Evidence for Nootropic Effect of BR-16A (Mentat), A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation, in Mice

Similar documents
BR-16A (Mentat), A Herbal Preparation, Improves Learning and Memory Performance in Mice

Prevention of Development of Tolerance and Dependence to Opiate in Mice by BR-16A (Mentat) A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation

Pharmacological Profile of BR-16A (Mentat)

Protective Effect of Mentat (BR-16A) A Herbal Preparation, on Alcohol Abstinence-Induced Anxiety and Convulsions

Effect of a Composite Indian Herbal Preparation, CIHP (III) on Avoidance Learning during Endurance Performance of Rats

7. NOOTROPIC AND ANTIOXIDANT ACTIVITY. 7.1 Introduction

Effect of BR-16A on Alpha-2 Adrenergic, Dopamine Autoreceptor and Dopamine Postsynaptic Receptor Functioning

THE PHARMA INNOVATION - JOURNAL Evaluation of nootropic activity of smrithi: a polyherbal formulation

BR-16A Protects Against ECS-Induced Anterograde Amnesia

Experimental Evaluation of Newer Non-Benzodiazepine (BZD) Hypno-Sedatives Using Different Animal Models

Behavioural Studies on BR-16A (Mentat), A Herbal Psychotropic Formulation

Journal of Global Trends in Pharmaceutical Sciences

EFFECT OF CONVULVULUS PLURICAULIS CHOISY. ON LEARNING BEHAVIOUR AND MEMORY ENHANCEMENT ACTIVITY IN RODENTS

Synergistic Anti-Depression Activity Evaluation of Different Composition of Volatile Oil of Eucalyptus and Neem Oil by Tail Suspension Model

ANXIOLYTIC PROFILE OF A POLYHERBAL DRUG MENTAT

VASILE HEFCO 1*, LUCIAN HRITCU 1, ADRIAN TIRON 1, ANDREEA-IOANA HEFCO 1

Science & Technologies COMPARISON THE EFFECTS OF TACRINE AND GALANTAMINE ON ACTIVE AVOIDANCE TEST IN RATS WITH DIAZEPAM-AMNESIA MODEL

MEMORY ENHANCING EFFECTS OF FICUS CARICA LEAVES IN HEXANE EXTRACT ON INTEROCEPTIVE BEHAVIORAL MODELS.

IMPROVES SCHOLASTIC PERFORMANCE OF CHILDREN REDUCES DAY-TO-DAY WORK STRESS IN ADULTS

International Journal of Pharma and Bio Sciences V1(2)2010 PLUMBAGO ZEYLANICA ROOTS: A POTENTIAL SOURCE FOR IMPROVEMENT OF LEARNING AND MEMORY

"One-trial tolerance" to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state?

Anxiolytic-Like Effects of Ginsenosides on the Elevated Plus-Maze Model in Mice

Nootropic Activity of Saponins obtained from Tinospora Cordifolia Stem in Scopolamine induced Amnesia

Singh et al., IJPSR, 2016; Vol. 7(8): E-ISSN: ; P-ISSN:

International Journal of Pharma and Bio Sciences ROLE OF ANGIOTENSIN ANTAGONISTS IN MEMORY ENHANCEMENT S.INDUMATHY* 1, S.KAVIMANI 2 AND K.V.

ATTENUATION OF STRESS-INDUCED BEHAVIORAL DEFICITS BY AZADIRACHTA INDICA (NEEM): ROLE OF SEROTONIN

Anxiolytic effects of Elaeocarpus sphaericus fruits on the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety in mice

Cholinergic influence on memory stages: A study on scopolamine amnesic mice

AM281, Cannabinoid Antagonist/Inverse agonist, Ameliorates Scopolamine- Induced Cognitive Deficit

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Effect of Withania somnifera Dunal in ethanol-induced anxiolysis and withdrawal anxiety in rats

IJBCP International Journal of Basic & Clinical Pharmacology

Journal of Chemical and Pharmaceutical Research

Rhodiola rosea L- Standardized Extract on Passive

The present investigation aims to investigate the effect of different doses of ethanolic extract of pods of Moringa oleifera

ABELMOSCHUS MOSCHATUS

An extinction trial as a reminder treatment following electroconvulsive shock

Learning and Memory. The Case of H.M.

