How Visibility and Convenience Influence Candy Consumption *

Similar documents
Blue Zones. Kitchen Checklist. Directions

Weigh To Go. September 1, 2010

Smarter Lunchrooms Movement

Obesity Management Modules Managing Your Appetite. Pause/play Backward/ Forward

Reducing Mindless Eating: Putting an End to the Crazy Cycle. Presented By Sam Aspenson Viterbo Dietetic Student

Consumer Research at the FDA

11/17/2017. Cafeteria Reinvention Small Changes that Make a Big Difference. It s Not Nutrition Until It s Eaten

BLUE ZONES PROJECT START CREATING A HEALTHIER, HAPPIER LIFE, TODAY.

How Are People Influenced to Eat Too Much Without Awareness? StanMark. Deborah A. Cohen, MD, MPH May, Outline

GMO Awareness and Eating Behaviors Among College Students. Sarah Galvin Department of Sociology University of New Hampshire December 17, 2015

Disclosures. Karen Ansel, MS, RDN, CDN. David R. Just, PhD. The presenters have certified that no conflict of interest exists for this program.

Fool Yourself Full: A Mindful Approach to Snacking and Weight Management

Faculty & Research. Working Paper Series. Health Halos: How Nutrition Claims Influence Food Consumption for Overweight and Normal Weight People

Purchasing Patterns for Nutritional-Enhanced Foods: The case of Calcium-Enriched Orange Juice

Remember kcal daily amounts to 10 ½ - 31 lbs. ( Kg) in a year! Safe estimate = 100 kcal / day = 10 lbs. / year

DON'T WORK AND WHAT YOU CAN DO ABOUT IT. BY DR. RHONA EPSTEIN

Sparking Youth to Create Healthy Communities

Nutrition Education Competencies for Preschool Children

Food Waste Reduction: How Can Technology Help? Andrew Parry Project Manager, WRAP

INTERNATIONAL FOOD INFORMATION COUNCIL. Food Psychology: Why We Eat More Than We Think. Speaker Disclosure 6/22/2017

JIM PAINTER PHD, RD SUN-MAID RAISINS

Swiss Food Panel. -A longitudinal study about eating behaviour in Switzerland- ENGLISH. Short versions of selected publications. Zuerich,

Food & Nutrition Environment Assessment

SENSORY SUGGESTIVENESS AND LABELING: DO SOY LABELS BIAS TASTE?

Changing Environments for Healthy Habits: Uncertainties and Challenges

Attitudes towards products with health claims. focus groups, Serbia

You probably don t spend a lot of time here, but if you do, you are reacting to the most basic needs a human has survival and protection.

Triggers of food behaviours: Habit and Unconscious Influences

TRACKS Lesson Plan. Caregiver Workshop - Deciphering the Nutrition Facts Label Audience: Caregivers

Food in Your Life. Chapter 5 Lesson 1

People who were served soup from bottomless, refillable soup bowls ate 73% more soup than those eating from conventional bowls 4

American Journal of Preventive Medicine

Chapter 13 Weight Loss: A Healthy Lifestyle Side Effect

MILLENNIALS AND ORANGE JUICE CONSUMPTION

Healthy Habits For Weight Management

Caffeine Consumption and Anxiety Levels: A Convenience Sample. Operation Righteous Cowboy Lightning: Ian Sande, Johnny Rider,

They Know They Ought to, So Why Don t They? Breaking Down the Barriers to Healthier Eating Laura Staugaitytė & Tino Bech-Larsen

Workshop on Understanding the Relationship Between Food Insecurity and Obesity Sentinel Populations November 16, 2010

Domain-specificity and domain-generality in self-control

HEDONIC HUNGER AND SELF-CONTROL: THE IMPACT OF PALATABILITY, POWER OF FOOD AND DIETARY RESTRAINT ON SELF-CONTROL DEPLETION. Kathleen M.

Consumption trends for fruits and vegetables Brussels December 2017

Cognitive Load and Neuro-Economics: Implications for Food Consumption and Health. George Davis and Elena Serrano Virginia Tech 8/15/2016

ESPEN Congress The Hague 2017

Sugar is a type of bodily fuel, yes, but your body runs about as well on it as a car would. Healthy Halloween. Fighting the Urge to Splurge

Why They Buy. Fighting Obesity Through Consumer Marketing Research.

