Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Similar documents
Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) select Arabic numerals or visual quantities corresponding to a number of sequentially completed maze trials

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Scalar effects in the visual discrimination of numerosity by pigeons

NUMEROSITY DISCRIMINATION IN PRESCHOOL CHILDREN ALZIRA ALMEIDA, JOANA ARANTES, AND ARMANDO MACHADO

How might the discrepancy in the effects of perceptual variables on numerosity judgment be reconciled?

Multiple spatially-overlapping sets can be enumerated in parallel

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

Chapter 3. Information Input and Processing Part II* Prepared by: Ahmed M. El-Sherbeeny, PhD *(Adapted from Slides by: Dr.

The Role of Reference Points in Ordinal Numerical Comparisons by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta)

XVI. SENSORY AIDS RESEARCH

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Supporting Information

Animal Arithmetic. Non-humans Represent Numbers

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans Jessica F. Cantlon and Elizabeth M. Brannon

STIMULUS FUNCTIONS IN TOKEN-REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULES CHRISTOPHER E. BULLOCK

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Pigeons and the Magical Number Seven

The association of color memory and the enumeration of multiple spatially overlapping sets

IAT 355 Perception 1. Or What You See is Maybe Not What You Were Supposed to Get

Mental operations on number symbols by-children*

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Contrast and the justification of effort

HOW DOES PERCEPTUAL LOAD DIFFER FROM SENSORY CONSTRAINS? TOWARD A UNIFIED THEORY OF GENERAL TASK DIFFICULTY

B.A. II Psychology - Paper A. Form Perception. Dr. Neelam Rathee. Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh

Behavioural Processes

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2003 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

Sensation and Perception

Time. Time. When the smaller reward is near, it may appear to have a higher value than the larger, more delayed reward. But from a greater distance,

Exemplar Memory and Discrimination

Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Counting in a Computerized Testing Paradigm

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Fall 2001 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer

Examining the Constant Difference Effect in a Concurrent Chains Procedure

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

= add definition here. Definition Slide

How Does NUMBER SENSE Begin?

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli

Overview. Non-associative learning. Associative Learning Classical conditioning Instrumental/operant conditioning. Observational learning

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

T he integration of cues from different modalities or from different sources of information is a topic of growing

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Review #6 ( )

GENERALIZATION GRADIENTS AS INDICANTS OF LEARNING AND RETENTION OF A RECOGNITION TASK 1

Chapter 2 Norms and Basic Statistics for Testing MULTIPLE CHOICE

Unconscious numerical priming despite interocular suppression

Three methods for measuring perception. Magnitude estimation. Steven s power law. P = k S n

Humans and nonhuman animals discriminate numbers in a

on both components of conc Fl Fl schedules, c and a were again less than 1.0. FI schedule when these were arranged concurrently.

Numerical cognition is the idea that a living being has a number sense similar to a

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast?

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Response to Comment on Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures

..." , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1

Numerical Cognition in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta)

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

Chapter 4. Activity of human hippocampal and amygdala neurons during retrieval of declarative memories

Sensation and Perception

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

Medical Statistics 1. Basic Concepts Farhad Pishgar. Defining the data. Alive after 6 months?

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

Instrumental Conditioning I

Chronometric Studies of Numerical Cognition in Five-month-old Infants. Justin N. Wood & Elizabeth S. Spelke Harvard University

Supplementary Figure 1. Recording sites.

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Behavioral training.

Discriminability of differences in line slope and in line arrangement as a function of mask delay*

Steven A. Cholewiak ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Pigeons memory for time: Assessment of the role of subjective shortening in the duration-comparison procedure

Content Scope & Sequence

The Interplay between Feature-Saliency and Feedback Information in Visual Category Learning Tasks

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

Topics in Animal Cognition. Oliver W. Layton

Transcription:

Entire Set of Printable Figures For Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity Emmerton Figure 1. Figure 2. Examples of novel transfer stimuli in an experiment reported in Emmerton & Delius (1993). Paired numerosities varied from 1 vs. 2 to 7 vs. 8 on intermixed test trials. L and R indicate the left or right pecking keys on which the arrays appeared.

Figure 3. Results of testing 8 pigeons with novel arrays. Dashed line: scores were significantly above chance (p =.05). (Adapted from Emmerton & Delius, 1993). Footnote 1. Davis & Pérusse s Classification of Numerical Competence Davis and Pérusse (1988) distinguish several types of numerical competence and suggest that different cognitive or perceptual processes underlie each type. They divide numerical competence into the categories of relative numerousness judgments, subitizing, estimation, and counting. They assume that relative numerousness judgments involve the simplest decision processes since no knowledge of absolute number is required. Instead an animal judges numerical inequalities that are ordered in magnitude (e.g. "more" vs. "less" decisions). In this chapter, the term "numerosity discrimination" is used instead of relative numerousness judgments. Subitizing is a form of pattern recognition that is used to rapidly assess small quantities of items. A label is then assigned to the perceived array. For instance, a glimpse of several dots arranged at the angles of an imaginary triangle leads to a judgment about the specific quantity. For us, that quantity would be labeled "3" because we know that triangles have three corners. For an animal, that quantity might simply be labeled "correct amount peck it!" without the animal necessarily knowing how this quantity relates to other numerical values. Other researchers doubt there is a separate subitizing process however (see section VI on Models and Mechanisms for Numerical Processing). Estimation refers to the ability to assign a numerical label to an array of large numbers of items without having to count or enumerate each one. When we judge at a glance that there are about 50 ducks on a lake we are "estimating". Animals judgments of large number arrays have not been systematically studied yet. Counting is the ability to discriminate the absolute number in a set by a process of enumeration. This involves tagging each item in a set, and applying a series of ordered labels as these items are counted off. To count the number of peanuts in a packet of mixed nuts, for instance, we might put each peanut to one side, at the same time labelling them "1", "2", "3", etc. The numerical label we apply to the last peanut we find is the absolute or cardinal number of peanuts in the packet. The criteria for counting behavior have been defined by Gelman and Gallistel (1978) (see section IV on Cardinal Numbers and Counting). Davis and Pérusse regard