Anxiolytic-Like Effects of Gastrodia elata and Its Phenolic Constituents in Mice

Assessing mouse behaviors: Modeling pediatric traumatic brain injury

EFFECTS OF CHLOROGENIC ACID ON LEARNING AND MEMORY IN RATS

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Evaluation of Anxiolytic Effect of XXX in Guinea Pig Maternal Separation Test.

Previous maze experience required to increase open arms avoidance in rats submitted to the elevated plus-maze model of anxiety

Herbal Treatments for ECS-Induced Memory Deficits: A Review of Research and a Discussion on Animal Models

PRODUCT STUDY TITLE DATA REQUIREMENT AUTHOR STUDY COMPLETED ON PERFORMING LABORATORY LABORATORY STUDY NUMBER

Running head: EFFECTS OF ALCOHOL ON OBJECT RECOGNITION. Impairing Effects of Alcohol on Object Recognition. A Senior Honors Thesis

Recovery of memory after reminder: Evidence for two forms of retrieval deficit induced by ECS

Encouraging effect of Brahmi Ghrita in amnesia

NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1999 VOL. 21, NO American College of Neuropsychopharmacology

Evaluation of Nootropic Effect of Argyreia speciosa in Mice

Influence of Unani polyherbal formulation on learning and memory retention in mice

A clinically proven. Cognition enhancer

TRAMADOL, A CENTRALLY ACTING OPIOID : ANTICONVULSANT EFFECT AGAINST MAXIMAL ELECTROSHOCK SEIZURE IN MICE

Retrograde Amnesia Gradients by Subconvulsive and High Convulsive Transcranial Currents in Chicks*

Transfer of memory retrieval cues attenuates the context specificity of latent inhibition

A Comparative study of the Anti convulsant effect of Nimodipine and Ketamine combination with standard anticonvulsant drug in Rodents

Nootropic, anxiolytic and CNS-depressant studies on different plant sources of shankhpushpi

A Test of the State-Dependent Hypothesis of Ecs Effects.

12µl) was implanted in the jugular vein (S1) under ketamine (100 mg/kg) /xylacine (1 mg/kg)

PRESENTER: DR. DEEPA JJM MEDICAL COLLEGE DAVANGERE

be a potential therapeutic agent for Alzheimer s type of dementia. These results give the promising effects of the selected drugs and the bioactive

Adolescent Prozac Exposure Enhances Sensitivity to Cocaine in Adulthood INTRODUCTION

Afr. J. Trad. CAM (2006) 3 (1): ZINGIBER OFFICINALE: EVAULATION OF ITS NOOTROPIC EFFECT IN MICE

THE EFFECT OF SYNTHETIC SCOTOPHOBIN ON MOTOR ACTIVITY IN MICE

EFFECTS OF NITRIC OXIDE SYNTHASE INHIBITOR N G -NITRO-L-ARGININE METHYL ESTER ON SPATIAL AND CUED LEANING

EFFECTS OF SOME TRA TQUll.LIZERS 0 LEARNING AND MEMORY TRACES IN RATS *

Learning and memory promoting effects of crude garlic extract

MEMORY ENHANCING ACTIVITY OF POLYHERBAL FORMULATION MEDHYA RASAYANA IN SCOPOLAMINE INDUCED AMNESIA IN RAT

Supplementary Online Content

Abstract. Introduction. R.N. Takahashi 1, O. Berton 2,3, P. Mormède 2 and F. Chaouloff 2

The effect of hyperthermia on memory in rats

Effect of Septilin A Herbal Preparation on Pharmacokinetics of Carbamazepine in Rabbits

Pharmacological study

CO DITIO ED LEARNING I PREGNA CY

Synergistic Muscle Relaxant Activity of Different Composition of Volatile Oil of Eucalyptus and Neem Oil by Rota Rod Model

MODIFICATION OF NOOTROPIC ACTIVITY OF ALBIZZIA LEBBECK SAPONINS BY SEROTONERGIC AGENTS. Suvarna P.Ingale*, Sanjay B, Kasture #

