References 1. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2006). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th edition. Minnesota: American Guidance Service. 2. Wilkinson, K.

Similar documents
Spontaneous Attention to Faces in Asperger Sndrome using Ecologically Valid Static Stimuli

The use of eye-tracking to explore social difficulties in cognitively able students with autism spectrum disorder: A pilot study

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Neuropsychologia xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

2017 Gatlinburg Conference Symposium Submission SS-10

Visual preference for social stimuli in individuals with autism or neurodevelopmental disorders: an eye-tracking study

Attentional allocation of autism spectrum disorder individuals : searching for a Face-in-the-Crowd

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Abnormal adaptive face-coding mechanisms in children with autism. spectrum disorder

Title: Symbol-Infused Play for Young Children with Complex Communication Needs

Differential effects of anxiety and autism on social scene scanning in males with fragile X syndrome

Story Time Language Program Summary Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University

Brief Report: Interrater Reliability of Clinical Diagnosis and DSM-IV Criteria for Autistic Disorder: Results of the DSM-IV Autism Field Trial

SOCIAL COMPARISON, SELF- CONCEPT AND MOOD IN ADULTS WITH DOWN SYNDROME

Shining in the Center: Central Gaze Cascade Effect on Product Choice

Multiple Measures of Fixation on Social Content in Infancy: Evidence for a Single Social Cognitive Construct?

Fact Sheet 8. DSM-5 and Autism Spectrum Disorder

Copyright 2007 (please consult author)

NeuroTracker Published Studies & Research

Eye tracking in early autism research

ASHA Comments* (ASHA Recommendations Compared to DSM-5 Criteria) Austism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: Version: Accepted Version

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

Chairperson, Dr. John Colombo. Dr. Christa Anderson. Dr. Brenda Salley

Evidence-Based Practices and Students With Autism Spectrum Disorders Focus Evidence-Based Reading Instruction for Individuals With Autism Spectrum.

Supporting Online Material for

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives

A Reliability Study for Transcription

Clinical Study Visual Scanning in Very Young Children with Autism and Their Unaffected Parents

60 minutes Give examples of SMARTs that are completed or in the field o ASD, child ADHD, women who are pregnant and abuse substances, adult alcohol

Applying models of visual attention to gaze patterns of children with autism

Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism

Unimpaired Attentional Disengagement and Social Orienting in Children with Autism

Take another look: Pupillometry and cognitive processing demands

Durham Research Online

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Handbook Of Autism And Pervasive Developmental Disorders Assessment Interventions And Policy

Non-intrusive and calibration free visual exploration analysis in children with autism spectrum disorder

Oklahoma Psychological Association DSM-5 Panel November 8-9, 2013 Jennifer L. Morris, Ph.D.

INSAR Autism Research 00: 00 00, Elisabeth A. H. von dem Hagen and Naomi Bright. Introduction

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Report of Children with Disabilities (IDEA) Ages 6 through 21 by Disability, Educational Environment, and Age Group (OSEP010)

Autism Spectrum Disorders

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

Research Article Normal Gaze Cueing in Children with Autism Is Disrupted by Simultaneous Speech Utterances in Live Face-to-Face Interactions

An Analysis of Emotion Recognition and Facial Processing Across Human and Cartoon Stimuli in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Soc Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 February 8.

A thesis presented to. the faculty of. the College of Health Sciences and Professions of Ohio University. In partial fulfillment

IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON AUTONOMOUS MENTAL DEVELOPMENT, VOL. X, NO. Y, MONTH

Differential Autism Diagnosis The Role of an SLP in Evaluating Social Communication Differences

Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Measuring the stare-in-the-crowd effect: a new paradigm to study social perception

Evidence-Based Communication Interventions for Persons with Severe Disabilities

Differential Viewing Strategies towards Attractive and Unattractive Human Faces

Attentional Differences in a Driving Hazard Perception Task in Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

Running head: SOCIAL SKILLS AND AUTISM 1

DSM-IV Criteria. (1) qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

Unimpaired Attentional Disengagement and Social Orienting in Children With Autism

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Augment Altern Commun. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 June 01.

