. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN

Similar documents
Manuscript Version Sage holds the Copyright. Introduction. Seemingly, no two investigators agree on what intelligence means or includes, but

Long-term memory for concepts by squirrel monkeys

Natural and Artificial Minds

INTERACTIVE MODELS OF COGNITIVE ABILITIES OF MONKEYS AND HUMANS ( Saimiri sciureus sciureus; S. boliviensus boliviensus; Homo sapiens sapiens)

Absolute versus relative class conceptual behavior in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus)

Wason's Cards: What is Wrong?

Sameness-difference judgments in Saimiri sciureus based on volumetric cues

Vertebrate Intelligence: A Review of the Laboratory Research

Transitive inference in pigeons: Control for differential value transfer

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

6. A theory that has been substantially verified is sometimes called a a. law. b. model.

Oddity and dimension-abstracted oddity (DAO) in squirrel monkeys

Mental operations on number symbols by-children*

Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Counting in a Computerized Testing Paradigm

Group Assignment #1: Concept Explication. For each concept, ask and answer the questions before your literature search.

Student Performance Q&A:

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Analogical Inference

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

Fukuoka University of Education

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Chapter 02 Developing and Evaluating Theories of Behavior

AP STATISTICS 2013 SCORING GUIDELINES

Virtual Reality Testing of Multi-Modal Integration in Schizophrenic Patients

SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL CONTROL OF MEMBERS OF AN EQUIVALENCE CLASS THESIS. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

ESTABLISHING FRACTION-DECIMAL EQUIVALENCE USING A RESPONDENT-TYPE TRAINING PROCEDURE.

Evolution of Intelligence: an Approach to Its Assessment

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

Color Cues and Viscosity in. Iterated Prisoner s Dilemma

Confidence in Causal Inferences: The Case of Devaluation

ON THE EFFECTS OF EXTENDED SAMPLE-OBSERVING RESPONSE REQUIREMENTS ON ADJUSTED DELAY IN A TITRATING DELAY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE PROCEDURE WITH PIGEONS

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

CFSD 21 st Century Learning Rubric Skill: Critical & Creative Thinking

Internal & External Validity

Convergence Principles: Information in the Answer

Fundamental Concepts for Using Diagnostic Classification Models. Section #2 NCME 2016 Training Session. NCME 2016 Training Session: Section 2

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

Doctoral Dissertation Boot Camp Quantitative Methods Kamiar Kouzekanani, PhD January 27, The Scientific Method of Problem Solving

REVERSAL OF EQUIVALENCE RELATIONS

The effect of premise order in conditional reasoning: a test of the mental model theory

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

A Bayesian Network Model of Causal Learning

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

VARIABLES AND MEASUREMENT

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

26:010:557 / 26:620:557 Social Science Research Methods

To conclude, a theory of error must be a theory of the interaction between human performance variability and the situational constraints.

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Simulations of Classification Learning using EPAM VI

Learning Within-Category Attribute Correlations in a One-Attribute Visual Search Classification Paradigm

THE "FOUR STIMULUS - TWO CHOICE " - PARADIGM IN MULTIDIMENSIONAL PSYCHOPHYSICS: SIZE, BRIGHTNESS, AND COLOR DIMENSIONS COMBINED *

Cognitive Modeling. Lecture 9: Intro to Probabilistic Modeling: Rational Analysis. Sharon Goldwater

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

Cognitive Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling. Mechanistic Modeling Rational Analysis

Doing After Seeing. Seeing vs. Doing in Causal Bayes Nets

Underlying Theory & Basic Issues

Content Effects in Conditional Reasoning: Evaluating the Container Schema

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016

Simpler for Evolution: Secondary Representation in Apes, Children, and Ancestors. Thomas Suddendorf University of Auckland

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

component solution modes

Does momentary accessibility influence metacomprehension judgments? The influence of study judgment lags on accessibility effects

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos

TRANSLATING RESEARCH INTO PRACTICE

Serial List Linking by Macaque Monkeys (Macaca mulatta): List Property Limitations

Item Analysis Explanation

THE TEST STATISTICS REPORT provides a synopsis of the test attributes and some important statistics. A sample is shown here to the right.