Simple and Conditional Discriminations in Rats: The Effects of Delays and Scopolamine

WHO Expert Committee on Drug Dependence Pre-Review: Extracts and tinctures of cannabis. Expert Peer Review 2

EVALUATION OF ANTIDEPRESSANT ACTIVITY OF ONDANSETRON IN ALBINO MICE

! ! " #$ % & ' # # ( # $ ! " #

Pharmacologyonline 3: (2010)

NNZ-2566 in Rett Syndrome and Autism Spectrum Disorders Role and Update

Circadian rhythm of blood pressure and plasma sodium

Comparative clinical evaluation of herbal formulation with multivitamin formulation for learning and memory enhancement

SHORT COMMUNICATION EFFECT OF SILDENAFIL ON ANXIETY IN THE PLUS-MAZE TEST IN MICE

Learning & memory enhancing activity of Aerial parts of Ervatamia coronaria (L)

1. One form of treatment for people with Alzheimer s disease is to use drugs that act on acetylcholinesterase.

Kappa Opioid Regulation of Stress-Related Behavior

Involvement of GABA in Nootropic and Anxiolytic Activity of Saponins of Albizzia lebbeck Leaves

Scholars Research Library

Infusions of AP5 into the basolateral amygdala impair the formation, but not the expression, of step-down inhibitory avoidance

Effects of Caffeine on Memory in Rats

Contrasting effects of acute and chronic treatments with ketamine on inhibitory avoidance and escape latency in the elevated-t-maze

Multiple memory trace theory Duncan, 1949

A Study on Analgesic activity of Matricaria chamomilla Dr A B Ram Jyothis.

EVALUATION OF SLEEP DEPRIVATION (A NOVEL ALZHEIMER S DISEASE MODEL) BY COMPARATIVE STUDY WITH SCOPOLAMINE AND DIAZEPAM INDUCED AMNESIA IN MICE

Page42 RESEARCH ARTICLE.

Cognitive Function Test. NOR & spatial NOR task. Introduction. Novel Object Recognition (NOR) Estrogen Receptor (ER) in Brain

How to measure rodent behavior and perform a neurological screen.

Transcription:

[Indian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology (1992): 36 (1), 29] Evidence for Nootropic Effect of BR-16A (), A Herbal Psychotropic Preparation, in Mice Kulkarni, S.K. and Anita Verma Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, Punjab University, Chandigarh, India. ABSTRACT BR-16A (, 50-500 mg/kg) improved acquisition and retention of a passive avoidance task in a step-down paradigm in mice. (50-500 mg/kg) reversed scopolamine ( mg/kg) induced disruption of acquisition and retention. (50 and 100 mg/kg) attenuated amnesia produced by the acute treatment with electroconvulsive shock (ECS), immediately after training. Chronic treatment with ECS, for 6 successive days at 24-h intervals, disrupted memory consolidation on the 7th day. Daily administration of (50 and 100 mg/kg) for 6 days significantly improved memory consolidation in mice receiving chronic ECS treatment. (20-500 mg/kg), administered on the 7 th day, also attenuated the disruption of memory consolidation produced by chronic treatment with ECS. On elevated plus-maze, (50 and 100 mg/kg) reversed scopolamine ( mg/kg)-induced delay in transfer latency on the 1st day. The above data suggest a nootropic effect of in naïve and amnesic mice. Keywords: passive avoidance; acquisition; retrieval; scopolamine; consolidation; elevated plus-maze; transfer latency; electroconvulsive shock; BR-16A INTRODUCTION BR-16A (), a herbal psychotropic preparation is claimed to be effective in improving learning ability and behavioural disturbances in mentally retarded children 1-3 and to exert a beneficial effect in cases of cerebral deficit 4, behavioral disturbances following postnatal organic lesions of the central nervous system and in cases having organic loss of bladder function (Personal communication). The powder preparation of contains essentially the following herbal ingredients: Brahmi (Hydrocotyl asiatica), Vach (Acorus calamus), Ashwagandha (Withania somnifera), Guduchi (Tinospora cordifolia), Amla (Emblica officnalis), Shankhpushpi (Evolvulus alsinoides), Jatamansi (Nardostachys jatamansi), Tagar (Valeriana wallichii) and Triphala 5. The commercial preparation, however, has several other components in addition to the above said ingredients, which help in the preparation of the product. Preliminary toxicity studies have shown it to be a safe preparation and no adverse effects were seen on chronic use. The LD 50 value has been reported to be 2400 mg/kg by the oral route of administration (Personal communication). Several existing models for the evaluation of learning and memory are based on positive or negative reinforcement behaviour. However, recently Itoh et al. 6,7, have reported that elevated plus-maze introduced for the measurement of anxiety in rodents 8,9, could be used for