Consumer Behaviours, Attitudes & Knowledge Towards Food & Waste

CONSUMER AGE INFLUENCE ON FOOD LABEL READING HABIT

Consumer food waste Evidence and action Andrew Parry

Exploring the gap between healthy eating intentions and healthy eating behaviours using qualitative in-depth interviews

OLDER ADULTS. Persons 65 or older

Changing Patterns. healthyfutures.nhs.uk/llb

Nancy Sittler Region Of Waterloo Public Health

James Morgan/WWF-International

Session 14: Take Charge of Your Lifestyle

The sociologist view point on the changes between the French consumer and the food

Food marketing. Tim Lobstein. International Association for the Study of Obesity

An evaluation of a theory based childhood overweight prevention curriculum

STAR Sportsmanship The STAR Method

Research Bulletin No 2: The influence of deprivation on knowledge, attitudes and healthy eating behaviours.

SECTION 1. Introducing Simply Beef and the Beef Alternative Merchandising program

Reducing Portion Size Reduces Food Intake and Plate Waste

A teaching presentation to help general psychology students overcome the common misconception that correlation equals causation

HOW TO ASSESS NUTRITION IN CHILDREN & PROVIDE PRACTICAL RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE FAMILY

Reasons. Storage options Buying and storing food. Activity: Where food should be stored and why

The Effects of Social Interactions on Consumption: A Test of Social Facilitation

English *P48984A0112* E202/01. Pearson Edexcel Functional Skills. P48984A 2015 Pearson Education Ltd. Level 2 Component 2: Reading

Nutritional Labeling. University of Connecticut. Emily J. Williams University of Connecticut - Storrs,


Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

Effect of Food Advertisements on Eating Pattern of School Going Children between Ages Years

Network for a Healthy California Retail Program Fruit and Vegetable Store Tour Guide

Using Olfaction to Prevent Obesity

The school food environment are we making the healthy choice the easy choice?

Fast food restaurant lighting and Music can reduce

Outline of the presentation

Ohio SNAP-Ed Adult & Teen Programs Foods to Decrease

Inhibitory Control Training as a potential behavioural intervention for overweight and obesity. Andrew Jones

Research Article A Study to Assess Relationship Between Nutrition Knowledge and Food Choices Among Young Females

Nutrition History and Questionnaire

HEALTHY FOODS AT WORK: GETTING THERE

K. Abeliotis, K. Lasaridi, C. Chroni, A. Papakosta PRELIMINARY ASSESSMENT of FOOD WASTE in HOUSEHOLDS in GREECE

Increased Portion Size Leads to Increased Energy Intake in a Restaurant Meal

APPENDIX 1. Examples of interventions that alter environmental stimuli to change health-related

Perspective. Biological perspective entering psychology. Understanding Eating from a psychological perspective

Taking fun to school lunchrooms

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 149 ( 2014 ) LUMEN Obesity and Nutritional Programs in Schools

Hull s Adult Health and Lifestyle Survey: Summary

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

A Cultural Hedonic Framework for Increasing the Consumption of Unfamiliar Foods: Soy Acceptance in Russia and Colombia

Welcome to Deaconess Weight Loss Solutions.

Hamburger Pork Chop Deli Ham Chicken Wing $7.76 $5.53 $4.52 $4.02 $2.45 $2.79 $3.10 $3.51

Behavioral strategies to control the amount of food selected and

Eggs and egg products: Consumers attitudes, perceptions and behaviours

Who eats what? The eating behaviour of high school students in Braşov

2017 FOOD & HEALTH SURVEY A Focus on Older Adults Funded by

So, What s In Your House? Strategies for Measuring the Home Food Environment

Activity Overview. The Eyes Have It: Portion Distortion Stations Activity 5C. Corpulosity. Activity Objectives: Activity Description:

Deadly E. coli strikes Europe

Hilary Seligman, MD, MAS

AP Statistics Chapter 5 Multiple Choice

Transcription:

The published version of this working paper is: Painter, James E., Brian Wansink, and Julie B. Hieggelke (2002), How Visiblity and Convenience Influence Candy Consumption, Appetite, 38:3 (June), 237-238 How Visibility and Convenience Influence Candy Consumption * James E. Painter (Ph.D., RD) Brian Wansink (Ph.D.) Julie Hieggelke (BS) * James E. Painter is Assistant Professor of Food Science and Human Nutrition, Division of Nutritional Sciences at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Brian Wansink is Associate Professor of Nutritional Science, of Business Administration, and of Agricultural and Consumer Economics and of at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. Julie Hieggelke is a graduate student in Food Science and Human Nutrition at the University of Illinois. Please direct correspondence to Professor James E. Painter, 905 S. Goodwin, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, 217-3333-8805, jpainter@uiuc.edu. 1 Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2719056

