OSTRACISM IS LOST IN TRANSLATION

Similar documents
The Vine Assessment System by LifeCubby

Choosing Life: Empowerment, Action, Results! CLEAR Menu Sessions. Health Care 3: Partnering In My Care and Treatment

Peer Support Meeting COMMUNICATION STRATEGIES

IMPROVING INTERPERSONAL RELATIONSHIPS. Facilitator: Ms. Vu Viet Hang (M.Ed)

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Lesson 12. Understanding and Managing Individual Behavior

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

CAREER BASE CAMP Day 2: Leverage Your Emotional Intelligence

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

Shyness: The fear of getting acquainted

Some Important Concepts in EFT

CONCEPTS GUIDE. Improving Personal Effectiveness With Versatility

Changes to your behaviour

REASON FOR REFLECTING

Managing conversations around mental health. Blue Light Programme mind.org.uk/bluelight

EFFECTS OF SOCIAL ANXIETY AND GROUP MEMBERSHIP OF POTENTIAL AFFILIATES ON SOCIAL RECONNECTION AFTER OSTRACISM

Functional Analytic Psychotherapy Basic Principles. Clinically Relevant Behavior (CRB)

Activities for Someone in Early in Dementia

STUDENT GUIDE NAVEDTRA A CHAPTER 9 BASIC NEEDS AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR

My Notebook. A space for your private thoughts.

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

Learn how to more effectively communicate with others. This will be a fun and informative workshop! Sponsored by

Chapter 5 Lesson 2: Mental Disorders. Mental disorders are medical conditions that require diagnosis and treatment.

7 WAYS TO BECOME YOUR OWN MEDICINE. In order to have a solid platform to heal from trauma, we need to ensure we have our foundation in place.

READY. Book. CURRICULUM ASSOCIATES, Inc. A Quick-Study Program TEST

2 Psychological Processes : An Introduction

Chapter 1. Dysfunctional Behavioral Cycles

Introduction to Social Psychology

COACH WORKPLACE REPORT. Jane Doe. Sample Report July 18, Copyright 2011 Multi-Health Systems Inc. All rights reserved.

IT S A WONDER WE UNDERSTAND EACH OTHER AT ALL!

Cambridge Public Schools SEL Benchmarks K-12

REDUCING MENTAL HEALTH STIGMA

TRACOM Sneak Peek Excerpts from. Self-Perception Guide

An INSIDE OUT Family Discussion Guide. Introduction.

Problem Situation Form for Parents

Personal Listening Profile Facilitator Report

Module one reflective account

Improving Personal Effectiveness With Versatility

Chapter 7. M.G.Rajanandh, Department of Pharmacy Practice, SRM College of Pharmacy, SRM University.

A strong bond between two or more people p refers to interpersonal relationship.

Interacting with people

Interviewer: Tell us about the workshops you taught on Self-Determination.

16.3 Interpersonal Needs L E A R N I N G O B JE C T I V E

SPORT PSYCHOLOGY. Effective Communication and Dealing with Athletes with Low Self-Esteem

ORIENTATION SAN FRANCISCO STOP SMOKING PROGRAM

MA 1 Notes. moving the hand may be needed.

FACT SHEET - CHILDCARE

Operation S.A.V.E Campus Edition

CONTENTS. Where do stem cells come from? Source of stem cells to be utilized. What are the clinic and physicians credentials?

Information Session. What is Dementia? People with dementia need to be understood and supported in their communities.

SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY. Social Influences on the Self. Self Concept. How do we see ourselves? How do we see others?

Conversation Tactics Checklist (Hallam, R S, Ashton, P, Sherbourne, K, Gailey, L, & Corney, R. 2007).

#025: MAINTAINING A POSITIVE ATTITUDE WHILE LEARNING ENGLISH

Mastering Emotions. 1. Physiology

A Model Minority A Culture-General Framework Analysis

Step One for Gamblers

Dealing with Depression Feature Article July 2008

It s Mental Health Week!