the evaluation of learning and memory in mice, although this method is not based on positive or negative reinforcers. Because of the dislike for the open arms, the animal spends more time in enclosed than open arms, and this aversive quality becomes apparent only when the animal enters them. Itoh et al. 6,7, therefore, speculated that the time in which the animal moves from the open arm to the enclosed arms (transfer latency; TL) might be shortened if the animal had previously experienced entering the open arms. The shortened TL was then related to memory. The present study was undertaken to study the effectiveness of in improving shortterm memory in naïve mice. The reversal effect of against memory deficits induced by scopolamine and acute or chronic treatment with ECS was evaluated on passive avoidance paradigm as well as elevated plus-maze in mice. METHODS Animals: Albino mice of either sex (LAKA strain, bred in Central Animal House facility of Punjab University), weighing 20-25 g were used. The animals were housed under standard light/dark cycle with food and water provided ad libitum. The experiments were performed between 9.00 and 12.00 hr. Drugs: BR-16A () (The Himalaya Drug Co., Bombay) powder was suspended uniformly in deionized water and administered orally. Scopolamine HBr (Merck and Co., Inc., Rahway, NJ, USA) was given i.p. in a constant volume (1 ml/100 g) as aqueous solution. Electroconvulsive shock: Electro convulsive shock (ECS; 10 ma, 0.2 sec) was applied through ear clip electrodes. The animals received either a single shock (acute treatment) or a series of 6 shocks at 24 hr interval (chronic treatment). Apparatus: Passive avoidance step-down paradigm: The method described by Sharma and Kulkarni 10 was used. In brief, the apparatus consisted of an electric grid (24 x 30 cm) with a shock-free zone (SFZ; 2 x 3 x 1 cm) in the centre and the entire grid having a perflex enclosure. Elevated plus-maze: An elevated plus-maze consisting of two open arms (16 x 5 cm) and two enclosed arms (16 x 5 x 12 cm) was used in the present study. The maze was elevated to a height of 25 cm. Procedure Experiment 1: Passive avoidance training was done as follows: The mice were put individually on the electric grid and allowed to explore for 1 min. The stimulus (20v) was then applied and latency to reach SFZ recorded three consecutive times as basal readings. Animals that reached the SFZ in 2 min. in the first trial were selected for the study. After 1 hr of the training, each animal was put on the electric grid again and the latency to reach SFZ