How Visibility and Convenience Influence Candy Consumption This paper addresses two questions: (1) How does the visibility and convenience of a hedonic food influence one s consumption, and (2) how do these factors influence one s estimate of how much he or she had consumed? Both questions have important implications for clinicians and for the nutritionally concerned. Answering the first question will show the extent to which two environmental factors (visibility and convenience) influence consumption. Answering the second will show the extent to which consumers overestimate or underestimate the influence of these factors. This is important in helping individuals monitor consumption patterns of which they may be unaware. While taste, 123 stress, anxiety 4, and motivation 5 have all been shown to influence unintended consumption, the impact of food visibility and convenience have generated mixed results. Two studies comparing food storage habits in homes of obese and non-obese families found conflicting results--the first showed that food was more visible in the homes of obese families, but the second showed the opposite. 6 Other studies have shown that convenient food can increase three fold, but that this occurs only when highly visible or salient in one s mind, 7 or when it is initially offered. 8 We do not understand whether factors like visibility and convenience have an impact that lasts beyond the first couple days of a study. 9 One problem behind these inconclusive studies may deal with the utilitarian foods they examined -- fruits, canned soup, and vegetables. There is recent evidence 10 that consumers relate to these utilitarian products in a more rational and calculating manner then they relate to hedonic products, such as chocolate, candy, ice cream, and cookies. What we do in this paper is 2 Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2719056

investigate how the visibility and the convenience of a hedonic product influences one s consumption of the product over a three week period. This addresses a topic that is relevant for dieters and for nutritionally conscious individuals. Do people eat more when a food is in sight, or when it is within reach? METHODS Subjects were 16 office workers in a university setting (10 female; median age 43 years) who agreed to be involved in a study related to candy consumption. The study was reviewed and approved by the Human Subjects Committee of the University, and it involved a three condition, between-subjects design with repeated measures. In the first week of the study, subjects were divided among three candy placement conditions. In each condition, the subjects were given a closed container holding 30 chocolate candy kisses. In the first condition, the container was placed on top of the desk, where it was visible and convenient. In the second condition, the container was placed in the subject s desk drawer, where it was convenient but not visible. In the third condition, the container was visible but inconveniently placed on a shelf two meters away so that the subject was required to leave the desk to obtain the candy. Each evening for three weeks, the containers were collected and replaced with new containers also containing 30 chocolate kisses. The number of chocolates consumed from each container was recorded daily. The replenished containers were kept in the same location for five consecutive days. On the 6 th test day (Monday of the following week), the containers were rotated to a new placement condition for each subject. On the 11 th test day, this was repeated. At the end of the three week (15 day) period, each subject was given a questionnaire which asked them to estimate their consumption of candy over the past three weeks in each of the three 3

conditions. Measures of attendance, nutrition consciousness, and dietary patterns were also taken. While subjects had agreed to be part of the study, they had not been provided with details about the study. When the study began, subjects were told that the chocolates were part of the study, but that they were also a partial thank you for their involvement. Each Monday when the containers rotated locations, the administrator who most directly dealt with the staff member inconspicuously changed the location of the candy. Because there might be a learning effect that occurred if subjects noted the location change with suspicion, analyses were conducted betweensubjects instead of within-subjects. Additionally, comparisons were made with data collected in the three conditions for the first week versus the same three conditions in the second and third week. The pattern of results were similar for each of the weeks, and therefore all data was used in the analysis. In analyzing the data using SPSS software, a repeated measures, between-subject analysis of variance was conducted. The between-subject factor was the location of the candies and the five days in each location was treated as repeated measure within that condition. Covariates for gender, age, and weight were also included in the analyses. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION As noted in Table 1, the visibility and the convenience of the chocolates significantly contributed to how many were consumed. On an average day, subjects with candies on their desk consumed 2.9 more than those who had the container in their desk (8.6 vs. 5.7; F 2,50 =3.7), and 5.6 more than those who had to walk two meters to reach them (8.6 vs. 3.0; F 2,50 =5.7). With this operationalization, convenience contributed more to overeating than did visibility. That is, 4

