depleting lesions of the nucleus accumbens (Acb) were made by injecting µl of 3 µm -hydroxydopamine (-OHDA) in.1% ascorbic acid at the following coordinates relative to bregma: AP +3., ML ±1.7, DV 7. from dura. After testing, subjects were killed by CO inhalation and the brains were processed for neurochemical analysis. Tissue levels of noradrenaline (NA), dopamine (DA), serotonin (5-HT), the catecholamines metabolites dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) and homovanillic acid (HVA), and the 5-HT metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) were determined by reverse phase high-performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection (HPLC-ECD). Samples of tissue were taken from the left and right nucleus accumbens (ventral striatum: VS) and from control areas, namely prefrontal cortex (PFC) and dorsal striatum (DS). Subjects that did not meet a criterion of 5% depletion of dopamine from the VS (that is, DA 3% of the mean level of the sham group) were excluded from the study. SFN autoshaping 3 of 9

1. Effects of lesions of the ante rior cingulate cortex (Ant Cing) 1 Ant Cing lesions impaired the performance of autoshaping. Number of approaches 1 sham CS+ ACCX CS+ sham CS- ACCX CS- Acquisition Performance Raw approach scores, preand post-operatively. (Breaks in the axis indicate the time of surgery.) Anterior cingulate lesions caused a persistent deficit in CS+ approach. Twenty subjects reached the initial performance criterion. Following histological analysis, the final group sizes were 11 (ACCX) and (sham). Pre-operatively, the groups did not differ. Post-operatively, the ACCX group was significantly impaired relative to the sham group. Both groups showed a post-operative decline in CS responding, and performance of the ACCX group did improve over testing. However, the ACCX group showed a persistent deficit in CS+ approaches. Difference score sham ACCX Difference scores (CS+ approaches minus CS approaches) calculated for the same data. The ACCX group is significantly impaired. SFN autoshaping of 9

. Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core (AcbC) 1 AcbC lesions impaired the performance of autoshaping. Number of approaches Acquisition Performance Raw approach scores, preand post-operatively. AcbC lesions caused a persistent deficit, particularly in CS+ approach. Final group sizes were 9 (AcbC) and 7 (sham). Though the groups did not differ pre-operatively, there was a severe and persistent impairment of performance in the AcbC-lesioned group. This group approached the CS+ significantly less than shams. sham CS+ AcbC CS+ sham CS- AcbC CS- 1 Difference score sham AcbC Difference scores calculated for the same data. The AcbC group is significantly impaired. Trial block SFN autoshaping 5 of 9

3. Effects of lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeN) 1 CeN lesions did not impair the performance of autoshaping. Number of approaches sham CS+ Ce CS+ sham CS- Ce CS- Raw approach scores, preand post-operatively. There was no deficit in CeNlesioned rats. Final group sizes were (CeN) and 1 (sham). The groups did not differ pre-operatively, and the lesion caused no deficit in performance. Acquisition Performance 1 9 Difference score 7 5 3 1 sham Ce Difference scores calculated for the same data. There was no difference between the groups. Trial block SFN autoshaping of 9

. Effects of dopamine depletion of the nucleus accumbens Difference score Number of approaches 1 1 9 7 5 3 1 sham CS+ -OHDA CS+ sham CS- -OHDA CS- Acquisition Performance Trial block sham -OHDA Raw approach scores, preand post-operatively. Dopamine depletion of the nucleus accumbens induced a small but persistent reduction in CS+ approach. Difference scores calculated for the same data. The - OHDA group is significantly impaired. Dopamine depletion of Acb impaired the performance of autoshaping. The groups did not differ pre-operatively. However, there was a small but persistent deficit in the -OHDA group post-operatively; the lesioned group approached the CS+ less often than shams. Final group sizes were 7 (-OHDA) and 7 (sham). The lesion induced an % depletion of DA from the ventral striatum, with corresponding decreases in the metabolites DOPAC and HVA. Changes in NA, 5-HT and 5-HIAA were not significant. The lesion also depleted DA and DOPAC from the dorsal striatum, and NA and DOPAC from the PFC. No other differences were significant. There was a significant correlation between VS dopamine and autoshaping performance (r =.51, p =.9). There was also a correlation of PFC NA with autoshaping performance, but when the correlation between PFC NA and VS DA was partialled out, the contribution of PFC NA was not significant (p =.59). No other correlations were significant. SFN autoshaping 7 of 9

