Cognitive Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling Rational Analysis

Similar documents
Cognitive Modeling. Lecture 9: Intro to Probabilistic Modeling: Rational Analysis. Sharon Goldwater

Utility Maximization and Bounds on Human Information Processing

Exploring the Influence of Particle Filter Parameters on Order Effects in Causal Learning

Memory by Temporal Sampling

Cognitive Modeling. Lecture 12: Bayesian Inference. Sharon Goldwater. School of Informatics University of Edinburgh

Wason's Cards: What is Wrong?

Effects of generative and discriminative learning on use of category variability

Can Bayesian models have normative pull on human reasoners?

Racing for the City: The Recognition Heuristic and Compensatory Alternatives

Categorisation, Bounded Rationality and Bayesian Inference

Group Assignment #1: Concept Explication. For each concept, ask and answer the questions before your literature search.

On The Scope of Mechanistic Explanation in Cognitive Sciences

Recommender Systems for Literature Selection: A Competition between Decision Making and Memory Models

Protocol analysis and Verbal Reports on Thinking

Chapter 11: Behaviorism: After the Founding

The Scientific Method

Negations in syllogistic reasoning: Evidence for a heuristic analytic conflict

Key Ideas. Explain how science is different from other forms of human endeavor. Identify the steps that make up scientific methods.

The role of sampling assumptions in generalization with multiple categories

Are Humans Rational? SymSys 100 April 14, 2011

An Efficient Method for the Minimum Description Length Evaluation of Deterministic Cognitive Models

Bill Wilson. Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology

The Logic of Categorization

An Account of Interference in Associative Memory: Learning the Fan Effect

The Value of Cognition

Hypothesis-Driven Research

How does Bayesian reverse-engineering work?

LEARNING. Learning. Type of Learning Experiences Related Factors

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

Chapter 1 Introduction to Educational Research

Principles of Sociology

Involuntary and Voluntary Memory Sequencing Phenomena

Dear Participants in the Brunswik Society

Although most previous studies on categorization

Problem representations and illusions in reasoning

Neural Cognitive Modelling: A Biologically Constrained Spiking Neuron Model of the Tower of Hanoi Task

DEVELOPING THE RESEARCH FRAMEWORK Dr. Noly M. Mascariñas

Streak Biases in Decision Making: Data and a Memory Model

Psych 136S Review Questions, Summer 2015

Psy 803: Higher Order Cognitive Processes Fall 2012 Lecture Tu 1:50-4:40 PM, Room: G346 Giltner

Neural Cognitive Modelling: A Biologically Constrained Spiking Neuron Model of the Tower of Hanoi Task

Introduction to the special issue

Does pure water boil, when it s heated to 100 C? : The Associative Strength of Disabling Conditions in Conditional Reasoning

Individual Differences in Attention During Category Learning

Chapter Two. Theory. Theories we ll look at. Theories of Development

The spacing and lag effect in free recall

The Current State of Our Education

Nature of Science and Scientific Method Guided Notes

The use of the Take The Best Heuristic under different conditions, modeled with ACT-R

Choice adaptation to increasing and decreasing event probabilities

Who Is Rational? Studies of Individual Differences in Reasoning

CARISMA-LMS Workshop on Statistics for Risk Analysis

Learning to Use Episodic Memory

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations

The Influence of Perceptual Difficulty on Family Resemblance Sorting

Supplementary notes for lecture 8: Computational modeling of cognitive development

Top-Down Control of Visual Attention: A Rational Account

Absolute Identification Is Relative: A Reply to Brown, Marley, and Lacouture (2007)

Remarks on Bayesian Control Charts

The shallow processing of logical negation

Samantha Sample 01 Feb 2013 EXPERT STANDARD REPORT ABILITY ADAPT-G ADAPTIVE GENERAL REASONING TEST. Psychometrics Ltd.

Cognitive Modeling. Problem-solving. History. Lecture 4: Models of Problem Solving. Sharon Goldwater. January 21, 2010

Learning a Hierarchical Organization of Categories

Current Computational Models in Cognition Supplementary table for: Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2011). Computational

Technical Specifications

Cognitive Modeling. Lecture 4: Models of Problem Solving. Sharon Goldwater. School of Informatics University of Edinburgh

D 7 A 4. Reasoning Task A

Negations Without Not : Alternative forms of Negation and Contrast Classes in Conditional Inference. James Richard Hazlett Vance

Knowledge Based Systems

The Discovery/Justification Distinction

Mechanical rationality, decision making and emotions

Survey Errors and Survey Costs

Distributed Cognition, Representation, and Affordance

Mind & Body Behaviourism

HOW TO IDENTIFY A RESEARCH QUESTION? How to Extract a Question from a Topic that Interests You?