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

VISUAL PERCEPTION OF STRUCTURED SYMBOLS

THE REFERENTIAL NATURE OF RULES AND INSTRUCTIONS: A RESPONSE TO INSTRUCTIONS, RULES, AND ABSTRACTION: A MISCONSTRUED RELATION BY EMILIO RIBES-IÑESTA

Chapter 1 Social Science and Its Methods

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast?

Myths! Myths and Realities of Evaluation, Identification, and Diagnosis of ASD 10/11/10. Facts. The Ziggurat Group

The Research Process: Coming to Terms

Inferring Actions and Observations from Interactions

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

Insight Assessment Measuring Thinking Worldwide

Value Transfer in a Simultaneous Discrimination Appears to Result From Within-Event Pavlovian Conditioning

PRESCHOOLERS INDUCTIVE SELECTIVITY AS A FUNCTION OF IMPLICIT AND CONCEPTUAL LEARNING

Alfred North Whitehead s Process and Reality

Supplemental Material 3a Wilhelm Wundt. In this module, we will take a closer look at Wilhelm Wundt in order to get a better idea

ANTECEDENT REINFORCEMENT CONTINGENCIES IN THE STIMULUS CONTROL OF AN A UDITORY DISCRIMINA TION' ROSEMARY PIERREL AND SCOT BLUE

Cognitive Neuroscience Section 8

European Federation of Statisticians in the Pharmaceutical Industry (EFSPI)

Uncertainty in causal inference: The case of retrospective revaluation

The idea of an essentially contested concept is incoherent.

UNIT 4 ALGEBRA II TEMPLATE CREATED BY REGION 1 ESA UNIT 4

Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing

Transcription:

1 Roger K. Thomas The University of Georgia Athens, Georgia Unites States of America rkthomas@uga.edu Synonyms. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN Pre-publication manuscript version. Published as follows: Thomas, R. K. (2012). Conditional reasoning by nonhuman animals. In Norbert Seel (Ed.). Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning: Part 3 (pp. 742-745). New York: Springer-Verlag. Conditional association, conditional discrimination learning, conditional rule learning, if-then reasoning, if-then rule learning, logical reasoning, relational concept learning Definition Conditional reasoning (conditional association or conditional rule-leeminq might be better terms) means that, when performing complex tasks, animals should partition discriminanda consistent with the truth-table manifestations for the conditional in symbolic logic (see example below). Conditional reasoning is representative of relational concept learning at the next-to-highest level of intellectual capabilities based on Thomas 's approach to assessing animal intelligence (e.g., Thomas, 1980; Bailey, McDaniel & Thomas, 2007). Theoretical Background It is generally accepted in the human concept learning literature that a nonverbal, experimental demonstration of conditional reasoning must result in the partitioning of discriminanda consistent with the truth-table manifestations specified for those discriminanda by the conditional in symbolic logic (Borne, 1970). Attending first only to the bold print letters and symbols in the truth-tables below, consider both the conditional and the conjunctive, because all known experiments using nonhuman animals have confounded conjunctive and conditional reasoning as potential explanations for successful performances. Conjunctive Conditional & p p>q T red T correct T red T T correct T red F incorrect T red F F incorrect F not-red F incorrect F not-red T T correct F not-red F incorrect F not-red F T correct Truth-tables are abstractions. To adapt them for experimental research, Borne (1970) used discriminanda that varied in color and form. Referring again to the truth-tables and using red and square as focal attributes, substitute red when p is T and not-red when p is F and substitute square when q is T and not-square when q is F. Regarding partitioning outcomes, beneath p&q or beneath p>q, read T as denoting a "correct" partition and F as denoting an "incorrect" partition according to contingencies for each row in the truth-tables. As may be seen in the truth-tables and in the illustration below (adapted from Borne), the only correct partition for the conjunctive is when the object is a red-square. For the conditional, the only incorrect partitions are red objects that are not-square; no conditions are specified for being incorrect when p is