Durham Research Online

PREDICTIVE GAZE DURING OBSERVATION OF IRRATIONAL ACTIONS IN ADULTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM CONDITIONS

Table 1: Comparison of DSM-5 and DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic Criteria. Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) Pervasive Developmental Disorders Key Differences

Lack of Visual Orienting to Biological Motion and Audiovisual Synchrony in 3-Year-Olds with Autism

40

Getting Started with AAC

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR HEALTH AND CLINICAL EXCELLENCE Centre for Clinical Practice SCOPE

2. Do you work with children and/or adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)? Yes No If No Is Selected, the survey will discontinue.

Correspondence should be addressed to Denise Davidson;

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ATTENTIONAL PROCESSES AND SOCIAL REFERENCING IN INFANTS AT HIGH AND LOW GENETIC RISK FOR AUTISM.

2017 PSHA Convention 3/31/2017

Slow Motor Responses to Visual Stimuli of Low Salience in Autism

National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. NICE Quality Standards Consultation attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Receptive Language Skills among Younger, Adolescent, and Adults with Down Syndrome: The Use of the Growth-Scale- Vocabulary as a Measure

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

"My Little Pony" in adults eyes. How do men and women watch cartoons? An eye-tracking experiment

Critical Review: Do Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) Increase Natural Speech Production in Children with Developmental Disabilities?

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010

Agenda. Making the Connection. Facts about ASD. Respite Presentation. Agenda. Facts about ASD. Triad of Impairments. 3 Diagnoses on spectrum

The Social Side of ASD

Improving Search Task Performance Using Subtle Gaze Direction

Development of Objective Index to Determine Autism by Eyeball Movements

Acoustic Analysis of Prosody in Spontaneous Productions of Minimally Verbal Children and Adolescents with Autism

Investigating a Robot as a Therapy Partner for Children with. Autism

Thank You: Developmental social neuroscience meets public health challenge: The next generations of children with autism spectrum disorder

Working memory in. development: Links with learning between. typical and atypical populations. TRACY PACKIAM ALLOWAY Durham University, UK

UC Merced UC Merced Undergraduate Research Journal

Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Extraction of Fusiform Gyrus Area of Brain to Analyze Autism

Why do we look at people's eyes?

Visual face scanning and emotion perception analysis between Autistic and Typically Developing children

Northern Illinois University. Joint Attention and Joint Engagement Among Minimally Verbal Children With Autism:

URL: <

The Effects of Familiarity on Visual Search Performance of Typically Developing Toddlers and Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Transcription:

Aim This presentation will present data from eye-tracking research technology concerning social attention patterns: gaze fixations on human figures either interacting or not interacting with each other in photographs. The data were obtained from school-aged children and adolescents with autism and typical development. Our specific aim was to evaluate the allocation of visual attention to photographs in which human figures are either interacting or not interacting with each other. The main purpose of the study was to help inform the design of visual scene displays, by determining whether interacting human figures in photographs attract visual attention in the same way as non-interacting human figures. A secondary outcome of the study was to add further information to the growing literature on patterns of visual attention to human figures in individuals with autism, which has yielded mixed results about whether gaze to human figures in static photographs is atypical. Method Data collection related to the present study is complete with 33 individuals with autism and 13 chronological age- and -gender-matched typically developing individuals, between the ages of 8 and 20 years. All participants with autism exhibited moderate to profound delays in vocabulary as evaluated by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4 th Edition (PPVT-4 1 ). A language-measure was used for selection, because the current study aims to inform materials that would be used to support communication functioning. 2 Participants without disabilities were all performing at grade level according to parental reports. Participants freely viewed 5 photographs containing interacting humans and 5 containing non-interacting humans. The study acquired data via the Tobii T60 eyetracking system, a set of technologies designed for research purposes. The T60 monitor contained a built-in projection strip, by which infrared light was projected onto the participants pupils and corneas. The light reflected off the eyes was captured by three built-in cameras. Point of gaze was recorded by the Studio software at a rate of 6 samples per second. A fixation was defined as a series of consecutive samples in which the gaze remained within a 35 pixel area for greater than 100 milliseconds. Patterns of fixation on photographs were evaluated for each participant. The dependent variable was the amount of time spent on viewing interacting versus non interacting human figures, and non-human elements (ball, flower, Christmas present, etc.). Results Both groups spent more time on humans than non-human elements, fixating on average on interacting-humans for 45.3% of the total picture-viewing time for participants with autism, and 57.5% for the typically developing controls, and fixating on non-interacting humans for 38.4% of the total picture-viewing time for those with autism, and 44.7% for the controls. The ratio of fixation duration to the interacting humans was 1.29 times for those with autism, and 1.63 times for those with typical development; the ratio of fixation duration to non-interacting humans was 4.25 times for those with autism, and 5.4 times for the controls. It demonstrated that given the sizes of human figures, both groups spent approximately 3.3 times more on viewing the non-interacting humans than on viewing the interacting humans. Two possibilities may explain the viewing pattern: first, irrespective of the clinical profiles, all participants needed more time to inspect the

