Requirements for LTP Induction by Pairing in Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Cells

Similar documents
SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Supplementary Figure 1

Part 11: Mechanisms of Learning

Synaptic plasticityhippocampus. Neur 8790 Topics in Neuroscience: Neuroplasticity. Outline. Synaptic plasticity hypothesis

previously shown (10), however, this manipulation by itself does not reliably result in the development of a large

Supporting Online Material for

A form of long-lasting, learning-related synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus induced by heterosynaptic low-frequency pairing

Memory Systems II How Stored: Engram and LTP. Reading: BCP Chapter 25

Supplementary Figure 1. Basic properties of compound EPSPs at

DOMINIQUE DEBANNE*, BEAT H. GÄHWILER, AND SCOTT M. THOMPSON MATERIALS AND METHODS

Supplementary Information

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Cellular Neurobiology / BIPN 140

BIPN 140 Problem Set 6

Sample Lab Report 1 from 1. Measuring and Manipulating Passive Membrane Properties

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Synaptic Transmission: Ionic and Metabotropic

Ube3a is required for experience-dependent maturation of the neocortex

Is activation of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor gated channels sufficient to induce long term potentiation?

Problem Set 3 - Answers. -70mV TBOA

Supporting Online Material for

Decreased Frequency But Not Amplitude of Quantal Synaptic Responses Associated with Expression of Corticostriatal Long-Term Depression

Neuroscience 201A (2016) - Problems in Synaptic Physiology

Differential Effect of TEA on Long-Term Synaptic Modification in Hippocampal CA1 and Dentate Gyrus in vitro

Synaptic Plasticity and the NMDA Receptor

GABA B Receptor-Mediated Presynaptic Inhibition Has History-Dependent Effects on Synaptic Transmission during Physiologically Relevant Spike Trains

Chapter 6 subtitles postsynaptic integration

Dendritic Mechanisms of Phase Precession in Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Neurons

Induction of Hebbian and Non-Hebbian Mossy Fiber Long-Term Potentiation by Distinct Patterns of High-Frequency Stimulation

Synaptic Integration

STRUCTURAL ELEMENTS OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM

5-Nervous system II: Physiology of Neurons

Is action potential threshold lowest in the axon?

Na + K + pump. The beauty of the Na + K + pump. Cotransport. The setup Cotransport the result. Found along the plasma membrane of all cells.

Neurons. Pyramidal neurons in mouse cerebral cortex expressing green fluorescent protein. The red staining indicates GABAergic interneurons.

Short-term facilitation evoked during brief afferent tetani is not altered by long-term potentiation in the guinea-pig hippocampal CA1 region

Topical Review. Dendritic potassium channels in hippocampal pyramidal neurons

Action potential. Definition: an all-or-none change in voltage that propagates itself down the axon

Cellular mechanisms of information transfer: neuronal and synaptic plasticity

Introduction to Neurobiology

Prolonged Synaptic Integration in Perirhinal Cortical Neurons

Pathway-Specific Properties of AMPA and NMDA-Mediated Transmission in CA1 Hippocampal Pyramidal Cells

How Synapses Integrate Information and Change

Chapter 3 subtitles Action potentials

A Model of Spike-Timing Dependent Plasticity: One or Two Coincidence Detectors?

Neurons of the Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis (BNST)

Supporting Information

Synaptic Plasticity and Memory

Novel expression mechanism for synaptic potentiation: Alignment of presynaptic release site and postsynaptic receptor

LESSON 3.3 WORKBOOK. Why does applying pressure relieve pain?

Chapter 4 Neuronal Physiology

Pairing subthreshold excitatory postsynaptic potentials

Hippocampal synapses are known to be highly plastic. Their

Temporal asymmetry in spike timing-dependent synaptic plasticity

LESSON 3.3 WORKBOOK. Why does applying pressure relieve pain? Workbook. Postsynaptic potentials

Linking Neuronal Ensembles by Associative Synaptic Plasticity

The action potential travels down both branches because each branch is a typical axon with voltage dependent Na + and K+ channels.

Synaptic plasticity. Activity-dependent changes in synaptic strength. Changes in innervation patterns. New synapses or deterioration of synapses.

Neurobiology: The nerve cell. Principle and task To use a nerve function model to study the following aspects of a nerve cell:

ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY OF NEURONS. AP Biology Chapter 48

Arnaud Ruiz, Emilie Campanac, Ricardo Scott, Dmitri A. Rusakov, Dimitri M. Kullmann

How Synapses Integrate Information and Change

Learning Rules for Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity Depend on Dendritic Synapse Location

Lecture 22: A little Neurobiology

Bursting dynamics in the brain. Jaeseung Jeong, Department of Biosystems, KAIST

Chapter 11 Introduction to the Nervous System and Nervous Tissue Chapter Outline

Chapter 45: Synapses Transmission of Nerve Impulses Between Neurons. Chad Smurthwaite & Jordan Shellmire

Intro. Comp. NeuroSci. Ch. 9 October 4, The threshold and channel memory

Electrophysiology. General Neurophysiology. Action Potentials

Dendritic Signal Integration

Bidirectional NMDA receptor plasticity controls CA3 output and heterosynaptic metaplasticity

BIONB/BME/ECE 4910 Neuronal Simulation Assignments 1, Spring 2013

The Nervous System. Dr. ZHANG Xiong Dept. of Physiology ZJU School of Medicine.