Assignment 2: Experimental Design

Forgetting Curves Emerge from Dynamics of Integrated Memory

An Account of Associative Learning in Memory Recall

MTAT Bayesian Networks. Introductory Lecture. Sven Laur University of Tartu

Generic Priors Yield Competition Between Independently-Occurring Preventive Causes

What is AI? The science of making machines that:

A Power-Law Model of Psychological Memory Strength in Short- and Long-Term Recognition

A Computational Model of Counterfactual Thinking: The Temporal Order Effect

BEHAVIOR CHANGE THEORY

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

Evaluating the Causal Role of Unobserved Variables

INVESTIGATING FIT WITH THE RASCH MODEL. Benjamin Wright and Ronald Mead (1979?) Most disturbances in the measurement process can be considered a form

2014 Philosophy. National 5. Finalised Marking Instructions

Classicalism and Cognitive Architecture

Mechanisms of long-term repetition priming and skill refinement: A probabilistic pathway model

L6: Overview. with side orders of lecture revision, pokemon, and silly experiments. Dani Navarro

Learning What Others Like: Preference Learning as a Mixed Multinomial Logit Model

Sage Publications, Inc. and American Sociological Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Sociometry.

Confirmation Bias in Software Development and Testing: An Analysis of the Effects of Company Size, Experience and Reasoning Skills

INDIAN HILL EXEMPTED VILLAGE SCHOOL DISTRICT Social Studies Curriculum - May 2009 Psychology Semester Course

Reduction. Marie I. Kaiser, University of Cologne, Department of Philosophy, Germany,

RECALL OF PAIRED-ASSOCIATES AS A FUNCTION OF OVERT AND COVERT REHEARSAL PROCEDURES TECHNICAL REPORT NO. 114 PSYCHOLOGY SERIES

2 Critical thinking guidelines

Artificial Intelligence Lecture 7

Transcription:

Lecture 9: Intro to Probabilistic Modeling: School of Informatics University of Edinburgh sgwater@inf.ed.ac.uk February 8, 2010 1 1 2 3 4 Reading: Anderson (2002). 2 Traditional mechanistic approach to cognitive modeling (Chater and Oaksford 1999): analyze cognitive phenomena (memory, reasoning, language) regarding their causal structure; stipulate architectures and algorithms; develop either symbolic or connectionist computational models; experimental and neuroscientific data provide constraints on these models. Problems with the mechanistic approach: cognitive systems are seen as an assortment of arbitrary mechanisms; they are subject to arbitrary constraints; the purpose or goal structure of the cognitive systems is left unexplained; the fact that cognitive systems are well adapted to the task they are solving and the environment they operate in is left unexplained. 3 4

Alternative: approach to cognitive modeling: provide purposive explanations: analyze cognitive system as to its goal and function; specify the task a cognitive system solves and the nature of its environment; assume the system is optimally adapted to task and environment; derive an optimal (rational) solution to the task, subject to constraints (resource limitations); historically, this approach is related to probability theory; Bayesian mathematics often used to formulate models. Methodology (Anderson 1990, 2002): 1 Goals: specify precisely the goals of the cognitive system. 2 Environment: develop a formal model of the environment to which the systems is adapted. 3 Computational Limitations: make minimal assumptions about the computational limitations. 4 Optimization: derive the optimal behavior function, given (1) (3). 5 Data: examine the empirical evidence to see whether the predictions of the behavior function are confirmed. 6 Iteration: repeat (1) (5); iterative refinement. 5 6 Memory Retrieval of Memory Retrieval Items in memory decay gradually over time: traditional explanation (modal model) in terms of the architecture of the memory system (short term vs. long term store); alternative explanation: recent items are more likely to be needed again soon; the memory system is optimally adapted to this decline in need probability over time. Example: if you read a fact about Iraq one sentence ago, then it s likely that you ll need this fact for understanding the next sentence. 1 Goals: efficient retrieval of items in memory; specifically: availability of an item should match the probability that it will be needed. 2 Environment: need-probability p for an item is determined by the environment; items with high p should be most available. 3 Computational Limitations: items are searched sequentially, with a fixed cost C with searching each item. 4 Optimization: stop retrieving items when pg < C, where G is the gain associated with retrieving an item; p depends on current context and item s history of use. 7 8

of Memory Retrieval Background: Power Law of Forgetting Number of items recalled decreases exponentially with time. 5 Data: need to account for two basic facts: power law of forgetting: memory items decay exponentially over time: predicts need-probability decays as a power function; power law of practice: reaction time decreases exponentially with no. of trials: predicts need-prob. increases as a power function of frequency of use. 6 Iteration: experiments that test the model: investigate the role of context: recurrence of items in newspaper headlines; manipulate need-probability experimentally; measure change in forgetting curves. 9 10 Background: Power Law of Practice Reaction time (latency) for a given task decreases exponentially with number of practice trials. Anderson (1990) proposes that the need-probability p of an item A depends on its history of use HA and the set of contextual cues Q that are present: p = P(A HA, Q) Assuming that the cues are independent of the history given A, p P(A HA)P(Q A) P(A HA): probability that A will be needed given its usage history; P(Q A): probability of observing the cues when A is needed (strength of association between A and Q). 11 12