and the number of mistakes (descents) the animal made in 15 min. were recorded as parameters for acquisition and retention respectively. Experiment 2: The procedure and the apparatus were identical to those described in experiment 1. Animals were divided into 10 groups. Thirty minutes after the first trial the mice of 6 of these groups were given a vehicle, (50-500 m/kg) or scopolamine ( mg/kg). The other 4 groups were used for investigating the effect of on amnesia produced by scopolamine. Scopolamine ( mg/kg) was given immediately after passive avoidance training, and 30 min later, (50-500 mg/kg) was administered. Experiment 3: The procedure and the apparatus were identical to those described in experiment 2 except that instead of the number of mistakes (descents) in 15 min, the latency to climb SFZ measured 24 hr after the training served as a parameter for retention. The animals were divided into 4 groups. One group received ECS through the ears and another group was subjected to the same procedure but without ECS (non-ecs: control groups). In the ECS experiment, ECS was applied immediately after the training. Latency to reach SFZ was then recorded 1 hr and 24 hrs after the training. (50 and 100 mg/kg) or vehicle was administered 30 min following the application of ECS. Experiment 4: The procedure and the apparatus were identical to those described in experiment 3, except that the treatment with ECS was repeated every 24 hrs over 6 successive days. 7 groups of mice were treated with ECS immediately after training. ECS treatment was given over 6 successive days at 24 hr intervals. On the seventh day mice received (20-500 mg/kg) or saline and returned to their home cages: 30 min later, latency to reach SFZ was recorded. Two other groups received (50 and 100 mg/kg) 30 minute prior to training on the 1st day. ECS was applied immediately after the training. Latency to reach SFZ was measured 1 hr and 24 hr after the training. On the second day, after the measurement of retention latency, administration of was followed 1 hr later by the application of ECS. On the third and successive days the mice received followed by ECS for a total of 6 days. On the seventh day, 24 hr after the last shock the latency to reach SFZ was measured. Experiment 5: Elevated plus-maze was employed for the measurement of transfer latency (TL). The mice were placed individually at the end of one open arm facing away from the central platform and the time it took to move from open arm facing away from the central platform and the time it took to move from open arm to either of the enclosed arms (TL) was recorded 6. Transfer latency was the time elapsed between the time the animal was placed in the open arm and the time when it fully entered (all the four paws in) the enclosed arm. On the 1st day the mouse was allowed to explore the plus-maze for 20 sec after the measurement of TL. The mice were returned to their home cages after the first trial. Twenty four hours later, the mice were placed on the elevated plus-maze individually as before and TL was recorded again. Transfer latency measured on the 1st and 2nd day served as parameters for acquisition and retrieval respectively. All the drugs were administered 30 min prior to the first trial, either alone or in combination and each treatment group consisted of 6-9 animals.

Statistical analysis: The data was analysed by one-way analysis of variance followed by Dunett's t-test. p<0.05 was considered statistically significant.. RESULTS Experiment 1: Performance of control mice on passive avoidance step-down paradigm: Control (untreated) mice, when placed on the grid, showed training latency of 3.68 ± 5 (n=17) sec. 1 hr later, when again placed on the electric grid, mice reached SFZ in 9.00 ± 2.25 sec and showed 23 ± 4.00 mistakes (descents) in 15 min (Table 1). Table 1: Effect of various doses of on, a) latency to reach SFZ and b) number of mistakes (descents) made in 15 minute in passive avoidance step-down paradigm in mice Treatment mg/kg Latency to reach SFZ in sec (a) Number of mistakes (descents) in 15 minutes (b) Vehicle 9.00 ± 2.25 23.00 ± 4.00 Results are expressed as Mean ± SEM. Control n=10 and n=5-7 in other treatment groups. *p<0.05 as compared with vehicle-treated control. a) ANOVA, F-ratio (28, 110) = 76.00, p<0.05; b) ANOVA, F-ratio (28, 110) = 15.03, p<0.05. 50 9.50 ± 1.5 19.50 ± 2.50 100 10.25 ± 1.75 12.50 ± 0.75 250 8.00 ±3.00 10.00 ± 1.25 500 4.25 ± 1.00* 6.75 ± 1.25* Experiment 2: Effect of on passive avoidance acquisition and retrieval: Latency to reach SFZ, measured 1 hr after training was not significantly affected by (50 and 100 mg/kg). The latency was reduced by the higher doses (250 and 500 mg/kg), the effect being significant at 500 mg/kg. (100-500 mg/kg) produced a significant decrease in the number of mistakes (descents) as compared to the vehicle-treated controls. However, 50 mg/kg dose failed to elicit any significant reduction in the number of mistakes (Table 1). Effect of on passive avoidance performance in scopolamine-treated mice: Scopolamine ( mg/kg) significantly increased the latency to reach SFZ and the number of mistakes as compared to the vehicle-treated controls. (100-500 mg/kg) reversed scopolamine-induced delay in the latency to reach SFZ and reduced the number of mistakes in mice pretreated with scopolamine. The lower dose of (50 mg/kg) failed to produce any significant reversal effect (Table 2).