Good Communication Starts at Home

Psychology in Your Life

M E N TA L A N D E M O T I O N A L P R O B L E M S

Communication (Journal)

Introduction to Research Methods

MA 1 Notes. Deaf vs deaf p. 3 MA1 F 13

Gender Differences Associated With Memory Recall. By Lee Morgan Gunn. Oxford May 2014

S.A.F.E.T.Y. TM Profile for Joe Bloggs. Joe Bloggs. Apr / 13

ADHD Explanation 2: Oppositional defiant disorder and MERIM

Reading Horizons. Case Studies of the Influence of Reading on Adolescents. Fehl L. Shirley JANUARY Volume 9, Issue Article 4

First, Manage Your Anger. Conflict Management!!! Healthy Anger. Causes of anger 5/14/14. Manage anger first Manage conflict second

Emotional-Social Intelligence Index

Managing emotions in turbulent and troubling times. Professor Peter J. Jordan Griffith Business School

Leading with Emotional Intelligence. Courtney Holladay, PhD Executive Director

Building Emotional Intelligence. Presented by: Andria Hill-Lehr

From the scenario below please identify the situation, thoughts, and emotions/feelings.

copyrighted material by PRO-ED, Inc.

How to stop Someone who is ADDICTED ENABLING

Emotional Changes After a Traumatic Brain Injury

Contents. Chapter. Coping with Crisis. Section 16.1 Understand Crisis Section 16.2 The Crises People Face. Chapter 16 Coping with Crisis

Focus of Today s Presentation. Partners in Healing Model. Partners in Healing: Background. Data Collection Tools. Research Design

Counseling Skills (I) Attending and Listening. Mr. Lema, Isaac Clinical Psychologist (MSc) 18 th December 2015

The Power of Feedback

Introduction to Stages of Change and Change Talk in Motivational Interviewing Lisa Kugler, PsyD. March 29, 2018

Anxiety Studies Division Annual Newsletter

INTERPERSONAL EFFECTIVENESS

What is Stress? Stress can be defined as our mental, physical, emotional, and behavioral reactions to any perceived demands or threats.

Practitioner Guidelines for Enhanced IMR for COD Handout #2: Practical Facts About Mental Illness

Examinee : - JOHN SAMPLE. Company: - ABC Industries Date: - December 8, 2011

Most scholars recognize that we are a combination Heredity and environmental factors interact with and affect one another

Emotional Intelligence

BIPOLAR DISORDER. BIPOLAR DISORDER is. a lifelong illness. It affects. millions of people each. year. With proper treatment,

Mindfulness Over Matter

Self-Injury. What is it? How do I get help? Adapted from Signs of Self-Injury Program

Treatment Approaches for Individuals with Brain Injury. Welcome!

Look to see if they can focus on compassionate attention, compassionate thinking and compassionate behaviour. This is how the person brings their

Living with Hearing Loss: Workbook

Client Care Counseling Critique Assignment Osteoporosis

OPWDD: Putting People First. Redefining the Role of the DSP Trainer s Manual. Developed by Regional Centers for Workforce Transformation

NONPROFIT CONFLICT COMMUNICATION SKILLS

K I N G. mentally ill E N. 38 myevt.com exceptional veterinary team March/April 2012

Altar Working Systems and Strategies. Pastor: Ball

Transcription:

OSTRACISM IS LOST IN TRANSLATION Student Author Han Kang is a senior in psychological sciences in the College of Health and Human Sciences. Supervised by Dr. Kipling D. Williams, she is currently enrolled in the Research-Focused Honors Program. Kang received a departmental research grant to complete her honors thesis. She has presented at the 10 th Annual Psychology Undergraduate Research Conference and will present at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association in Chicago. With research experience in both social psychology and industrial/organizational psychology, Kang s plan after graduation is to attend graduate school in counseling. Mentor Kipling D. Williams is a social psychologist in the Department of Psychological Science at Purdue University. He graduated from The Ohio State University in 1981 with a Ph.D. in social psychology. Currently, he is a professor of psychological sciences and is the associate editor of the Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. He is most notable for his research on ostracism, developing unique methods to study the processes and consequences. Williams has contributed psychological articles and written several books related to these interests. Abstract How do you feel when people around you start conversing in a language you do not understand? In addition to feeling ostracized, you may also wonder if they are talking about you. Participants were either talked to or not talked to in three different language conditions: English, English-Chinese, and Chinese. Participants experienced more distress when being ostracized than included by English, but they found inclusion more distressing than ostracism by English-Chinese. Additionally, more paranoid attributions were made by participants who were included, rather than ostracized by those speaking an unfamiliar language. This study is the first to show that, under some exceptional circumstances, it is worse to be included than ostracized. Kang, H. (2012). Ostracism is lost in translation: When being ignored and excluded does not matter, Journal of Purdue Undergraduate Research, 2, 36 41. doi:10.5703/jpur.02.1.06 Keywords antisocial thoughts, exclusion, inclusion, ostracism, paranoid attributions, social psychology 36 journal of purdue undergraduate research: volume http://dx.doi.org/10.5703/jpur.02.1.06 2, fall 2012

