Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia

Similar documents
Maximizing Generalization Effects of Semantic Feature Analysis

Changes in Chronic Global Aphasia at Impairment and Functional Communication Levels following SGD Practice and Use. Richard. D. Steele Lingraphica

2.1 Participants 122 stroke patients with aphasia and 25 healthy controls (Table 1). <insert Table 1>

Measuring Changes in Quality of Life in Persons with Aphasia: Is Communication Confidence a Good Measure?

Improvements in Chronic Broca s Aphasia at Impairment and Functional Communication Levels following SGD Practice and Use

TITLE: Acquisition and generalization responses in aphasia treatment: Evidence from sentence-production treatment

A Modified Semantic Feature Analysis Approach With Two Individuals With Chronic Aphasia

Methods. Participants

CLINICAL AND PROGNOSTIC PROPERTIES OF STANDARDIZED AND FUNCTIONAL APHASIA ASSESSMENTS

The effect of word familiarity and treatment approach on word retrieval skills in aphasia

Comparing the outcomes of intensive and non-intensive context-based aphasia treatment

Critical Review: Do mind-body therapies improve language outcomes in persons with aphasia?*

Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key

Using Virtual Clinicians to Promote Functional Communication Skills in Aphasia Method. Participants. Experimental design

References. Embretson, S. E. & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah,

Clinical Aphasiology Submission 2006

Critical Review: The Effectiveness of Constraint-Induced Language Therapy in a Distributive Format

How difficult is it? How well Adults with Aphasia Perceive Task Demands

Impact of Personalization on Acquisition and Generalization of Script Training: A Preliminary Analysis. Abstract

Long-Term Recovery Outcomes in Aphasia

Case Study 1 Aimee Burns, Stephanie Kramer, Courtney Scholl, Diona Ysaac

Evidence Tables and References 5.10 Rehabilitation to Improve Communication Canadian Best Practice Recommendations for Stroke Care Update

Psy /16 Human Communication. By Joseline

Clinical Application of Evidence Based Treatments for Aphasia and Acquired Apraxia of Speech

Investigating the effectiveness of word level therapy in two different approaches

JSLHR. Research Article. Treatment Response to a Double Administration of Constraint-Induced Language Therapy in Chronic Aphasia

fmri scans were compared pre- and post-treatment and perilesional activated volumes on the left hemisphere were compared.

Does Galantamine Facilitate Recovery from Non-Fluent Aphasia After Ischemic Stroke?

Effort Invested in Cognitive Tasks by Adults with Aphasia: A Pilot Study

Management of Wernicke s Aphasia: A Context Based Approach

Combination Treatment in Aphasia: Unilateral Nostril Breathing and Language Therapy

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

Innovative Aphasia Intervention: Optimize Treatment Outcomes through Principles of Neuroplasticity, Caregiver Support, and Telepractice

Critical Review: The Efficacy of Animal-Assisted Therapy for Improving Communication Skills in Adults with Aphasia or Apraxia

Background. Can APT-3 Improve Reading Impairment in Mild Aphasia?

Ischemia in Broca Area Is Associated With Broca Aphasia More Reliably in Acute Than in Chronic Stroke

Introduction Persons with aphasia who are trained to generate abstract words (e.g., justice) in a specific context-category (e.g.

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

Critical Review: Effectiveness of EMA in improving articulatory accuracy in adults with AOS

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Review

Reading comprehension of literal, translational, and high inference level questions in aphasic and right hemisphere damaged adults

INTRODUCTION PURPOSE METHOD

Handheld AAC Program for People with Aphasia. Marjorie Nicholas, PhD Krystal Peralta, MS

observed. In spite of the fact that D.H. demonstrated minimal definitive changes on the conversational measures selected, by the end of both

Aphasia And Associated Disorders: Taxonomy, Localization And Recovery By Andrew Kertesz

Short-term memory refers to the ability to temporarily

shows syntax in his language. has a large neocortex, which explains his language abilities. shows remarkable cognitive abilities. all of the above.

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Toddler Module

MANUAL FOR THE LIDCOMBE PROGRAM OF EARLY STUTTERING INTERVENTION 2008 CONTENTS

Disclosures. Background. Equipment 2/4/2015

The Agency for Health Care Policy and Research Post-

When neurotypical children look at peoples faces, regions in the limbic system light up with endorphins and reward that child.

ABSTRACT A FUNCTIONAL GESTURAL COMMUNICATION INTERVENTION FOR INDIVIDUALS WITH CHRONIC AND SEVERE APHASIA. by Skylar Kay Powlen

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Conversational script performance in adults with non-fluent aphasia: Treatment intensity and aphasia severity

! Introduction:! ! Prosodic abilities!! Prosody and Autism! !! Developmental profile of prosodic abilities for Portuguese speakers!