Table 2: Effect of various doses of on, a) latency to reach SFZ and b) number of mistakes (descents) made in 15 minute in passive avoidance step-down paradigm in scopolamine-treated mice Treatment mg/kg Latency to reach SFZ in sec (a) Number of mistakes (descents) in 15 minutes (b) Vehicle 9.00 ± 2.25 23.00 ± 4.00 Scopolamine 36.30 ± 3.00**** 71.00 ± 9.00**** Scopolamine + Scopolamine + Scopolamine + Scopolamine + 50 100 250 500 40.00 ± 8.12 65.00 ± 8.00 29.00 ± 6.00 52.00 ± 9.00* 21.08 ± 5.03* 38.00 ± 6.01** 17.21 ± 5.11** 25.00 ± 4.01*** All the values are Mean ± SEM. Control n=10 and n=6-10 in different treatment groups. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 and compared with scopolamine-treated group. ****p<0.001 as compared with vehicle-treated control. a) ANOVA, F-ratio (28, 110) = 76.00, p<0.05; b) ANOVA, F-ratio (28, 110) = 15.03, p<0.05. Experiment 3: Effect of on passive avoidance performance in mice receiving acute treatment with ECS: ECS, applied immediately after training, produced a significant increase in latency to reach SFZ, measured 1 hr and 24 hr after training, as compared with the non- ECS control. Pretreatment with (50 and 100 mg/kg) significantly reduced the latency to reach SFZ, measured 1 hr and 24 hr after training as compared with the ECS-treated group (Table 3). Table 3: Effect of pretraining administration of on amnesia produced by acute treatment with ECS (10 ma, 0.2 Sec.) Treatment, mg/kg Latency (sec) measured after 1 hour 24 hour Non-ECS 5.30 ± 0.53 3.13 ± 0.62 ECS 9.13* a ±0.75 9.45* a ± 0.75, 50 10.15 ± 1.24 8.83 ± 1.25, 100 5.70* b ± 0.12 3.90* b ± 0.55 Results are expressed as Mean ± SEM, n=6-7. * a p<0.05 as compared with non-ecs control * b p<0.05 as compared with ECS-treated group, ANOVA, F-ratio (3, 23) = 19, p<0.05. Experiment 4: Effect of on passive avoidance performance in mice receiving chronic treatment with ECS: Chronic treatment with ECS for 6 successive days at 24 hr interval produced a significant prolongation of latency to climb SFZ, measured on the 7 th day after training as compared with non-ecs control.

Mental (20-500 mg/kg), administered on the 7th day, 24 hr after the last treatment with 6 successive shocks, produced significant reversal of ECSinduced delay in latency to reach SFZ as compared with the ECStreated group (Table 4). Daily administration of (50 and 100 mg/kg), 30 min prior to the application of ECS, for 6 successive days, prevented any delay in latency to reach SFZ on the 7th day after training as compared with ECS-treated control. The latency of the non- ECS group on the 7th day was the same as that in the group receiving concurrent treatment with ECS and (100 mg/kg; Table 5). Experiment 5: Effect of on TL in scopolamine-treated mice: The TL on the 2nd day was not significantly different than that on the 1st day in the vehicle-treated control group. Scopolamine ( mg/kg) produced a significant increase in TL on the 1st day but not on the 2nd day as compared to control. Scopolamine-induced increase in TL was, however, reduced/reversed by prior treatment with (50 and 100 mg/kg; Table 6). Table 4: Effect of pre-retention administration of various doses of on latency to reach SFZ, measured on 7 th day after training in mice chronically exposed to ECS (10 ma, 0.2 sec) Treatment mg/kg Latency (sec) to reach SFZ on 7 th day Non-ECS 7.43 ± 0.42 ECS 16.85 ± 1.52** a 20 13.66 ± 2.33 50 10.00 ± 1.04* 100 8.00 ± 0.89** 250 6.50 ± 5** 500 3.00 ± 5*** The data are expressed as Mean ± SEM, n=5-7. ** a p<0.01 as compared with non-ecs control, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 as compared with ECS-treated control. ANOVA, F-ratio (6,35) = 17.41, p<0.05. Table 5: Effect of daily administration of on amnesia produced by chronic treatment with ECS (10 ma, 0.2 sec) over 6 successive days in mice Treatment mg/kg Latency (sec) to reach SFZ on 7 th day Non-ECS 7.43 ± 0.42 ECS 16.85 ± 1.52** a 50 10.66 ± 1.02* 100 6.59 ± 0.68** The data are expressed as Mean ± SEM, n=6-7. ** a p<0.01 as compared with non-ecs control, *p<0.05, **p<0.01 as compared with ECS-treated control. ANOVA, F-ratio (3, 23) = 24.78, p<0.05. Table 6: Effect of on transfer latency (TL) as studied on elevated plus-maze in scopolamine-treated mice Treatment mg/kg n Transfer Latency (TL) in sec (Mean ± SEM) 1st day 2nd day Vehicle 8 36.25 ± 9.50 28.87 ± 3.56 Scopolamine 7 101.43 ± 10.89*** a 39.97 ± 7.04 Scopolamine + Scopolamine + 50 100 8 58.25 ± 7.20* 37.25 ± 4.82 8 29.50 ± 5.64*** 25.50 ± 3.06 *** a p<0.001 as compared with the vehicle-treated control. *p<0.05, ***p<0.001 as compared with scopolamine-treated control. ANOVA, F-ratio (6, 48) = 15.45, p<0.05.