History factor Context factor Anderson s (1990) model of history is based on earlier model of library borrowings (Burrell 1980). Model predicts that P(A HA ) Holding history constant, need-probability is proportional to P(Q A). decreases as a power function of time t since last use: P(Q A) is a product of separate cue strengths P(qi A). P(A HA ) t k Strength of cue i depends on direct association with A and association with items similar to A. increases as a power function of number of previous uses n. Model predicts various effects, including is maximized when t is equal to the interval between previous two uses. Memories are more accessible in the presence of related elements (priming). all of which match subjects memory behavior. More subtle effects of prime frequency, number of related elements, etc. Schooler (1998) shows that these properties also hold for items in newspaper headlines. 13 Predictions 14 Relationship betw. need probability p and retention interval t: Controversy about power laws: can arise as an artifact of averaging over subjects. But, evidence that power laws of forgetting and practice also hold for individual subjects. Experimental evidence for both context and history factors; Some effects (e.g. primacy) are not predicted by the model. Need to take into account underlying mechanism (capacity of short-term memory). Attempts to integrate cognitive architectures with rational explanations (ACT-R). Filled dots: strong cue associations; open dots: weak cue associations. (Chater and Oaksford 1999) 15 16

Categorization Categorization Features associated with categories: Training stimuli: (Lea and Wills 2008) (Lea and Wills 2008) 17 18 The purpose of categories of Categorization Anderson (1990) argues that psychologists often confuse categorization with labeling. In the real world, purpose of categories is prediction: objects in the same category behave similarly or have similar properties. The label assigned to an object is simply another feature of that object. Subjects predictions may be based on a categorization that is different from the labeling used in an experiment. 1 Goals: Predict features of a new object. 2 Environment: Disjoint partitioning of objects (species), independent variation of features within categories. 3 Computational limitations: Items are categorized sequentially. 4 Optimization: Probability that nth object has value j for feature i: P(ij x)p(x Fn) x x: a partition, Fn: features of the n objects. 19 20

of Categorization Model of Categorization Under sequential categorization, we assume that categories of previous objects are fixed. Then 5 Data: Many experimental phenomena, including effects of similarity to central tendency of category (prototype effect); similarity to specific instances in category (exemplar effect); category size; feature correlations within categories; number of non-matching features (exponential function). P(ij) = k P(ij k)p(k Fn) P(ij k): probability of nth object taking on jth value for feature i, given that it belongs to category k. Depends on feature values for other objects in k. P(k Fn): probability that nth object belongs to category k, given features observed for all objects. Depends on relative sizes of categories and feature values observed for different categories. 21 22 : Rational or irrational? Model assumes categories are defined by items with similar features; category labels are simply features. Correctly predicts many experimental phenomena, including both prototype and exemplar effects, by learning multiple categories for a single label when appropriate. Assumes objects fall into disjoint categories; less true for non-species categories (artifacts, etc.). Ongoing work examining non-optimal categorization due to sequential constraints. Many experiments conclude that people are irrational. Decision-making: subjects don t integrate information about probability of events (base rate neglect). Deductive reasoning: subjects don t follow rules of logic (Wason selection task). But: behavior is often far more optimal when probabilities are experienced or rules are framed in real-world scenarios. Experiments often assume information is certain; real world is uncertain. 23 24

Adaptive rationality Summary Rational analysis assumes organisms are adapted to real world environments. Behavior is optimized over a range of situations, and given certain costs. Behavior may be non-optimal in specific situations (experiments). Example: Choice of local optimum over global optimum for reinforcement. Irrational behavior may be the result of unnatural or unusual situations. 25 Traditional modeling approaches treat the cognitive system as a collection of arbitrary mechanisms, with arbitrary performance limitations; they don t explain why these mechanisms cope with a complex and changing environment; rational analysis provides such explanations: analyze the task that a cognitive system solves, and its adaptation to the environment; optimal behavior functions explain why cognitive mechanisms are the way they are; provide constraints on possible theories and predict new data; successfully applied to memory, categorization, and other tasks. 26 References Anderson, John R. 1990. The Adaptive Character of Thought. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. Anderson, John R. 2002. Is human cognition adaptive? In Thad A. Polk and Colleen M. Seifert, editors,, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pages 1193 1228. Burrell, Q. 1980. A simple stochastic model for library loans. Journal of Documentation 36:115 32. Chater, Nicholas and Mike Oaksford. 1999. Ten years of the rational analysis of cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(2):57 65. Lea, S. and A. Wills. 2008. Use of multiple dimensions in learned discriminations. Comparative Cognition and Behavior Reviews 3:115 133. Schooler, L. 1998. Sorting out core memory processes. In Nicholas Chater and Mike Oaksford, editors, Rational Models of Cognition, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pages 128 155. 27