OSTRACISM IS LOST IN TRANSLATION: When Being Ignored and Excluded Does Not Matter Han Kang, Psychological Sciences INTRODUCTION Imagine a group of lions, a pride, living together as a big family. A lion kicked out by its pride will soon suffer from food deprivation and loneliness. The only way to survive is to find another pride, either by joining one that existed previously, or building a brand new one. Human beings often feel rejected when remaining unnoticed not only by strangers we know nothing about, but also by friends and family members we know so well. Social butterflies are not always the center of attention. Teenagers may be left out by their friends who attend an interesting event without them. A couple dealing with a disagreement might ignore and not speak to each other for a few months. Ostracism is everywhere. being ostracized threatens four fundamental human needs: belonging, self-esteem, control, and meaningful existence. Short-term effects of ostracism include fortifying saliently threatened needs and reestablishing inclusionary status. Ostracism over a long period of time deprives individuals of the resources necessary for fortifying threatened needs, resulting in alienation, depression, and helplessness. Ostracism by language (or linguistic ostracism) is relatively a new paradigm of ostracism. As defined by Dotan-Eliaz, Sommer, and Rubin (2009), linguistic ostracism is a situation in which two or more people converse in a language that others around them cannot understand. In the few studies examining linguistic ostracism, it has been found that ostracized targets Ostracism being ignored and excluded is a phenomenon that occurs among all social animals (Figure 1). Until the mid-1990s, it had not received much attention from social psychologists (Williams, 2001). Since then, over one hundred research publications have examined many facets of ostracism (also called exclusion and rejection), resulting in a better understanding of its effects on human physiology, emotion, cognition, and behavior (Williams, 2007). Regardless of how ostracism is manipulated or in what context, to date all studies indicate that ostracism causes more distress than inclusion (Williams, 2009). Williams (1997; 2009) developed a Temporal NeedThreat Model which argues that ostracism is first detected quickly and crudely. It takes the slightest representation of ostracism allowing for frequent over-detection. Whether or not it is rational to feel distressed by ostracism, it nevertheless causes pain. Williams s model asserts that Figure 1. Humans are not the only animals to live profound social lives. ostracism is lost in translation 37

experienced more negative emotions, less competence, and formed negative impressions of the ostracizers (Dotan-Eliaz et al., 2009). In a similar study, linguistically included participants reported higher levels of organizational commitment and organizational citizenship behaviors than excluded participants. In addition, participants who had been excluded linguistically expressed higher levels of threat and prejudice against immigrants than those who had been included (Hitlan, Kelly, Schepman, Schneider, & Zárate, 2006). The purpose of the current research is to examine the effects of linguistic ostracism and inclusion on individuals fundamental needs, moods, antisocial thoughts, paranoid attributions, and cognitive performance. I hypothesized that being ostracized by people speaking a foreign language would result in greater psychological distress than being ostracized by people speaking in one s own language, because the target (the individual who had been ostracized) could still have some amount of explanatory control over the situation by understanding the content of others conversations. Contrary to other published studies on ostracism, however, I also hypothesized that being included by people speaking a foreign language would result in higher levels of distress, desired aggression, and paranoia than being ostracized by people speaking a foreign language. The target may assume that others were saying something derogatory and perceive the inclusion as extremely offensive. Participants memory performance was tested without specific predictions, but with the general expectation that with distress, performance would suffer (Baumeister & Bushman, 2008). METHOD Participants and Design One hundred twenty-two Purdue University undergraduates enrolled in introductory psychology participated in the study as part of a course requirement and were assigned to a 2 (inclusion/ostracism) 3 (English/English-Chinese/Chinese) between-s design. There were two factors (independent variables) in this design, the ostracism condition and the language condition. The combination of each level of one factor to each level of the other factor produced six different situations. Each participant was only assigned to one of the six scenarios, and no one was ever allowed to participate in this experiment more than once. Procedure Participants were ostracized or included by two confederates (student assistants pretending to participate in the same study session) who either conversed (following a general script about what classes they were taking, their schedules for a coming vacation, and their families and hometowns) only with each other or also with the participant. Language condition was manipulated by whether the confederates spoke English (the language of the participants) or Chinese. In the two foreign language conditions, confederates either only spoke Chinese, or else they first spoke English, and then shifted to Chinese. Manipulation checks were used to confirm the validity of present paradigm. We wanted to make sure that the manipulation of the ostracism condition (by being talked to or not being talked to) and the manipulation of the language condition (by confederates speaking language) worked the way they were supposed to work. For one thing, participants should be aware of whether or not they were ostracized. For another, the speaking language of confederates should be noticed by participants. The dependent variables (e.g., the effects of the inclusion or ostracism that we measured) were participants distress, antisocial thoughts, and paranoid attributions, all assessed on a computer. Manipulation checks (e.g., To what extent do you remember other people looking at you while you were waiting ; To what extent do you remember other people speaking English to you while you were waiting ; I felt ignored ; I felt excluded ), distress (e.g., I felt disconnected to the group ; My self-esteem was high ; I had control over the course of the conversation ; I felt invisible ), antisocial thoughts (e.g., Shouting or yelling at another person ; Throwing something at another person that could hurt him or her ; Slapping another person ), and paranoid attributions (e.g., To what extent were you suspicious that others were talking about you ; To what extent were you suspicious that others were laughing at/speaking ill of you ; To what extent do you agree that other people had the conversation because they are mean-spirited ) were all assessed using 5-point scales from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). In each experimental session, one participant who did not speak Chinese was in a room with two Chinese confederates. Participants were told that we were interested in examining the effects of emotion on memory but needed another 5 minutes to set up. They were asked to wait quietly in the room. Depending upon the condition to which each participant had been assigned, our confederates began to chat either with each other or with the participant in English only, English at first and then switched to Chinese, or just Chinese. The experimenter returned to the room after 5 minutes and delivered a brief instruction for the experiment coming next. The two confederates then followed her out, pretending to go to a different lab. Participants were left alone to complete 38 journal of purdue undergraduate research: volume 2, fall 2012