DISCUSSION The present study demonstrates that in a paradigm of short-term memory, produces improvement in passive avoidance acquisition and memory retrieval. The memory improving effect of manifested as decrease in latency to reach SFZ (acquisition) and number of mistakes (descents) the animal made in 15 min (retention) on passive avoidance paradigm. A deficient cholinergic system has been implicated for the progressive decline of learning and memory in various neuropsychiatric disorders 12. Scopolamine-induced amnesia has been used as a pharmacological tool in various clinical and experimental paradigms 13-15. In the present study scopolamine ( mg/kg), an anticholinergic agent, produced deficits in learning as well as memory retention as the animals showed a delay in reaching SFZ and increase in the number of mistakes. (50-500 mg/kg) reversed scopolamine-induced deficits in acquisition and retrieval. In another study in which latency measured 24 hr after the training served as a parameter for memory retention, acute treatment with ECS, immediately after training, produced a significant increase in latency measured 1 hr and 24 hr after the training. This suggests that ECS impairs acquisition as well as retention of a learned passive avoidance task. To substantiate the claim for the improvement in memory retention, the effect of on two different parameters i.e., the number of mistakes (descents) in 15 min and latency to reach SFZ, 24 hr after training was studied. Pretraining administration of (50 and 100 mg/kg) prevented increase in the latency to reach SFZ, measured 1 hr and 24 hr after the application of shock. Since the treatments such as scopolamine or ECS cause retrograde amnesia by interfering with the memory consolidation process 16-18, the above studies suggest an effectiveness of in improving short-term memory in naïve as well as amnesic mice. Additional evidence for the nootropic action of was obtained from the studies on elevated plus-maze. In plus maze, mice show natural aversion to open and high spaces and, therefore, spend more time in enclosed arms. Itoh et al. 6, suggested that TL might be shortened if the animal had previously experienced entering the open arms. The shortened TL could be related to memory. In our study, the shortened TL was obtained on the 2 nd day in controls but the effect was not statistically significant due to large variation in animal behaviour. However, TL was significantly increased in the 1st trial in the experimental amnesic mice, in which amnesia was induced by scopolamine ( mg/kg), injected 30 min before the 1st rial. Transfer latency of the amnesic mice was shortened by the administration of (50 and 100 mg/kg). However, only the latency on the 1st but not 2nd day was affected. This study consolidates that improves acquisition in amnesic mice. The results of this study are well in agreement with those of passive avoidance paradigm. The observation that the TL on the 2nd day in all the treatment groups was not significantly different from that of controls suggests the failure to retain the learned task in the absence of any forceful motivation. Chronic application of ECS for 6 days produced a significant increase in latency to reach SFZ on the 7th day as compared with non-ecs controls. This suggests that application of ECS