Psychology Formative Assessment #2 Answer Key

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

When right is all that is left: plasticity of right-hemisphere tracts in a young aphasic patient

The inner workings of working memory: Preliminary data from unimpaired populations

Fostering Communication Skills in Preschool Children with Pivotal Response Training

Evaluation of Verbal Behavior in Older Adults

What is Autism? Katherine Lamb, Ph.D., CCC/SLP GSHA2018 1

Speech Modifications for Supporting Auditory Comprehension in Aphasia

EXPLORATORY ANALYSIS OF SPEECH FEATURES RELATED TO DEPRESSION IN ADULTS WITH APHASIA

Psychology of Language

Effective Interventions for Students with ASD: Practical Applications for Classroom Success. Seminar Two

Effective Interventions for Students with ASD: Practical Applications for Classroom Success. Seminar Two. Objectives

Neuroplasticity, Dosage, and Repetition Priming Effects in Individuals with Stroke-Induced Aphasia

REFERENCES Blank, M., & Franklin, E. (1980). Dialogue with preschoolers: A cognitively-based system of assessment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1,

Word-Retrieval Treatment for Aphasia: Facilitation of Generalization

NEUROGENIC COMMUNICATIVE DISORDERS KAREN RIEDEL, PH.D. CCC-SLP CLINICAL ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR RUSK REHABILITATION SCHOOL OF MEDICINE, NYULMC

The Communicative Effectiveness Index: Psychometric properties of a Danish adaptation

ADES QUICK SCORE SCHOOL VERION PROFILE SAMPLE

Julie Bolton M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

POSTER PRESENTATIONS FROM THE 2002 NATIONAL

Adapting Patient Provider. with Communication Disorders

The Relation between Language, Nonverbal Cognition, and Quality of Life in People with Aphasia

Bringing Your A Game: Strategies to Support Students with Autism Communication Strategies. Ann N. Garfinkle, PhD Benjamin Chu, Doctoral Candidate

Global aphasia without hemiparesis: language profiles and lesion distribution

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

LANGUAGE IN INDIA Strength for Today and Bright Hope for Tomorrow Volume 8 : 2 February 2008

Critical Review: Speech Perception and Production in Children with Cochlear Implants in Oral and Total Communication Approaches

Acentral theme in categorization research has been

Critical Review: Amanda Davidson M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate Western University: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

INTRODUCTION METHODS

Severity of Post-stroke Aphasia According to Aphasia Type and Lesion Location in Koreans

How Does the ProxTalker Speech- Generating Device Compare to PECS?

Learning Objectives.

DEMENTIA Dementia is NOT a normal part of aging Symptoms of dementia can be caused by different diseases Some symptoms of dementia may include:

Conversational Scaffolding with Couples with an Aphasic Partner

Positive and Negative Symptoms of Psychosis The Care Transitions Network

Early Educational Placement and Later Language Outcomes for Children With Cochlear Implants

Higher Cortical Function

Contextual bias and inferencing in adults with right hemisphere brain damage It is widely accepted that damage to the right cerebral hemisphere (RHD)

Transcription:

Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia Response Elaboration Training (RET; Kearns, 1985) is a treatment approach designed to increase the content and length of verbal productions in aphasia (Gaddie, Kearns, & Yedor, 1991; Kearns, 1985, 1997; Kearns & Scher, 1989; Kearns & Yedor, 1991; Yedor, Conlon, & Kearns, l993). RET has received a significant amount of study, particularly with nonfluent speakers (Gaddie, et al., l991; Kearns, l985; Kearns & Scher, l989; Kearns & Yedor, l991; Yedor, et al., l993). Although promising results have been reported with fluent aphasia (Kearns & Scher, 1989; Yedor, et al., l993), additional study is warranted with such speakers. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of RET with two persons with fluent aphasia. More specifically, RET was applied to picture descriptions and to personal recounts and its effects were evaluated in terms of production of correct information units (CIUs). CIU production (number of CIUs and efficiency) was measured in response to trained and untrained pictures, and in unrehearsed personal narratives. Of additional interest were changes in functional communication as measured by the Communication Activities of Daily Living-Second Edition (CADL-2; Holland, Fratalli, & Fromm, l999) and the Communication Effectiveness Index (CETI; Lomas, Pickard, Bester, Elbard, Finlayson, & Zoghaib, l989). Method Participants Two stroke survivors with fluent aphasia (see Tables 1 & 2) served as the participants for this study. One male (Participant 1) who was 42 MPO and one female (Participant 2) who was 12 MPO were enrolled. They both were college educated and native speakers of English. Each had a negative medical history for mental illness and neurological problems other than CVA. In addition, they both passed pure-tone air conduction screenings. Participant 1 and 2 s aphasia classifications were transcortical sensory aphasia and Wernicke s aphasia, respectively (WAB; Kertesz, 1982). Experimental Design A single subject, multiple baseline design across behaviors and subjects was implemented to evaluate the acquisition effects of treatment as well as response and stimulus generalization effects. The following contexts comprised the multiple baseline behaviors: 1) description of a set of 1 pictures, 2) description of another set of 1 pictures, and 3) production of language in a personal recount. An additional context that was used to measure generalization effects, but did not receive treatment consisted of a description of a third set of 1 pictures. In order to balance for effect of treatment order, the condition under treatment was quasi-randomly assigned for each participant (note: this report is a portion of a larger investigation involving additional participants). For the first participant, treatment was applied to the personal recount context. Following the personal recount condition treatment was applied to one set of pictures and subsequently to the second set of