follow-up questionnaires. The questionnaires were always presented in a fixed order. Items within each questionnaire were presented in a randomized order. A hidden camera was used to videotape each session. Participants were fully debriefed after finishing all questions. RESULTS Manipulation Checks Participants better remembered others speaking English when confederates spoke English only than when they shifted from English to Chinese, and when confederates spoke English to Chinese compared to Chinese only. They better remembered confederates looking at them under the condition of inclusion rather than ostracism. Participants also reported greater perceptions of being ignored and excluded when they were ostracized rather than when included. Psychological Distress Distress was measured by the extent to which participants fundamental needs were satisfied after the conversations. Less satisfaction of the needs of belonging, self-esteem, control, and meaningful existence served as our measure of distress. By examining only the effects of language condition, regardless of whether people were ostracized or included, we found no overall effects. As with prior studies, participants experienced more distress when being ostracized than included by English only. But, contrary to existing literature, participants reported less distress when being ostracized than included when the confederates first spoke English, then shifted to Chinese. See Figure 2. Antisocial Thoughts Analyses revealed both the language condition and the ostracism condition had a significant effect on antisocial thoughts. Participants reported being more tempted to behave antisocially toward the confederates when Chinese was the only language spoken, and also when they were included rather than ostracized. However, the interaction between the language and ostracism conditions was not significant. See Figure 3. Paranoid Attributions Paranoid attributions were an integration of three individual scales I was suspicious that others were talking about me ; I was suspicious that others were laughing at/speaking ill of me ; and I agree that others had the conversation because they are mean-spirited. The language condition particularly affected paranoid attributions. Participants were more likely to make paranoid attributions both in the English-Chinese condition and the Chinese-only condition, compared to the English-only condition. The ostracism condition also significantly affected paranoid attributions. Unexpectedly, more paranoid attributions were made when participants were included than when ostracized, and there was no interaction found between the language and ostracism conditions on paranoid attributions. See Figure 4. DISCUSSION As with past research, when ostracized during a conversation, participants felt more negative reactions than when they were included. However, if the other people are speaking a foreign language, in this case Chinese, then inclusion resulted in more distress than ostracism. These results point to an important boundary condition for the general view that inclusion is better than ostracism. Apparently, being talked to and included by foreign language speakers is more distressing than being ignored by those foreign language speakers. Previous research on linguistic ostracism failed to include this condition, leading to the general conclusion that all forms of inclusion are less distressing than any form of ostracism. The other prediction made was that being included by people speaking an unfamiliar language would result in higher levels of desired aggression and paranoid attributions than being ostracized by people speaking in an unfamiliar language. This prediction was firmly supported by present findings. When being included by Chinese only rather than by English only, participants were more tempted to hurt the others and make the others uncomfortable. Speaking an unfamiliar language to someone apparently violates a social norm, which in turn evoked antisocial impulses. More paranoid attributions were made under the conditions of English-Chinese and Chinese only than English only. As participants had no idea what the two confederates were saying, they appeared to have assumed that the others were talking about or even speaking ill of them. Things turned worse when Chinese was spoken given the condition of inclusion instead of ostracism. Participants suspicion that the other two students were discussing a topic closely relevant to them was confirmed this time because they were directly talked to. Perhaps they felt that others were teasing them in a foreign language. Further research needs to assess their thought processes during this condition. General Discussion Language is a new domain of ostracism research that occurs in everyday life and has special relevance to immigrants. To our surprise, the present findings neither show similar effects to previous research that also uses a ostracism is lost in translation 39