Bidirectional modifications in synaptic efficacy, exemplified

9.01 Introduction to Neuroscience Fall 2007

浙江大学医学院基础医学整合课程 各论 III. The Nervous System. Dr. ZHANG Xiong Dept. of Physiology ZJU School of Medicine

Postsynaptic Complex Spike Bursting Enables the Induction of LTP by Theta Frequency Synaptic Stimulation

Effects of adrenaline on nerve terminals in the superior cervical ganglion of the rabbit

CHAPTER 44: Neurons and Nervous Systems

MCB MIDTERM EXAM #1 MONDAY MARCH 3, 2008 ANSWER KEY

The control of spiking by synaptic input in striatal and pallidal neurons

Supralinear increase of recurrent inhibition during sparse activity in the somatosensory cortex

BIPN140 Lecture 12: Synaptic Plasticity (II)

Predictive Features of Persistent Activity Emergence in Regular Spiking and Intrinsic Bursting Model Neurons

PSY 215 Lecture 3 (1/19/2011) (Synapses & Neurotransmitters) Dr. Achtman PSY 215

Nervous System. Master controlling and communicating system of the body. Secrete chemicals called neurotransmitters

Cell communication. Gated ion channels. Allow specific ions to pass only when gates are open

Cell communication. Gated ion channels. Voltage-Gated Na + Channel. Allow specific ions to pass only when gates are open

Chapter 3 Neurotransmitter release

MOLECULAR AND CELLULAR NEUROSCIENCE

Brief anoxic episodes induce long-lasting changes in synaptic properties of rat CA3 hippocampal neurons

Portions from Chapter 6 CHAPTER 7. The Nervous System: Neurons and Synapses. Chapter 7 Outline. and Supporting Cells

Electrical Properties of Neurons. Steven McLoon Department of Neuroscience University of Minnesota

Chapter 11: Nervous System and Nervous Tissue

Chapter 5 subtitles GABAergic synaptic transmission

The Nervous System. Nervous System Functions 1. gather sensory input 2. integration- process and interpret sensory input 3. cause motor output

Inhibitory and Excitatory Spike-Timing-Dependent Plasticity in the Auditory Cortex

Ivy/Neurogliaform Interneurons Coordinate Activity in the Neurogenic Niche

CELLULAR NEUROPHYSIOLOGY

Astrocyte signaling controls spike timing-dependent depression at neocortical synapses

Transcription:

Requirements for LTP Induction by Pairing in Hippocampal CA1 Pyramidal Cells HUAN-XIN CHEN, NIKOLAI OTMAKHOV, AND JOHN LISMAN Volen Center for Complex Systems, Biology Department, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts 02454 Chen, Huan-Xin, Nikolai Otmakhov, and John Lisman. Requirements for LTP induction by pairing in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. J. Neurophysiol. 82: 526 532, 1999. The induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampal CA1 region requires both presynaptic activity and large postsynaptic depolarization. A standard protocol for inducing LTP using whole-cell recording is to pair low-frequency synaptic stimulation (100 200 pulses, 1 2 Hz) with a depolarizing voltage-clamp pulse (1 3 min duration). In this standard protocol, a Cs -based internal solution is used to improve the fidelity of the depolarization produced by voltage-clamp. In an attempt to induce LTP more rapidly, we tried to induce LTP by pairing highfrequency stimulation (200 pulses, 20 100 Hz) with a short depolarization ( 15 s). Surprisingly, we found that this protocol failed to induce LTP, even though large LTP ( 300% of baseline) could be induced by a subsequent standard protocol in the same cell. Pairing brief high-frequency stimulation at the beginning of a long depolarization (3 min) also did not induce LTP. However, the same highfrequency stimulation at the end of the long depolarization did induce LTP. When similar experiments were done with a K -based internal solution, pairing high-frequency stimulation with a short depolarization did induce LTP. This indicates that the requirement for long depolarization is related to the use of Cs. We speculate that, when recording is made with Cs, a tetanus given at the beginning of depolarization initiates a process that inhibits N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) dependent LTP. This inhibitory process itself decays away during prolonged depolarization. INTRODUCTION Long-term potentiation (LTP) of the Schaffer collateral synapses in the CA1 region of the hippocampus is the primary model system for the study of the associative synaptic modification thought to underlie learning and memory (Bliss and Collingridge 1993). The form of LTP at these synapses has a Hebbian property: synapses are strengthened if there is both presynaptic activity and substantial postsynaptic depolarization (Brown et al. 1990). The requirement for postsynaptic depolarization is due to the properties of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) channel. To open, the Mg 2 block of these channels must be relieved by depolarization (Mayer et al. 1984; Nowak et al. 1984). Once NMDA channels open, they allow the influx of Ca 2 that triggers synaptic strengthening (Bliss and Collingridge 1993). A commonly used protocol for inducing LTP is to give tetanic stimulation in which a large number of axonal inputs is stimulated at high frequency (100 Hz) for 1 s. This synaptic input produces the postsynaptic depolarization required to open the NMDA conductance, but the quantitative The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked advertisement in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact. properties and mechanism of this depolarization are not yet established. What is clear is that many factors are likely to be involved, including temporal and spatial summation of excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs and IPSPs, respectively), the triggering of back-propagating Na spikes (Magee and Johnston 1997), the triggering of bursts (Thomas et al. 1998), and the function of a variety of other voltagedependent conductances (Magee 1998; Magee et al. 1998). Because of the complexity of the postsynaptic processes that affect depolarization during tetanus-induced LTP, many investigators have sought to evoke LTP in a simpler and more defined way by using what is termed a pairing protocol. This protocol circumvents the complexities of synaptically induced postsynaptic depolarization by simply imposing depolarization by current injection through the microelectrode (Gustafsson et al. 1987) or by voltage clamp, using the somatic patch electrode (Malinow 1991). Typically Cs is used as the major internal cation because it blocks K channels and makes it possible to achieve a larger and more uniform dendritic depolarization. Because postsynaptic depolarization is produced by the clamp rather than by synaptic stimulation, only a few input fibers need to be stimulated (Kullmann and Nicoll 1992; Malinow 1991). This improves accuracy with which synaptic currents can be quantified, because small currents reduce voltage-clamp errors. A further aspect of standard pairing protocols is that a much lower frequency of synaptic stimulation (0.1 2 Hz) is used (Colino et al. 1992; Malinow and Tsien 1990; Manabe et al. 1992; Perkel and Nicoll 1993) than during tetanic stimulation (100 Hz). This produces an additional simplification by avoiding the transient forms of presynaptic plasticity that occur during high-frequency stimulation. A consequence of using low-frequency stimulation is that the depolarization during pairing must be long. Typically, this depolarization is applied for more than one minute (Colino et al. 1992; Manabe et al. 1992; Otmakhov et al. 1997). The experiments reported began with the attempt to devise a much briefer pairing protocol that more closely resembles the duration of depolarization during tetantically induced LTP. Based on what is known about LTP, we reasoned that it should be possible to induce LTP by tetanic stimulation during a brief depolarizing voltage-clamp pulse. We found, however, that it is not possible to do so. Our results further show that this surprising inability to induce LTP is related to the use of Cs as the major internal cation. METHODS Transverse hippocampal slices were prepared from male Long- Evans rats (14 19 days old) as described previously (Otmakhov et al. 526 0022-3077/99 $5.00 Copyright 1999 The American Physiological Society