pictures. The treatment order for the second participant was as follows: first set of pictures, second set of pictures, and then personal recount condition. Experimental Stimuli The experimental stimuli consisted of three sets of 1 black and white line drawings, each depicting a single, different action. The sets were matched on the basis of the actions for familiarity, argument structure, homophonous noun root, and image agreement, as well as for average number of CIUs elicited from non-brain-damaged speakers. Baseline and Probe Procedures Prior to the application of treatment, 8 baseline probes were conducted for Participant 1 and 1 baseline probes were completed for Participant 2 to attempt to ensure stability of responding. These probes consisted of elicitation of descriptions of the three sets of 1 pictures and the personal recount. The personal recount involved asking the participant to talk for 5 minutes about a topic of his/her choice. The order of presentation for the conditions was randomly assigned for each baseline. Each set of 1 pictures was randomized prior to presentation. The schedule for probing during the treatment phase varied according to the condition being treated. Probes were conducted following every two treatment sessions for the picture set or the personal recount condition that was currently receiving treatment. Probes were conducted every three to four treatment sessions for all conditions not currently under treatment. At the end of each treatment phase all conditions were probed. Follow up probes were conducted at 3 and 6 weeks. Dependent Measures The dependent measures were number of CIUs produced and efficiency of CIU production. CIUs were calculated according to procedures described by Nicholas & Brookshire (1993). The number of words produced for each picture description was first calculated. All words that were correct in context, informative and relevant (i.e., CIU) were then calculated for each picture. Communicative efficiency was calculated as the percentage of CIUs per word. Treatment Procedures Treatment was conducted 3 times per week. When RET was applied to one of the picture sets, the pictures were submitted to the treatment hierarchy (see Appendix) one at a time in random order. A treatment session consisted of two trials with each picture. When RET was applied to the personal recount, the examiner asked the participant to provide a topic for elaboration. The treatment hierarchy was applied as with the pictures. A treatment session consisted of 12-2 topic elaborations. Treatment was applied to each condition until 2 treatment sessions were completed. Reliability All probes were independently scored by two separate examiners and results were compared. All scoring disagreements between the examiners were resolved by consensus.

Results The number of CIUs produced and percentage of CIUs per words (efficiency) are represented in Figure 1 and 2 for Participant 1 and in Figures 3 and 4 for Participant 2. The data in these figures reflects performance on probes. The conservative dual criteria (CDC) method (Fisher, Kelley, & Lomas, 23) was used to aid in analysis of probe data. In this method, two criterion lines (i.e., regression line & mean line) are raised by.25 standard deviations to create a conservative criteria for inspection and interpretation of results. A prespecified number of data points (e.g., 8 out of 1) must fall above both criterion lines to conclude a positive treatment effect. For Participant 1, the number of CIUs produced did not reach CDC criteria in the three treatment conditions. However, the production of CIUs during the final treatment condition approached criteria with 7 of the 1 treatment probes above both lines. A positive treatment effect was found for communicative efficiency for Set 1 pictures for Participant 1. The other two treatment conditions did not reach criteria. Probe performance for Participant 2 also did not reach CDC criteria for number of CIUs. However, a positive treatment effect was observed for efficiency of CIU production for Set 1 pictures. Significant treatment effects were not seen for either of the remaining two conditions for communicative efficiency. Generalization to untrained picture sets was not observed for either participant (Figures 1-4). Post testing results with the CETI revealed modest gains for both participants. Increases in CADL-2 performance were also evident for Participant 1. Discussion Discussion will compare the results of this study to results obtained in previous studies with fluent aphasic speakers with respect to qualitative changes, expectation for generalization and outcome measures. Direction for future research will be addressed.