having to walk two meters to reach the candies led subjects to eat 2.7 fewer chocolates each day than if they were conveniently located an arm s length away in one s desk (3.0 vs. 5.7; F 2,50 =3.2). Table 1. Actual and Estimated Amount of Candy Consumption Location of Candy Actual Number of Candies Consumed Estimated Number of Candies Consumed Difference Between Number of Candies Consumed and Estimate On Desk (Visible & Convenient) In Desk (Non-visible & Convenient) Two Meters from Desk (Visible & Inconvenient) F-Test (F 2.50 ) 8.6 5.7 3.0 6.4* 9.7 4.7 1.1 17.3** -1.1 0.9 1.9 * p < 0.05 ** p < 0.01 What was also interesting was how placement influenced perceptions of consumption. Recall that following the experiment, subjects had been asked to estimate how many candies they ate when the candy was in each of the three locations. * While the actual consumption of candies was 8.6, 5.7, and 3.0 for the on-desk, in-desk, and two-meters-from-desk conditions, the estimated consumption was 9.7, 4.7, and 1.1 respectively (see Figure 1). Consumers overestimated their consumption of candy that was on the desk, visible and convenient (9.7 vs. 8.6 actual). They underestimated the consumption of candy that was in their desk, non-visible and convenient (4.7 vs. 5.7 actual). Last, they more extremely underestimated the consumption of candy that was two meters from the desk in a visible but inconvenient location (1.1 vs. 3.0 actual). * While there are memory concerns when one is retrospectively recalling their consumption of a product, subjects had been randomly assigned to their starting conditions. Therefore any bias in recall would be evenly distributed across each of the conditions. Furthermore, what we are interested in is less how these 5

Figure 1. Differences In Actual and Estimated Candy Consumption 9.0 Estimated Number Eaten 6.0 Number of Candies Actual Number Eaten 3.0 On Desk In Desk Two Meters From Desk These results are interesting because they reflect assumptions we make about how the convenience and visibility of a food influences how much we consume. We appear to believe that if a food was convenient and visible, we probably consumed more of it than a food that was less visible, or one that was less convenient. Because our poor ability at consumption monitoring is one major contributor to overeating, this suggests that we need to compensate for how much we believe we consume by taking a food s visibility and convenience in to account. A food that is inconvenient to consume say a package of cookies in the cupboard versus a package on the counter is also likely to be a food we over-consume relative to what we think. APPLICATIONS We eat more of a food when it is in sight and in reach. conditions vary between each other than we are interested in how their estimated consumption varies with 6

While it must be underscored that a visible and convenient food will also be a food that one will tend to over-consume, a significant deterrent to over-consumption is convenience. If a product is out of sight, it is not always out of mind. However, if it is out of reach, we are less likely to overeat it. If visibility and convenience increase the consumption of chocolate, it may also work for healthy foods. What makes the cookie jar nutritionally dangerous, might bring the fruit bowl back in vogue. We underestimate and over-consume food that is inconvenient or less accessible. That is, we underestimate how many times we have gone to the refrigerator for ice cream or to the cupboard for cookies. This may be one of the factors that most unknowingly influences our over-consumption -- we underestimate our consumption of inconvenient foods. their actual consumption. 7

1 Motlagh, Cyrus K. Health of America optimized in good taste. Journal of Food Products Marketing 1997;4(3):61-82. 2 Pellet, Jennifer. A craving for candy. Discount Merchandiser 1991;31:54. 3 Popkess-Vawter, Sue. Wendel, Shirley. Schmoll, Serena. O Connell, Kathleen. Overeating, reversal theory, and weight cycling. Western Journal of Nursing Research 1998; 20(1):67-83. 4 Ward, Andrew. Mann, Traci. Don t mind if I do: Disinhibited eating under cognitive load. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 2000;78(4):753-763. 5 Faber, Ronald J. Christenson, Gary A. de Zwaan, Martina. Mitchell, James. Two forms of compulsive consumption: Comorbidity of compulsive buying and binge eating. Journal of Consumer Research 1995;22(3):296-304. 6 Terry, Karen. Beck, Steven. Eating style and food storage habits in the home: Assessment of obese and nonobese families. Behavior Modification 1985;9(2):242-261. 7 Wansink, Brian and Rohit Deshpande (1994), Out of Sight, Out of Mind : The Impact of Household Stockpiling on Usage Rates, Marketing Letters, 5:1 (January), 91-100. 8 Hearn, Marsha Davis. Baranowski, Tom. Baranowski, Janice. Doyle, Colleen. Smith, Matthew. Lin, Lillian S. Resnicow, Ken. Environmental influences on dietary behavior among children: availability and accessibility of fruits and vegetables. Journal of Health Education 1998;29(1):26-32. 9 Wansink, Brian, S. Adam Brasel, and Stephen Amjad (2000), The Mystery of the Cabinet Castaway: Why We Buy Products We Never Use, Journal of Family and Consumer Science, Vol. 92:1, 104-108. 10 Chandon, Pierre A Hedonic Salience Framework of Consumption Stockpiling, working paper, University of Pennsylvania. 8