REQUIREMENTS FOR PAIRING-INDUCED LTP 527 1997). The CA3 region of each slice was removed from the slice by a surgical cut. Slices were incubated on cell culture inserts (Falcon, 8 m pore diameter) covered by a thin layer of artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF containing 2 mm Ca 2 and6mmmg 2 ) and surrounded by a humidified 95% O 2-5% CO 2 atmosphere at room temperature ( 22 C). For recording, a single slice, after at least 2 h incubation, was transferred to a submerged recording chamber with continuous flow (1.5 2 ml/min) of ACSF. The ACSF contained (in mm) 124 NaCl, 26 NaHCO 3, 1.25 NaH 2 PO 4, 2.5 KCl, 4 CaCl 2, 4 MgSO 4,10 D-glucose, and 0.05 picrotoxin, gassed with 95% O 2-5% CO 2 giving ph 7.4. All experiments were carried out at room temperature. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings were performed from CA1 pyramidal cells located 30 90 m beneath the slice surface under visual control using infrared dark-field illumination and a chargecoupled device (CCD) TV camera. The patch electrodes were made from hard borosilicate glass and filled with (in mm) 120 Cs-gluconate, 10 CsCl, 10 HEPES, 8 NaCl, 0.2 EGTA, 2 MgATP, 0.3 Na 3 GTP, and 10 phosphocreatine (ph 7.3 with CsOH, osmolarity 290 300 mosm), or 125 K-gluconate, 10 HEPES, 8 NaCl, 0.2 EGTA, 2 MgATP, 0.3 Na 3 GTP, and 10 phosphocreatine (ph 7.3 with KOH, osmolarity 290 296 mosm). The electrodes had resistance 3 5 M when filled with internal solution. Whole-cell recording was made in voltage-clamp mode using an Axopatch-1D (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA) and cells were voltage-clamped at 68 mv. To evoke synaptic current, two glass electrodes filled with ACSF (300 K ) were placed in the dendrite region 70 and 150 m away from the cell body layer to stimulate two separate groups of Schaffer collaterals. Stimuli (100 s) were delivered alternatively to each input pathway through current output isolation units. The interval between stimuli in each pathway was 6switha3sinterval between pathways. Stimulation intensity was adjusted to produce an excitatory postsynaptic current (EPSC) with an amplitude of 100 pa at the beginning of recording. Series and input resistances during the recording were monitored every 3 s by measuring the peak and steady-state currents in response to 2 mv, 38 ms depolarizing pulses. The series resistance ranged from 6 to 12 M. The input resistance ranged from 300 to 100 M. In all pairing protocols used for inducing LTP, a total of 200 stimuli were delivered to the test pathway during the depolarization. During all induction protocols, stimulation of the control pathway was stopped altogether. The specific LTP protocols were 1) four brief high-frequency tetani (50 pulses at 50 or 20 Hz per each; 4 s intervals) paired with a short depolarization ( 15 s to 0 mv); 2) four brief high-frequency tetani (50 pulse of 50 or 20 Hz per each; 4 s intervals) paired with a long depolarization ( 3 min to 0 mv) given either at the beginning or at the end of the long depolarization; 3) the standard pairing protocol: low-frequency stimulation (200 pulses, 1.4 Hz) paired with a long depolarization ( 3 min to 0 mv). Data were acquired using a 486 PC computer, Labmaster DMA ADC and program written in Axobasic. The amplitude of a synaptic response was calculated as the difference between the average of data points in a window before the stimulus and in a window around the peak of the synaptic response. The average of responses during a 5 min period before LTP induction was taken as the baseline, and all values were normalized to this baseline. The level of LTP was calculated from this normalized data as the average (over a 3 min period) at the times after LTP induction indicated in each case. Values were expressed as means SE. Two-tail paired and unpaired t-test were used for calculation of the statistical significance of differences. Drugs used included D-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (D-AP5, Research Biochemicals International) and picrotoxin (Sigma). RESULTS Whole-cell recordings were made under visual control from CA1 pyramidal cells using a Cs -based internal solution (see METHODS), and test stimuli were given every 6 s to two independent pathways. In the form of the standard pairing procedure for LTP induction used in our laboratory, 200 synaptic stimuli are given at 1.4 Hz during a 3 min depolarization to 0 mv. We sought to test whether the same number of stimuli given at high-frequency during a brief depolarization could also induce LTP. In these experiments we first established a stable baseline for synaptic responses. The pairing protocol was then given as follows: the holding voltage was increased from 68to0mVovera3speriod. Immediately after that a series of four high-frequency tetani (50 stimuli at 50 Hz) were given at 4 s intervals to one of the pathways. The holding voltage was then rapidly restored to 68 mv. The total period of depolarization was thus around 15 s. Figure 1, A C, shows that this brief pairing with high-frequency stimulation induced only a small, transient potentiation. In most cases, this transient potentiation was seen in both the stimulated (test) and unstimulated (control) pathways (123 9.3% of the baseline in the test and 118 6% in the control input, control 10 min after the pairing, n 6; Fig. 1C). Ten minutes later, a standard pairing protocol was used (200 stimuli at 1.4 Hz, depolarization to 0mVfor 3 min). This standard pairing produced a very large potentiation (307 36%, 30 min after induction, n 6), and the potentiation was specific to the stimulated pathway. The efficacy of the standard pairing stimulus cannot be attributed to the greater delay after the onset of whole-cell recording because the standard protocol also induces large LTP if given after shorter delays (data not shown). The failure of the highfrequency stimulation to induce LTP was surprising. We explored several minor variants that used higher or lower frequencies (100 Hz, n 2; 20 Hz, n 8), and they also failed to induce strong LTP. Rather, they induced only weak potentiation that was evident in both test and control pathways. Ten minutes after 100 Hz tetani given during short depolarization, the level of potentiation was 116 11% in the test and 122 5% in control input. Ten minutes after 20 Hz tetani given during short depolarization, the potentiation was 138 10% in the test and 119 8% in the control input. The standard pairing protocol given later in the same experiments produced robust synapse specific LTP (300 7% in the test and 116 8% in the control input, n 8, 30 min after induction). There are two major differences between the brief protocol and the standard protocol: the length of the depolarization and the frequency of synaptic stimulation. We next sought to determine which of these differences is important. In the experiments illustrated in Fig. 2, pairing was done with the long depolarization ( 3 min) used in the standard pairing protocol, but synaptic stimulation was done using brief periods of highfrequency (50 Hz) stimulation. Two variants of this experiment were done, one in which the high-frequency stimulation was given at the beginning of the long depolarization, the other in which the same high-frequency stimulation was given at the end of the long depolarization. When stimulation was given at the beginning of the depolarization (Fig. 2A), there was virtually no potentiation (101.5 15.7%, at 20 min after pairing, n 5). However, when stimulation was given at the end of the depolarization (Fig. 2B), large input-specific LTP was induced (243 22% in the test and 101 11% in the control at 20 min after induction of LTP, n 7). In a separate set of experiments when 20 Hz tetani were given at the end of the long depolarization, strong and synapse-specific LTP was also induced (197 6% in the test and 102 5% in the control at 20 min