Discussion and conclusions Anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus accumbens core Lesions of the Ant Cing and AcbC both impaired the performance of autoshaping. Both these structures are also necessary for the acquisition of autoshaping (Bussey et al., 1997a; Parkinson et al., b), which requires a functional connection between the two (Parkinson et al., b). However, these structures probably make different contributions to conditioned approach behaviour. While the AcbC is required for many forms of conditioned approach (Parkinson et al., 1999; Parkinson et al., b), the Ant Cing appears critical only when multiple CSs must be discriminated (see Cardinal et al., this meeting, programme #3.13). Some recovery of function was observed in Ant Cing-lesioned rats, raising the possibility that the Ant Cing has a time-limited role in autoshaping, as has been suggested by other rodent studies (Freeman et al., 199; Gabriel, 1993; Gabriel et al., 19, p1; Hart et al., 1997). Dopamine innervation of the nucleus accumbens Central nucleus of the amygdala Lesions of the CeN did not impair autoshaping performance. This result is in dramatic contrast to that of Parkinson et al. (a), who found that CeN lesions profoundly impaired the acquisition of the autoshaped response. These results suggest that the CeN, unique among the structures studied here, contributes selectively to learning of the conditioned approach response. One possibility is that the CeN is crucial for learning because of its role as an attentional controller. Several theories of learning postulate a role for attention in the formation of Pavlovian associations (Pearce & Hall, 19; Sutherland & Mackintosh, 1971), while the CeN has been shown to be involved in the attentional regulation of stimulus associability (Gallagher & Holland, 199; Holland & Gallagher, 1999), and to contribute to performance in other tests of attentional function (Holland et al., ). -OHDA-induced dopamine depletion of Acb impaired autoshaping performance by reducing the number of approaches to the CS+. This difference was not attributable to generally low levels of activity, as these animals were significantly hyperactive (data not shown). These data suggest that Acb DA contributes to the performance of a welllearned conditioned approach response. However, the impairment was much smaller than that observed when DA depletion occurs before training (see Everitt et al., 1999), suggesting an additional role for Acb DA in learning. SFN autoshaping of 9

Acknowledgements Supported by the Medical Research Council (UK) and a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant. Conducted within the MRC Co-operative for Brain, Behaviour and Neuropsychiatry. RNC was supported by a research studentship from the UK Medical Research Council and a James Baird award from the University of Cambridge School of Clinical Medicine. JH was supported by Trinity College, Cambridge. Presented at the Society for Neuroscience 3 th Annual Meeting, 9 November, New Orleans, Louisiana, USA. (Monday November, 5 pm; programme #3.1; location MM-3.) A PDF version of this poster will shortly be available at <http://www.pobox.com/users/rudolf/publications>. R e f e r e n c e s Brown, P. L. & Jenkins, H. M. (19). Auto-shaping of the pigeon's keypeck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11, 1-. Bussey, T. J., Everitt, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. (1997a). Dissociable effects of cingulate and medial frontal cortex lesions on stimulus-reward learning using a novel Pavlovian autoshaping procedure for the rat: implications for the neurobiology of emotion. Behav Neurosci 111(5), 9-19. Everitt, B. J., Parkinson, J. A., Olmstead, M. C., Arroyo, M., Robledo, P. & Robbins, T. W. (1999). Associative processes in addiction and reward - The role of amygdala-ventral striatal subsystems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 77, 1-3. Freeman, J. H., Jr., Cuppernell, C., Flannery, K. & Gabriel, M. (199). Limbic thalamic, cingulate cortical and hippocampal neuronal correlates of discriminative approach learning in rabbits. Behav Brain Res (1-), 13-3. Gabriel, M. (1993). Discriminative avoidance learning: a model system. In Neurobiology of cingulate cortex and limbic thalamus: a comprehensive handbook (Vogt, B. A. & Gabriel, M., eds.), pp. 7-53. Birkhauser, Boston, Massachusetts. Gabriel, M., Foster, K., Orona, E., Saltwick, S. & Stanton, M., Eds. (19). Neuronal activity of cingulate cortex, anteroventral thalamus, and hippocampal formation in discriminative conditioning: encoding and extraction of the significance of conditional stimuli. Vol. 9. Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology. Edited by Sprague, J. M. & Epstein, A. N.: Academic Press. Gallagher, M. & Holland, P. C. (199). The amygdala complex: multiple roles in associative learning and attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 91(5), 11771-. Hart, M., Poremba, A. & Gabriel, M. (1997). The nomadic engram: overtraining eliminates the impairment of discriminative avoidance behavior produced by limbic thalamic lesions. Behav Brain Res (), 19-77. Holland, P. C. & Gallagher, M. (1999). Amygdala circuitry in attentional and representational processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3, 5-73. Holland, P. C., Han, J. S. & Gallagher, M. (). Lesions of the Amygdala Central Nucleus Alter Performance on a Selective Attention Task. Journal of Neuroscience, 71-7. Parkinson, J. A., Olmstead, M. C., Burns, L. H., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. (1999). Dissociation in effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on appetitive Pavlovian approach behavior and the potentiation of conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity by d-amphetamine. J Neurosci 19(), 1-11. Parkinson, J. A., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. (a). Dissociable roles of the central and basolateral amygdala in appetitive emotional learning. European Journal of Neuroscience 1(1), 5-13. Parkinson, J. A., Willoughby, P. J., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. (b). Disconnection of the anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus accumbens core impairs Pavlovian approach behavior: Further evidence for limbic cortical-ventral striatopallidal systems. Behavioral Neuroscience 11(1), -3. Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. (199). The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Fourth edit, Academic Press. Pearce, J. M. & Hall, G. (19). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol. Rev. 1, 53-55. Pellegrino, L. J., Pellegrino, A. S. & Cushman, A. J. (1979). A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Rat Brain, Plenum, New York. Sutherland, N. S. & Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of Animal Discrimination Learning, Academic Press, New York. Tomie, A. (199). Locating reward cue at response manipulandum (CAM) induces symptoms of drug abuse. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (3), 55-35. Williams, D. R. & Williams, H. (199). Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent nonreinforcement. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1, 511-5. SFN autoshaping 9 of 9