disrupts acquisition, retention and consolidation of a learned task Concurrent administration of (50 and 100 mg/kg) and ECS for 6 days prevented any impairment in memory consolidation. Daily administration of for 6 days produced significant attenuation of amnesic effect of chronic application of ECS. The latency measured on the7th day was of the same magnitude as in the non-ecs group. The above results further supplement the effectiveness of in improving cognitive functions in acute as well as chronic amnesic models in mice. The above proposition is strengthened by the observation that single dose administration of (20-500 mg/kg) on the 7th day, 24 hr after the last treatment with ECS, produced a significant reduction in latency to reach SFZ. Thus, the present study suggests that possess nootropic action in naïve as well as amnesic mice. In conclusion, (BR-16A), a herbal preparation with a wide margin of safety is useful in cognitive dysfunctions. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was supported by grants from The Himalaya Drug Co., Bombay. REFERENCES 1. D Souza, B.D. and Chavda, K.B. in hyperactivity an attention deficiency disorders A double blind, placebo-controlled study. Probe (1991): 3, 227 2. Mehta, U.R. Therapy of mentally backward children with or without behaviour disorders. Probe (1991): 3, 233. 3. Sheth, S.C., Tibrewala, N.S., Pal, P.M., Dube, B.L. and Desai, R.G. Management of behavioural disorders and minimal brain dysfunction. Probe (1991): 3, 222 4. Bai, K.I. and Sastry, V.N. Minimal cerebral dysfunction evaluation of a new drug. Probe (1991): 3, 247. 5. Iyengar, M.A. In: Study of crude drugs (Ed. Iyengar M.A.)India: Manipal Power Press, 1981. 6. Itoh, J., Nabeshima, T. and Kameyama, T. Utility of elevated plus-maze for the evaluation of memory in mice; effects of nootropics, scopolamine and electroconvulsive shock. Psychopharmacology (1990): 101. 27. 7. Itoh, J. Nabeshima, T. and Kameyama, T. Utility of elevated plus-maze for disassociation of amnesic and behavioural effects of drugs in mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. (1991): 194, 71 8. Pellow, S., Chopin, P., File, S.E. and Briley, M. Validation of open: closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J. Neurosci. Meth. (1985): 14, 149. 9. Lister, R.G. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology (1987): 92, 180.

10. Sharma, A.C. and Kulkarni, S.K. Evidence for GABA-BZ receptor modulation in shortterm memory passive avoidance takes paradigm in mice. Meth. Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. (1990): 12, 175. 11. Sharma, A.C. and Kulkarni, S.K. MK 801 produces anti-anxiety effect in elevated plusmaze in mice. Drug Dev. Res. (1991): 222, 251. 12. Smith, G. Animal models of Alzheimer s disease. Experimental cholinergic denevation. Brain Res. Rev. (1988): 13, 103. 13. Flood J. and Cherkin, A. Scopolamine effects on memory retention in mice: A model of dementia. Behav. Neural. Biol. (1986); 45, 169. 14. Preston, G.C., Ward, G.E., Brooks, P., Traub, M and Stahl, S.M. Effects of lorazepam on memory, attention and sedation in man: Antagonism by Ro 15-1788. Psychopharmacology (1989): 97, 222 15. Preston, G.C., Ward, G.E., Lines, G.R., Poppleton, P., Haigh, J.R.M. and Traub, M. Scopolamine and benzodiazepine modes of dementia: cross reversal by Ro 15-1788 and physostigmine. Psychopharmacology (1989): 98, 487. 16. Cumin, R., Bandle E.F., Gamzu, E. and Haefely, W.E. Effects of the novel compound aniracetam (Ro 13-5057) upon impaired learning and memory in rodents. Psychopharmacology (1982): 78, 104. 17. Spangler, E.L., Rigby, P. and Ingram, D.K. Scopolamine impairs learning performance in rats in a 14-unit T-maze. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. (1986): 25, 673 18. Wirsching, B.A., Beninger, R.J., Jhamandas, K., Boegman, R.J. and El-Defrawy, S.R. Differential effects of scopolamine on working and reference memory of rats in the radial maze. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. (1984): 20, 659.