528 H.-X. CHEN, N. OTMAKHOV, AND J. LISMAN after induction of LTP, n 7). These results indicate that LTP can be induced by a high-frequency stimulation during pairing, but that stimulation must be preceded by a long depolarization. The LTP that can be induced by the standard low-frequency pairing protocol can be blocked by 2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (APV), a blocker of NMDA channels (n 4, data not shown). To check whether the LTP produced by high frequency given at the end of a long depolarization was also dependent on the NMDA channels, we repeated these experiments in the presence of APV (50 M). Figure 3A shows that this type of LTP was dependent on NMDA channels. We also tested whether the small, FIG. 1. High-frequency synaptic stimulation paired with a brief depolarization did not induce long-term potentiation (LTP), whereas a subsequent standard protocol induced robust LTP. A: individual experiment in which highfrequency stimulation (4 trains with 4 s interval, each contains 50 pulse, 50 Hz) was given to the test pathway ( ) paired with 15 s depolarization to 0 mv (at arrow). E, control pathway not stimulated during the depolarization. Subsequently (at bar) a standard pairing protocol (200 pulses, 1.4 Hz, holding potential, 0 mv) was applied to the test pathway. Inset: diagram representing the 4 short high-frequency trains. B: averages of excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs; 20 successive responses) in the test ( ) and control (E) pathway taken at the times marked by letters in A. C: summary data (n 6). FIG. 2. High-frequency stimulation given at the end, but not the beginning of a long depolarization, could induce LTP. A: summary data (n 7) when high-frequency stimulation was given at the beginning of a long depolarization (3 min). and E, test pathway and control pathway, respectively. B: summary data (n 5) when high-frequency stimulation given at the end of the long depolarization. Insets in A and B are the diagrams of the protocols. transient potentiation produced by pairing high-frequency stimulation with a brief depolarization was NMDA dependent. Figure 3B shows that it was not sensitive to APV (50 M; 126 6%, 10 min after tetanus). This level of potentiation was not significantly different from the level of potentiation produced in control ACSF (P 0.05). It should also be noted that the NMDA-independent potentiation was not input specific (123 9% potentiation on the control input 10 min after tetanus). It is of interest to know whether the properties of the synaptically evoked current during pairing give any hint about why LTP occurs under some conditions and not others. Figure 4A shows the currents during each of four tetani that were given early (left column) or late (right column) during a long depolarization. The records were from the same cell and the same input. Figure 4B shows an example from another cell, but where different inputs were used for early and late responses. These currents have the slow kinetics characteristic of the NMDA current and are almost completely blocked by APV (Fig. 4C). It can be seen that the current is larger when the tetani are given early than when they are given late during the depolarization. Similar results were obtained in all cells examined. One curious feature of these records that might be sig-