References Brown, L., Sherbenou, R.J., & Johnsen, S.K. (1997). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (3 rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Fisher, W., Kelley, M., & Lomas, J. (23). Visual aids and structured criteria for improving visual inspection and interpretation of single-case designs. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 387-46. Gaddie, A., Kearns, K.P., & Yedor, K. (1991). A qualitative analysis of response elaboration training effects. Clinical Aphasiology, 19, 171-183. Holland, A., Fratalli, C., & Fromm, D. (1999). Communication Activities of Daily Living (2 nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, Inc. Kearns, K.P. (1985). Response elaboration training for patient initiated utterances. In R.H. Brookshire (Ed.) Clinical Aphasiology (pp. 196-24). Minneapolis, MN: BRK. Kearns, K.P. (1997). Broca s aphasia. In L.L. LaPointe (Ed.) Aphasia and related neurogenic language disorders (2 nd ed., pp. 1-41). New York: Thieme. Kearns, K.P., & Scher, G.P. (1989). The generalization of response elaboration training effects. Clinical Aphasiology, 18, 223-245. Kearns, K.P., & Yedor, K. (1991). An alternating treatments comparison of loose training and a convergent treatment strategy. Clinical Aphasiology, 2, 223-238. Kertesz, A. (1982). The Western Aphasia Battery. New York: Grune & Stratton. Lomas, J., Pickard, L., Bester, S., Elbard, H., Finlayson, A., & Zoghaib, C. (1989). The communication effectiveness index: Development and psychometric evaluation of a functional measure for adult aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 113-124. Nicholas, L.E., & Brookshire, R.H. (1993). A system for quantifying the informativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 338-35. Porch, B. (21). Porch Index of Communicative Ability: Vol. 2. Administration, Scoring, and Interpretation (4 th ed.). Albuquerque, NM: PICA Programs. Robey, R., Schultz, M., Crawford, A., & Sinner, C. (1999). Single-subject clinical-outcome research: designs, data, effect sizes, and analyses. Aphasiology, 13, 445-473. Saffran, E.M., Berndt, R.S., & Schwartz, M.F. (1989). The quantitative analysis of agrammatic production: Procedure and data. Brain and Language, 37, 44-479. Yedor, K.E., Conlon, C.P., & Kearns, K.P. (1993). Measurements predictive of generalization of response elaboration training. Clinical Aphasiology, 21, 213-223. Yorkston, K.M., & Beukelman, D.R. (1981). Assessment of Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech. Austin TX: Pro-Ed.

Table 1. Participant 1. Pre and Post Treatment Assessment Porch Index of Communicative Ability 4 th Ed. (PICA; Porch, 21) Modality Pre Post Verbal %ile 44 53 Auditory %ile 29 4 Overall %ile 36 47 Western Aphasia Battery (WAB; Kertesz, 1982) Subtest Pre Post Fluency 6 8 Spontaneous Speech 13 15 Comprehension 6.5 7.5 Repetition 8.2 1 Naming 4.3 6.4 Aphasia Quotient 64 76.9 Aphasia type Transcortical Sensory Anomic Communication Activities of Daily Living-2 nd Ed. (CADL-2; Holland, Frattali, & Fromm, 1999) Pre Post Percentile 25 th 32 nd The Communicative Effectiveness Index (CETI; Lomas et al., 1989) Pre Post Mean 36 56 Test of Nonverbal Intelligence 3 rd Ed. (TONI-3; Brown et al., 1997) Nonverbal Intelligence Score 61%ile Assessment of Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech (AIDS; Yorkston & Beukelman, 1981) Word level 74%

Table 2. Participant 2. Pre and Post Treatment Assessment Porch Index of Communicative Ability 4 th Ed. (PICA; Porch, 21) Modality Pre Post Verbal %ile 25 33 Auditory %ile 25 17 Overall %ile 33 35 Western Aphasia Battery (WAB; Kertesz, 1982) Subtest Pre Post Fluency 8 8 Spontaneous Speech 1 11 Comprehension 4 3.65 Repetition 1 1.2 Naming 1.3 1.3 Aphasia Quotient 32.6 34.3 Aphasia type Wernicke s Wernicke s Communication Activities of Daily Living-2 nd Ed. (CADL-2; Holland, Frattali, & Fromm, 1999) Pre Post Percentile 2 th 7 th The Communicative Effectiveness Index (CETI; Lomas et al., 1989) Pre Post Mean 51.25 68.19 Test of Nonverbal Intelligence 3 rd Ed. (TONI-3; Brown et al., 1997) Nonverbal Intelligence Score 9%ile Assessment of Intelligibility of Dysarthric Speech (AIDS; Yorkston & Beukelman, 1981) Word level 95.6%