human figures because the sizes of non-interacting humans were generally smaller than those of the interacting humans; second, in contrast to the controls, social-interaction events did provoke more avoidance than non-social interaction events for the autistic group. To rule out the first possibility, a second study is underway in which human figures of comparable sizes across stimuli are used. Stimuli sets are larger than the ones in the preliminary study: 32 target photos (containing interacting or non-interacting human figures) and 16 fillers. Additionally, considering the possibility that atypical fixations on human figures were not specific to autism, 2 the current study also involved languagematched participants with Down syndrome. Data collected using the new stimuli set showed that individuals with Down syndrome fixated on average on interacting-humans for 58.5% of the total picture-viewing, whereas typically developing controls fixated for 56.7%; individuals with Down syndrome fixated on average on non-interacting humans for 51.7% of the total picture-viewing time, whereas the typically developing controls fixated for 60.1%. This indicates that individuals with Down syndrome attended to interacting humans more than non-interacting humans whereas opposite pattern was observed in typically developing controls. Additional data for the new stimuli sets will be collected within the month to bring more participants with autism, Down syndrome, and typical development for pair-wise comparisons (e.g., autism vs. Down syndrome). Conclusion This research has implications for our understanding of gaze patterns across etiology categories, and in turn, informs the design of augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) materials. Research with individuals with autism yielded mixed results as to whether there is avoidance of gaze to images containing humans. Some reports suggest the presence of gaze avoidance, 3,4,5 while other research does not. 2,6 The level of social demands in stimuli may account for the discrepancies. 7 Many studies used one-person photographs, reporting no significant between-group differences in viewing patterns. In contrast, when viewing sets of stimuli in which two or more persons were present, individuals with autism spent significantly less time viewing the faces than the typically developing controls. 4 Therefore, the current study is of value as it aims to evaluate whether the degrees of social demands would influence gaze patterns in participants with autism. All participants spent more time viewing the human figures than non-human elements. From the perspective of AAC displays, the results strongly indicate that in static images of the sort we used, human figures were generally attractive to participants with autism. However, the presence of social-interaction events in photographs may influence viewing patterns differently from that of non-social interaction events. If additional data collected suggest atypical gaze behaviors toward interacting humans, clinicians and manufacturers creating photograph-based AAC displays should consider the degree of social demands in photographs based on the concerns about gaze avoidance. This presentation will illustrate the method, findings, and results of this study.

References 1. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2006). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th edition. Minnesota: American Guidance Service. 2. Wilkinson, K. M., & Light, J. (2014). Preliminary study of gaze toward humans in photographs by individuals with autism, down syndrome, or other intellectual disabilities: implications for design of visual scene displays. Augmentative Alternative Communication, 30, 130-146. 3. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809-816. 4. Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2008). Viewing it differently: social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2855-2860. 5. Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. B. (2009). Looking at movies and cartoons: eye-tracking evidence from Williams syndrome and autism. Journal of Intellect Disability Research, 53, 169-181. 6. Kemner, C., van der Geest, J. N., Verbaten, M. N., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Effects of object complexity and type on the gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder. Brain Cognition, 65, 107-111. 7. Gillespie-Smith, K., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2014). Designing AAC systems for children with autism: evidence from eye tracking research. Augmentative Alternative Communication, 30, 160-171.

Oct. 29 th, 2015