REQUIREMENTS FOR PAIRING-INDUCED LTP 529 nificant is that the tetanus given early sometimes evoked currents with seemingly supralinear summation on the rising edge, followed by a rapid collapse of the current. These features were not observed when the tetanus was given at the end of the depolarization. Possible interpretations of these complex kinetics are given in the DISCUSSION. An additional feature of these experiments is that the total outward membrane current falls gradually during a long depolarization (Fig. 4D). In a final series of experiments, we sought to determine whether the failure of the short depolarization protocol might be related to the use of Cs as the major internal cation. To examine this issue, we changed the internal solution to one containing K instead of Cs. Figure 5 shows that, with the use of this solution, it became possible to induce LTP by pairing high-frequency stimulation (50 Hz) with a brief (15 s) depolarization to 0 mv. Although the amount of LTP is large (217 21.6%, n 6), it is significantly smaller than that induced by the standard pairing protocol with Cs as the internal cation (307 36%, P 0.05). FIG. 3. Test of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR) dependence of 2 forms of potentiation. A: 50 M D-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (D-APV) blocks the LTP induced by high-frequency stimulation given at the end of a long depolarization (n 3), as in Fig. 2B. and E, test and control pathways, respectively. B: the potentiation produced by pairing high-frequency stimulation (to test pathway) with a brief depolarization (as in Fig. 1) is not blocked by 50 M D-APV. The control pathway (E) also showed some potentiation (n 10). Insets in A and B show the diagrams of the protocols. FIG. 4. Synaptic currents induced by the trains of high-frequency stimulation given during depolarization, and the holding current during depolarization. A: comparison of synaptic currents induced by 2 protocols given to same inputs in the same neuron at different times during long depolarization. The diagram at the top indicates the protocols. Left: responses evoked by 4 trains of high-frequency stimulation (50 pulse, 50 Hz) given at the beginning of depolarization, which did not induce LTP. Right: responses induced by 4 trains of high-frequency stimulation given at the end of depolarization, which did induce LTP. B: another comparison of synaptic currents induced by 2 protocols given in same cell but different inputs at different times. Left: the synaptic currents evoked by the trains of high-frequency stimulation paired with short depolarization, which did not induce LTP. Right: the synaptic current induced by trains of high-frequency stimulation given at end of long depolarization, which did induce LTP (note that stimulus artifacts were not picked up by this electrode). C: synaptic current induced by 1 train of high-frequency stimulation during the short high-frequency pairing protocol was blocked by APV (50 M). D: holding current during long depolarization (from 68 to 0 mv). Bars over holding current trace indicate where high-frequency stimulations were given. DISCUSSION We have investigated the requirements for inducing LTP using variants of the standard pairing protocol. In the standard protocol, depolarization is imposed on the postsynaptic neuron by voltage clamping the membrane to 0 mv while stimulating the input axons at fairly low frequency (0.1 2 Hz) for several minutes (Colino et al. 1992; Manabe et al. 1992; Otmakhov et al. 1997). Generally, Cs has been used as the major internal cation to block several K channels and thereby make it easier to impose depolarization. We have found that if we modified the standard protocol and used high-frequency (20 100 Hz) stimulation given during a brief (15 s) depolarization to 0 mv, LTP cannot be induced. This failure is not simply due to the use of high-frequency stimulation because LTP can be induced by high-frequency stimulation given at the end of a long depolarization. Furthermore, we have found that the failure to induce LTP during brief depolarization is related to the use of Cs -based internal solution. When K is the major cation, high-frequency stimulation given during a brief depolarization does induce LTP. Thus it would appear that Cs somehow prevents LTP induction when

530 H.-X. CHEN, N. OTMAKHOV, AND J. LISMAN FIG. 5. Short high-frequency pairing protocol induced LTP when a K - based internal solution was used. A: an individual experiment. 2, time when the short high-frequency pairing protocol was given to the test pathway ( ). E, control pathway. Inset: diagram of protocol. B: averages of EPSCs (20 successive responses) in test ( ) and control pathway (E) taken at the time marked by letters in A. C: summary data of experiments as shown in A (n 6). and E, test and control pathways, respectively. short high-frequency stimulation is paired with short depolarization. Importantly, this blocking action of Cs must then dissipate, because if high-frequency stimulation is given at the end of a long depolarization, LTP occurs normally. These findings are quite surprising and cannot be simply explained in terms of what is known about LTP. Cs is used to block K channels and should therefore enhance depolarization. It has been shown that depolarization up to the reversal potential for EPSC should enhance LTP induction, not inhibit it (Perkel et al. 1993). Because we do pairing at a potential somewhat negative of reversal potential, any dendritic depolarization may occur during synaptic stimulation (due to clamp failure) should cause the voltage to become closer to the reversal potential and therefore lead to larger potentiation rather than the very small potentiation we observed. The use of brief high-frequency stimulation should bring about facilitation of transmitter release and enhance LTP induction, not inhibit it. Unfortunately, we are unable to provide any clear explanation of our findings and can only point out several possibilities. A key unresolved question is how the NMDA current itself is affected by various induction conditions. The total synaptically evoked current during pairing can be measured and gives hints about what might be going on. This current is inward and appears larger when high-frequency stimulation is given during a brief depolarization (or early during a long depolarization) than when the same stimulation is given toward the end of a long depolarization (Fig. 4). A curious and unexplained feature of this current is that it often has an accelerating rising edge and a transient character. The inward current is strongly reduced by APV, suggesting that it is either largely NMDA current or consists of both NMDA current and components triggered as a consequence of current through the NMDA channel. On the assumption that the current is primarily NMDA current, one might ask why the current has such complex kinetics (Fig. 4). Recent work showed that the NMDA conductance is enhanced by intracellular Na (Yu and Salter 1998). So it is possible that the odd kinetics of the current during the tetanus are the consequence of Na -dependent regulation. If the total current is indeed largely NMDA current, then the failure to induce LTP during brief depolarization can certainly not be attributed to a reduced NMDA conductance, because the total synaptic current is larger at early times (Fig. 4). If anything, the results raise the possibility that there could be an optimal NMDA-mediated Ca 2 entry and that if this optimum is exceeded, smaller LTP will be induced (Lisman 1985). The transient nature of the synaptically evoked current during pairing suggests a second interpretation in which the initial NMDA-mediated synaptic current triggers a dendritic Ca 2 spike (assuming that clamping is not adequate in the dendrites). There are indications that Ca 2 elevations can inhibit the NMDA conductance itself (Legendre et al. 1993), and this might explain why LTP induction was blocked. Ca 2 spikes might be much more difficult to induce when K channels are functional, and this might explain why LTP can be induced when K is the major internal cation. In a related way, it might be supposed that inactivation of the Ca 2 conductance by a long depolarization prevents initiation of a Ca 2 spike late during the depolarization, and this could explain why stimulation given late during a long depolarization can induce LTP. It is possible that the failure to induce LTP by highfrequency stimulation given at the beginning of long depolarization (or during short depolarization) is related to both the high frequency and the timing when the tetanus is given. In this case, short low-frequency stimulation would induce LTP even if it is given at the beginning of long depolarization (or during short depolarization). Indeed the work has indicated that weak NMDA-dependent potentiation can be produced when a small number of stimuli are given at low frequency during brief depolarizations (Petersen et al. 1998). One possible explanation in terms of the Ca 2 spike model given above would be that only high-frequency stimulation can produce sufficient depolarization to trigger a Ca 2 spike.