Personal Recount Condition Picture Description Set 1 Picture Description Set 2 Figure 1. Participant 1 - Number of Correct Information Units 16 14 12 1 8 6 4 2 4 3 2 1 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 4 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 3 2 1 Baseline Treatment Adjusted mean line Adjusted regression line Maintenance Follow-Up 3 & 6 wks 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 4 Untreated Picture Description Set 3 2 1 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Sessions

Figure 2. Particpant 1 - Percentage of CIUs vs. Words Personal Recount Condition Picture Description Set 1 1 8 6 4 2 1 8 6 4 2 Baseline Treatment Maintenance Follow-Up 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Picture Description Set 2 1 8 6 4 2 Adjusted mean line Adjusted regression line 1 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Untreated Picture Description Set 8 6 4 2 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Sessions

Picture Description Set 1 Picture Description Set 2 Personal Recount Condition Figure 3. Participant 2 - Number of Correct Information Units 5 4 3 2 1 5 4 3 2 1 4 3 2 Baseline Treatment 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Adjusted mean line Adjusted regression line Maintenance 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 1 5 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Follow-Up 3 & 6 wks Untreated Picture Description Set 4 3 2 1 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Sessions

Picture Description Set 1 Picture Description Set 2 Personal Recount Condition Figure 4. Participant 2 - Percentage of CIUs vs. Words 1 8 6 4 2 1 8 6 4 2 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 1 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Adjusted mean line 8 Adjusted regression line 6 4 2 Baseline Treatment Maintenance 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 1 Follow-Up 3 & 6 wks Untreated Picture Description Set 8 6 4 2 5 1 15 2 25 3 35 4 45 5 55 6 Sessions

Appendix Modified Response Elaboration Training Picture Level Application (after Wambaugh & Martinez, 2) 1. The clinician presents a picture and elicits a response (e.g., Tell me about this picture., What does this remind you of?, Tell me what s happening. ) A. If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 2. B. If there is no or an inappropriate response, the clinician models two response options (e.g., You could say something like noun phrase [NP] or verb phrase [VP]. ) If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 2. C. If there is no or an inappropriate response, the clinician models a one-word response and requests a repetition (e.g., Say noun. or Say verb. ) If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 2. D. If there is no or an inappropriate response, the clinician uses integral stimulation, with a maximum of 4 attempts, to elicit the noun or verb production. If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 2. In the event of an inappropriate or no response, the next item is presented. 2. The clinician models and reinforces the participant s production from Step 1 (e.g., Shoe. Great. That s a shoe. ) 3. The clinician requests an elaboration of the response from Step #1 (e.g., What s happening with the shoe? ) A. If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 4. B. If there is no or an inappropriate response, the clinician models two response options (e.g., You could say something like noun phrase [NP] or verb phrase [VP]. ) and requests a response. If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 4. C. If there is no or an inappropriate response, the clinician models a one-word response and requests a repetition (e.g., Say noun. or Say verb. ) If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 4. D. If there is no or an inappropriate response, the clinician uses integral stimulation, with a maximum of 4 attempts, to elicit the noun or verb production. If the response is appropriate, the clinician moves to Step 4. In the event of an inappropriate or no response, the next item is presented. 4. The clinician reinforces the production from Step 3 and models a phrase/sentence that combines the participant s productions from Steps 1 and 3 (e.g., Right, tie. Tie shoe. ) 5. The clinician models the combined production again and requests a repetition. A. If the response is appropriate, the clinician requests 3 more productions, using integral stimulation as necessary. The clinician moves to Step 6. B. If there is an inappropriate or no response, the clinician attempts to elicit 4 productions of the target, using integral stimulation. The clinician moves to Step 6. 6. The clinician waits for at least 5 seconds and requests that the participant again describe the picture. A. If the entire elaborated response is produced, the clinician reinforces the production and moves to the next item (omission of functors was not considered incorrect).

B. If a partial elaborated response is produced; the clinician reinforces the production, models the entire elaboration, and requests a production with integral stimulation. The clinician then moves to the next item. C. If no response, the clinician reinforces the production, models the entire elaboration, and requests a production with integral stimulation. The clinician then moves to the next item. D. If an alternate appropriate response is produced, the clinician reinforces the production and moves to the next item.