Figure 2. Participants experienced more distress when being ostracized by English only, but less distress when being ostracized than included by English-Chinese. Figure 3. Participants were more tempted to behave antisocially toward the others when being included than ostracized, and when Chinese was the only language spoken. Figure 4. More paranoid attributions were made by participants who were included, rather than ostracized by those speaking English-Chinese and Chinese only. 40 journal of purdue undergraduate research: volume 2, fall 2012

language paradigm nor are they exactly consistent with our hypotheses. Nevertheless, the current study expands ostracism theory to areas such as inclusion by a foreign language and paranoid attributions. In this case, ostracism by an unfamiliar language does not appear to be more hurtful than ostracism by an understandable language. But inclusion by an unfamiliar language is definitely more distressing than ostracism by an unfamiliar language. While distressing, responses to ostracism are not severe. In today s modern global climate, the possibility of meeting foreigners who have no knowledge of one language or another is growing at a fantastic speed. This study demonstrates that it is better to leave an individual alone if we cannot speak his or her native language or a language he or she is able to understand. The individual will not feel particularly distressed when being ostracized by us speaking a language foreign to them, but they may have antisocial thoughts against us and question our motivation and personality if we try to speak to them first, even though all we mean to do is to show our respect and concern. Limitations and Future Directions The current study has three major limitations. First of all, confederates were not blind to condition. Four international undergraduates were recruited as research assistants to work in dyads. Their job was either to ostracize participants from or to include participants in conversations by either English or Chinese, according to the condition each participant was assigned. After receiving quick training and practicing for a week, our confederates became quite familiar with their scripts as well as all six conditions. Although all conversations between confederates and their interactions with participants were based on formulated scripts, previously knowing which condition a participant was in could influence confederates verbal and nonverbal behaviors, such as tones, accents, gestures, and facial expressions. Furthermore, confederates were not given training in looking at each participant for the same amount of time. Future research may consider using a video-chat paradigm instead. sessions showed a rich array of nonverbal, paraverbal, and verbal responses to the various conditions, too many sessions were unsuccessful to warrant analysis. Followup research would benefit from these analyses, so better control over the hardware is suggested. CONCLUSIONS Although follow-up research is needed, the present research establishes that people can feel ostracized through conversations, but that the language used in the conversation can alter their feelings of distress. Apparently, and for the first time, this research indicates an exception to the rule that ostracism is always worse than inclusion. If others are speaking a foreign language, individuals unfamiliar with that language may wish to be ignored and excluded from those conversations rather than being subjected to awkward and ambiguous inclusion. REFERENCES Baumeister, R. F., & Bushman, B. J. (2008). Social psychology and human nature (Annotated Instructor s Ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth. Dotan-Eliaz, O., Sommer, K., & Rubin, Y. (2009). Multilingual groups: Effects of linguistic ostracism on felt rejection and anger, coworker attraction, perceived team potency, and creative performance. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 31(4), 363-375. Hitlan, R. T., Kelly, K. M., Schepman, S., Schneider, K. T., & Zárate, M. (2006). Language exclusion and the consequences of perceived ostracism in the workplace. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10(1), 56-70. Williams, K. D. (1997). Social ostracism. In R. M. Kowalski (Ed.), Aversive Interpersonal Behaviors (pp. 133 170). New York: Springer. Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. New York: Guilford Press. Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425-452. Williams, K. D. (2009). Ostracism: A temporal need-threat model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 275-314. Second, attributions and other variables could have been affected specifically by United States stereotypes of Chinese people. Thus, generalizations to other forms of foreign speakers should be made with caution. Third, a hidden camera was used, but there were no signals showing whether the camera was on or off. Researchers were unable to tell if an experiment had been properly videotaped until connecting it to the computer afterward. Although many successfully videotaped ostracism is lost in translation 41