REQUIREMENTS FOR PAIRING-INDUCED LTP 531 Our work is not the first attempt to induce LTP by combining high-frequency stimulation with imposed depolarization. A number of works reported that LTP could be induced by pairing procedures of this kind (Hjelmstad et al. 1997; Kato et al. 1993; Manabe et al. 1993; Perkel and Nicoll 1993). These experiments were done using Cs. However, the detailed timing of synaptic stimulation during the depolarization is not clear. These previous results may therefore not be incompatible with our findings. In a study done under experimental conditions (5-wk-old guinea pigs, at 32 C) (Chen et al. 1998) somewhat different from those used here ( 3-wk-old rats, room temperature, Cs as the major intracellular cation was used in both cases), it was possible to induce LTP by delivering two 20 Hz trains given 10 s apart during continuous depolarization lasting overall 15 s. It is unclear whether these technical differences are important. One previous paper reported that intracellular Cs can block tetanus-induced LTP (Haas and Rose 1984). Their results are consistent with ours, although they used sharp microelectrode recording, and depolarizing current was not injected during the tetanus. Our results indicate that there are aspects of the standard pairing procedure that are not understood, specifically, the requirement for long depolarization. This raises the question of whether pairing protocols are a good model for the LTP induced by tetanic stimulation or whether there are fundamental differences. One difference is the size of the LTP induced by these different protocols. We and others find that the standard pairing protocol produces a very large LTP. The synaptic response after LTP induction is often 400% and can sometimes be 1,000% (Malinow 1991; Otmakhov et al. 1997; Otmakhov, unpublished results). In contrast, the LTP induced by tetanic stimulation and field recording methods typically show an LTP that is 200% and is often only 150%. We find that the brief pairing protocol with K as the internal cation evokes an LTP that is intermediate in size (220%). Similarly, Lu et al. (1998) using whole-cell recording with K found an LTP of 200%. The factors that determine the size of LTP remain generally unclear. In particular, there is no good explanation for why multiple tetani evoke LTP of the field EPSP, which is at most 200% (saturation occurs with 3 tetani), whereas pairing in Cs can evoke a much larger LTP of up to 400%. Despite this difference in magnitude, there are no other reasons to think that fundamentally different mechanisms are involved in the LTP produced by pairing and tetanus protocols. Both forms of LTP are dependent on NMDA channels and can be blocked by kinase inhibitors (Otmakhov et al. 1997). In summary, we have studied the requirements for inducing LTP by pairing protocols. We unexpectedly found that when a tetanus is given at the beginning, but not at the end of depolarization, LTP induction is inhibited. The generation of this transient inhibition is related to the use of Cs as a major intracellular cation. These results are not expected from the known properties of LTP and suggest that there is a process that can powerfully regulate LTP induction that has not yet been identified. The authors thank Dr. Nonna Otmakhova for useful comments. This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grants 5 R01 NS-35083 and 5 R01 NS-27337. The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the W. M. Keck Foundation. Address reprint requests to J. Lisman. Received 21 December 1998; accepted in final form 21 April 1999. REFERENCES BLISS, T. V. AND COLLINGRIDGE, G. L. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361: 31 39, 1993. BROWN, T. H., KAIRISS, E. W., AND KEENAN, C. L. Hebbian synapses: biophysical mechanisms and algorithms. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13: 475 511, 1990. CHEN, H. X., HANSE, E., PANANCEAU, M., AND GUSTAFSSON, B. Distinct expressions for synaptic potentiation induced by calcium through voltagegated calcium and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor channels in the hippocampal CA1 region. Neuroscience 86: 415 422, 1998. COLINO, A., HUANG, Y. Y., AND MALENKA, R. C. Characterization of the integration time for the stabilization of long- term potentiation in area CA1 of the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 12: 180 187, 1992. GUSTAFSSON, B., WIGSTROM, H., ABRAHAM, W. C., AND HUANG, Y. Y. Longterm potentiation in the hippocampus using depolarizing current pulses as the conditioning stimulus to single volley synaptic potentials. J. Neurosci. 