2 not-red. In Borne's (1970) research, subjects had to infer which truth-table was applicable based on experimenter feedback, such as, saying "correct" or "incorrect" according to whether the discriminanda were being partitioned consistently with a given truth-table's contingencies. The illustration also shows how discriminanda must be partitioned according to conjunctive, disjunctive, conditional, or biconditional truth-tables when red and square are the focal attributes. When Red and Square are Focal Attributes, Correct and Incorrect Assignments of Discriminanda according to Truth-Table Requirements for Conjunctive, Disjunctive, Conditional and Bi ~oljditiqjjlllb_elationships _ Co There I::; all extensive rustory ot Investigating "conditional discrimination learning," "conditional rule learning," "if-then rule learning," etc. by nonhuman animals using various procedures, and often it is stated or implied that the animals had demonstrated conditional reasoning corresponding to forms such as, "if p, then q." However, this article questions whether there has ever been a valid demonstration of conditional reasoning by nonhuman animals. Previous investigators used methods that either (a) confounded conditional reasoning with the possibility of rote-memorization or (b) confounded the possibility of conditional reasoning with conjunctive reasoning. The only nonverbal procedure of which I am aware that might be used to show unequivocal conditional reasoning by an animal was developed for use with humans. However, that experiment appears to be impractically difficult for nonhuman animals, and its author (Bourne, 1970) relied partly on the subjects' verbal explanations to confirm how they had reasoned. It is hoped that one result of the present article will be to prevent future researchers from misinterpreting or misrepresenting, either inadvertently or intentionally, the results of typical conditional-discrimination, rule-learning research using nonhuman animals. Important Scientific Research and Open Questions The typical conditional learning task used with nonhuman animals involves two successivelypresented discriminanda, represented here as A and B, only one of which is presented on a given trial, and two simultaneously-presented discriminanda, represented here as X and Y, which appear on every trial. A or B serves as an associative cue to select either X or Y. It is

3 tempting to describe and conceptualize such tasks, as many investigators have done, as embodying conditional reasoning such as: "If A, then X and if B, then Y." Typically, relatively few discriminanda are used and they are presented more than once. Repeated presentations make it likely that the relatively few specific configurations afforded by the discriminanda might be learned by rote-memorization. As others have noted, such configuration learning is confounded with the possibility that the animals used conditional reasoning. However, such confounding prevents such studies from providing conclusive evidence for conditional reasoning by animals. Even if specific configuration learning is precluded, there remains a fundamental problem that all known experiments using animals have confounded the possibility of conjunctive with conditional reasoning. There are three basic ways to avoid specific configuration learning: (a) use exemplars from conceptual categories for the successive discriminanda, (b) use exemplars from conceptual categories for the simultaneous discriminanda, or (c) use exemplars from conceptual categories for both the successive and simultaneous discriminanda. Burdyn and Thomas's (1984) investigation will illustrate both the use of conceptual categories as discriminanda and how conjunctive and conditional reasoning are confounded. Burdyn and Thomas (1984) used exemplars of the conceptual categories "same" and "different" as the simultaneous discriminanda; an exemplar of "same" was an identical pair of objects and an exemplar of "different" was a non-identical pair of objects. New pairs of objects were used on each trial in the conceptual category phases of the testing which precluded the monkeys from memorizing specific discriminanda and reinforcement associations. The successive discriminanda involved the conceptual categories "triangularity" and "heptagonality" which were represented by using 120 discriminable triangles and 120 discriminable heptagons. Such a large number of discriminanda together with trial-unique exemplars of "same" and "different" made it unlikely that the monkeys memorized and associated specific triangles and heptagons with same and different. An apparatus with three guillotine doors was used. During most of the training, all three doors were raised and lowered concurrently. On a given trial, (a) either a triangle or a heptagon appeared as the center door was raised, (b) a pair of identical objects appeared as a result of raising one of the outer doors, and (c) a pair of non-identical objects appeared as a result of raising the other outer door; the choice of triangle or heptagon and the left-right locations of the same and different pairs were determined quasi-randomly for each trial. When a triangle was presented, the correct response was to displace the object-member of the same-pair that was closest to the center door; doing so revealed a food well with a bit of fruit reinforcement beneath the object. When a heptagon was presented, the correct response, similarly reinforced, was to the object-member of the difference-pair that was closest to the center door. In the final stage of training, the center door was raised to expose either a triangle or a heptagon; then, it was closed to cover the triangle or heptagon before the outer doors were raised to expose the same and different pairs of objects. Intervals between closing the center door and concurrently raising the outer doors were increased systematically. The best performing monkey met a stringent criterion of correct responding (13 of 15 correct on 15 triangle-same trials and 13 of 15 correct on 15 heptagon-different trials within a 3D-trials session) with a 16 sec. interval. Therefore, when the successive cues were visually absent, "triangularity" and "heptagonality" had to be retained symbolically in working memory as cues for "same" and "different," respectively.