counting as a more complex process than the ones involved in relative numerousness judgments or subitizing (and presumably estimation too). They also discuss the concept or sense of number as an attribute of counting. This term implies an ability to transfer numerical discriminations across sensory modalities (e.g. 5 sound pulses are equivalent to 5 light flashes) or across modes of presentation (e.g. 4 red squares shown simultaneously on a computer screen are equivalent to 4 red squares presented one after the other). Figure 4. Animation can't be printed. Figure 5. Test choices with novel arrays after pigeons learned to discriminate "many" from "few" dots. The summed areas of the dots, and thus the array brightnesses, were equated across numerosities. Dot sizes were the same or variable within an array. (Adapted from Emmerton et al., 1997.)

Figure 6. Sample of arrays used in various tests of serial ordering. Numerosities ranged from 1 to 7 items. In the experiment, the arrays were shown in reversed contrast. The results in Fig. 5 are based on arrays equated for area.

Figure 7. Sample of arrays at high ( near ) and low ( far ) densities. When the numerosity was 1 (not shown), the single dot varied in size and location. In the experiment, the arrays were white dots on a dark background.

Footnote 2. Counting Criteria Gelman and Gallistel (1978) list three criteria that formally define the process of counting. They are: 1. The one-to-one principle Each item in a set (or event in a sequence) is given a unique tag, code or label so that there is a one-to-one correspondence between items and tags. No item or tag may be omitted, nor conversely used more than once. In pacemaker-accumulator models of counting (see section VI), the code or tag is equivalent to the current value of pulses in the accumulator or in working memory. In the neuronal filtering model, the tag corresponds to the particular numerosity detector that has been activated. 2. The stable-order principle The tags or labels must always be applied in the same order (e.g. 1, 2, 3, 4 and not 3, 2, 1, 4). This principle underlies the idea of ordinality that the label "3" stands for a numerosity greater than the quantity called "2" and less than the amount called "4". In counting models (see section VI) this ordering of tags is achieved by the progressive accumulation of pulses, or the increasing thresholds that must be crossed for different numerosity detectors to be activated. 3. The cardinal principle The label that is applied to the final item represents the absolute quantity of the set. Two other principles generally apply but are not fundamental to the process of counting. These are: 4. The abstraction principle Any types of items may be counted (e.g. birds in a flock or windows in a building). 5. The order-irrelevance principle The order in which the items themselves are tagged is irrelevant. (If we want to count the number of flowers in a bunch, it does not matter which one we label "1", which we label "2", etc.) Video can't be printed.

Footnote 3. Left Side - Different symbols denote specific numbers of pecking responses. Right Side - When a symbol appears on the symbol key, the bird has to peck it the required number of times (e.g. 2 pecks to N), then peck once to the enter key. A food reward is given for a correct response sequence, and timeout for an incorrect one. Animation can't be printed. Figure 8. Mean performance (bar height) for pigeons trained to emit a specific number of responses to 6 different visual symbols. Solid portion of bar: chance level of correct responding. (Data from Xia, Siemann, & Delius, 2000.)

Animation can't be printed. Figure 9. Pigeons discrimination of the proportion of red/green color in pairs of horizontal bars. Choice of the bar containing the greater proportion of positive color was rewarded. The positive color was red or else green for different birds. pc S+ = proportion of positive color in correct bar. pc S- = proportion of positive color in incorrect bar. Animation can't be printed. Figure 10. Pigeons discrimination of color proportion with arrays of red and green rectangles.

Animation can't be printed. Figure 11. Test of discrimination based on disparity ratio in red/green horizontal bars. Color proportions in paired stimuli were complementary in rewarded training trials (S+/S-) but not in nonreinforced test trials (S+/0.5 and 0.5/S-). Discrimination performance was related to relative rather than absolute differences in proportion. Figure 12.

Figure 13. Figure 14. An animal judges the numerical magnitude of various stimulus groups and assigns a code or tag (represented by the color of each curve) for each magnitude. On average, it accurately assigns a specific tag for each amount, as shown by the peak probability for each "n" judgement. But the probability of assigning a tag varies: as numerical magnitude increases, so does the variance of the judgements. Where curves overlap, the animal may tag the number of items incorrectly. Note: the figure is not drawn to scale.

Figure 15. Transformed mean data from pigeons early training with arrays of "more" or "less" dots. Performance level was significantly related to relative numerosity differences for the group trained to choose "more", but not for birds that had to choose "less". n 1, n 2 are the number of dots in paired stimulus arrays. Figure 16. Initial acquisition of "more" vs. "less". One group of pigeons was trained to choose the arrays with the greater number of dots. The other group, shown the same stimuli, was trained to choose the arrays with fewer dots. Mean scores are shown.

Figure 17. Pigeons trained to choose "more" or "less" numerous arrays performed differently (p <.01) on non-reinforced test trials with novel stimuli, but not on reinforced training trials with familiar stimuli.