7: 774 780, 1987. HAAS, H. L. AND ROSE, G. The role of inhibitory mechanisms in hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neurosci. Lett. 47: 301 306, 1984. HJELMSTAD, G. O., NICOLL, R. A., AND MALENKA, R. C. Synaptic refractory period provides a measure of probability of release in the hippocampus. Neuron 19: 1309 1318, 1997. KATO, K., CLIFFORD, D. B., AND ZORUMSKI, C. F. Long-term potentiation during whole-cell recording in rat hippocampal slices. Neuroscience 53: 39 47, 1993. KULLMANN, D. M. AND NICOLL, R. A. Long-term potentiation is associated with increases in quantal content and quantal amplitude. Nature 357: 240 244, 1992. LEGENDRE, P., ROSENMUND, C., AND WESTBROOK, G. L. Inactivation of NMDA channels in cultured hippocampal neurons by intracellular calcium. J. Neurosci. 13: 674 684, 1993. LISMAN, J. E. A mechanism for memory storage insensitive to molecular turnover: a bistable autophosphorylating kinase. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 82: 3055 3057, 1985. LU, Y. M., RODER, J. C., DAVIDOW, J., AND SALTER, M. W. Src activation in the induction of long-term potentiation in CA1 hippocampal neurons. Science 279: 1363 1367, 1998. MAGEE, J., HOFFMAN, D., COLBERT, C., AND JOHNSTON, D. Electrical and calcium signaling in dendrites of hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 60: 327 346, 1998. MAGEE, J. C. Dendritic hyperpolarization-activated currents modify the integrative properties of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. J. Neurosci. 18: 7613 7624, 1998. MAGEE, J. C. AND JOHNSTON, D. A synaptically controlled, associative signal for Hebbian plasticity in hippocampal neurons. Science 275: 209 213, 1997. MALINOW, R. Transmission between pairs of hippocampal slice neurons: quantal levels, oscillations, and LTP. Science 252: 722 724, 1991. MALINOW, R. AND TSIEN, R. W. Presynaptic enhancement shown by whole-cell recordings of long-term potentiation in hippocampal slices. Nature 346: 177 180, 1990. MANABE, T., RENNER, P., AND NICOLL, R. A. Postsynaptic contribution to long-term potentiation revealed by the analysis of miniature synaptic currents. Nature 355: 50 55, 1992. MANABE, T., WYLLIE, D. J., PERKEL, D. J., AND NICOLL, R. A. Modulation of synaptic transmission and long-term potentiation: effects on paired pulse facilitation and EPSC variance in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. J. Neurophysiol. 70: 1451 1459, 1993. MAYER, M. L., WESTBROOK, G. L., AND GUTHRIE, P. B. Voltage-dependent block by Mg 2 of NMDA responses in spinal cord neurons. Nature 309: 261 263, 1984. NOWAK, L., BREGESTOVSKI, P., ASCHER, P., HERBET, A., AND PROCHIANTZ, A. Magnesium gates glutamate-activated channels in mouse central neurones. Nature 307: 462 465, 1984. OTMAKHOV, N., GRIFFITH, L. C., AND LISMAN, J. E. Postsynaptic inhibitors of calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase type II block induction but not

532 H.-X. CHEN, N. OTMAKHOV, AND J. LISMAN maintenance of pairing-induced long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci. 17: 5357 5365, 1997. PERKEL, D. J. AND NICOLL, R. A. Evidence for all-or-none regulation of neurotransmitter release: implications for long-term potentiation. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 471: 481 500, 1993. PERKEL, D. J., PETROZZINO, J. J., NICOLL, R.A.,AND CONNOR, J. A. The role of Ca 2 entry via synaptically activated NMDA receptors in the induction of long-term potentiation. Neuron 11: 817 823, 1993. PETERSEN, C. C., MALENKA, R. C., NICOLL, R. A., AND HOPFIELD, J. J. All-or-none potentiation at CA3-CA1 synapses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95: 4732 4737, 1998. THOMAS, M. J., WATABE, A. M., MOODY, T. D., MAKHINSON, M., AND O DELL, T. J. Postsynaptic complex spike bursting enables the induction of LTP by theta frequency synaptic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 18: 7118 7126, 1998. YU, X. M. AND SALTER, M. W. Gain control of NMDA-receptor currents by intracellular sodium. Nature 396: 469 474, 1998.