4 It is tempting to conceptualize the monkeys' successful performances as conditional reasoning which might be expressed as "iftriangle, then same" and "if heptagon, then different." However, Burdyn and Thomas realized that they could not conclude that unequivocally, because it was also possible that the monkeys were reasoning conjunctively such as "triangle and same" and "heptagon and different." This general interpretational problem appears to have affected all other so-called "conditional rule learning" studies in animals. It should be noted also that most animal studies have not used conceptual-category discriminanda which means their subjects might have memorized the specific configurations associated with the discriminandareinforcement contingencies. Bourne (1970) also realized that his subjects might have performed on some basis other than implementing the requirements of the appropriate truth-table, but he was able to determine through a series of transfer experiments that his subjects had learned the rules. Some of the transfer experiments involved the experimenter and the subjects discussing the applicable rule. It is unlikely that such verbal validation will be available to animal researchers, and it remains to be seen whether animals will show the kind of perfect or near-perfect transfer of training that is necessary otherwise to confirm that the subject reasoned conditionally. By "near-perfect," it is meant that there must be so few mistakes that the subject likely could not have memorized specific discriminanda and reinforcement relationships. A minimum of four trials is necessary merely to present the minimal information to show which rule is operating, namely, one trial each to manifest each row contingency in a given truth-table. After being trained on a succession of problems based on the same logical operation, Bourne's human subjects learned to use the four informational trials to attain thereafter perfect or nearperfect performances on new problems. Presumably, this could be done only if the subjects had inferred correctly and followed the appropriate truth-table. Future animal research on conditional reasoning can and must be improved by precluding the possibility of rote-memorization of the discriminanda or configurations of the discriminanda. This is best done by using conceptual-category discriminanda. Response contingencies that allow the subject to affirm or negate exemplars might be helpful. If animal experiments are based on Bourne's procedure, they would involve reinforcing an animal's responses that correctly affirmed or negated each discriminandum in accordance with the applicable truth table. A series of problems should be administered according to a single operation, until, following the administration of the four mandatory, informational trials on new problems, the animal continued with perfect or near-perfect performances, or until it seemed unlikely that the animal would be able to attain such performances. If perfect or near-perfect performances were seen on new problems, it should be reasonable to attribute the use of the conditional reasoning to the animal (or conjunctive reasoning, etc., depending upon which truth-table was being applied). This article would be incomplete without acknowledging that some scholars have tried to reconcile standard logic with what some refer to as "natural" or "mental logic" (e.g., Braine & O'Brien, 1998). Such logic is said to apply to cases of reasoning that reflect genuine, "if-then" conditional reasoning without using procedures that fulfill the requirements of the truth-table for the conditional. However, consideration of natural versus standard logic has not revealed how the methods associated with natural logic will enable us to design experiments to distinguish how animals may have reasoned. Thus, it appears that the most conservative and justifiable approach is to continue to attempt to investigate animals' use of the conditional reasoning based on methods that embody truth-functional logic.

5 Cross-References - Abstract concept learning in animals - Animal learning and intelligence - Associative learning - Categorical learning - Complex learning - Complex problem solving - Concept learning - Conditional reasoning - Conditions of learning - Robert M. Gagne - Discrimination learning model - Evolution of learning - Human learning - Inductive reasoning - Judgment of similarity - Laboratory learning - Logical reasoning and learning - Nature of creativity - Problem solving - Rote memorization References Bailey, A.M., McDaniel, W.F, & Thomas, RK. (2007). Approaches to the study of higher cognitive functions related to creativity in nonhuman animals. Methods, 42, 3-14. Bourne, L. E., Jr. (1970). Knowing and using concepts. Psychological Review, 77, 546-556. Braine, M. D. S., & 0' Brien, D. P. (Eds.) (1998). Mental logic. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Burdyn, L. E., Jr., & Thomas, R K. (1984). Conditional discrimination with conceptual simultaneous and successive cues in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 98, 405-413. Thomas, R K. (1980). Evolution of intelligence: An approach to its assessment. Brain, Behavior & Evolution, 17,452-474.