Evaluating the interactive effects of responsible drinking messages and attentional bias on actual drinking behaviours

Similar documents
Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers

A new approach to measuring drinking cultures in Britain

Alcohol Insight 117. Roles of Alcohol in Intimate Partner Abuse. Key findings. Research team. Background. November 2014

Prescribing for substance misuse: alcohol detoxification. Clinical background

The effectiveness of attentional bias modification for substance use disorder symptoms in adults: a systematic review

Introducing: Saying When. An Innovative Knowledge Tool for Moderate Drinking

Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers

Thank you Dr. XXXX; I am going to be talking briefly about my EMA study of attention training in cigarette smokers.

Centre for Addictive Behaviours Research. Attitudes to tobacco and e- cigarettes, e-cig ads and ASMs

Pump up the volume! Can transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tdcs) enhance Cognitive Bias Modification in Addiction?

Alcohol misuse and cognitive impairment in older people: An exploratory study

Information and education

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Prevention in Addiction: Using Serious Games to (re)train Cognition in Dutch Adolescents

Can a new form of inhibitory training reduce heavy drinking?

Prepared by: David Hammond Department of Health Studies University of Waterloo

Poor impulse control and heightened attraction to alcohol-related imagery in repeat DUI offenders

Party Positive: A Comprehensive Harm-Reduction Program Targeting High-Risk Drinking

An experimental test of an alcohol expectancy challenge in mixed gender groups of young heavy drinkers

The alcohol treatment needs of violent and non-violent prisoners

Library of ALCOHOL HEALTH WARNING LABELS

Psychology Research Institute, University of Ulster, Northland Road, Londonderry, BT48 7JL, UK

EDRS. trends. bulletin. Alcohol use disorders amongst a group of regular ecstasy users. Key findings. july Introduction.

Archived at the Flinders Academic Commons:

Here s Looking at You, Bud: Alcohol-Related Memory Structures Predict Eye Movements for Social Drinkers With Low Executive Control

Retraining Automatic Action Tendencies Changes Alcoholic Patients Approach Bias for. Alcohol and Improves Treatment Outcome

Implicit priming of conflicting motivational orientations in heavy drinkers

Assessment Schedule 2015 Mathematics and Statistics (Statistics): Evaluate statistically based reports (91584)

ALCOHOLISM: CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH Vol. 30, No. 2 February 2006

DOES COGNITIVE BIAS MODIFICATION DURING ALCOHOL WITHDRAWAL REDUCE CRAVING?

Hazardous drinking in 2011/12: Findings from the New Zealand Health Survey

Alcohol Research UK Research Strategy

Trent Occupational Medicine Symposium October 2016 Why don t they just stop drinking?

21 units per week. 14 units per week

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

Liking, Craving, and Attentional Bias in Non- Dependent Drinkers

A SOCIAL MARKETING CAMPAIGN AGAINST HAZARDOUS AND HARMFUL DRINKING IN ENGLAND

COGNITIVE EFFECTS Avoidance of Alcohol-Related Stimuli Increases During the Early Stage of Abstinence in Alcohol-Dependent Patients

How is your work affecting you? The effects of exposure to risky drinking on perceptions of risk.

Foundations of Addictions

The Effect of Self-Control on Attentional Bias for Alcohol Cues in Male Heavy Drinkers 1

HEALTHIEST STATE MONTH TOOLKIT. for the WORKPLACE

Development and maintenance of evidence base

Responsible alcohol consumption

Are Alcohol Expectancies Associations? Comment on Moss and Albery (2009)

CHILD SEXUAL EXPLOITATION AWARENESS RAISING CAMPAIGN

Population based latent class analysis of drinking behaviour and related psychological problems and cognitive impairment.

In preparation : The impact of raising minimum alcohol prices in Saskatchewan, Canada: Improving public health while raising government revenue?

The Social Norms Review

DRINKAWARE BAROMETER REPORT

ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION IN YOUNG ADULTS: THE ROLE OF MULTISENSORY IMAGERY

APPENDIX 1. Examples of interventions that alter environmental stimuli to change health-related

The more you drink, the greater your risk of alcohol-related harm.

Supply of Alcohol to Young People Aged Under 18 Years. June 2017

Consumption of different types of alcohol and mortality

ACAMH Malta Addictions in Adolescents Conference

Nudges: A new instrument for public policy?

Brief tips on Using the Drink-less package for brief intervention for alcohol use disorders. The University of Sydney 2004

Running head: RANK-FRAMING IN SOCIAL NORMS INTERVENTIONS 1

Programme Name: Climate Schools: Alcohol and drug education courses

Overview. APPLIED PREVENTION & intervention STRATEGIES. APPLIED PREVENTION & intervention STRATEGIES

At university you celebrate the fact that somebody s bought a pen. SPECIALS: A preliminary qualitative

Alcohol Impact. Changing Alcohol-Related Behaviours. Lucy Henry Alcohol Impact Programme Manager

Alcohol Health Alliance UK response to the Chief Medical Officer s Alcohol Guidelines Review. March 2016

Computer tailored persuasion: effectiveness and side-effects. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

working with public health partners

The propositional basis of cue-controlled reward seeking

ICMC Labelling Effects for Alcoholic Drinks in Australia: Evidence from Three Experimental Designs. Doina Olaru and Wade Jarvis 3 RD JULY 2013

DOES THE SELF-CONTROL OF ALCOHOL USE become

Alcohol Indicators Report Executive Summary

Cannabis intoxication inhibits avoidance action tendencies: a field study in the Amsterdam coffee shops

Do Angry Women Choose Alcohol? ALYSSA ANDERSON, SHAREE HASKINS, LUKE GIBSON, KODY JURPIK

Alcohol Impact Monitoring and evaluation. Rachel Drayson Insight manager

Encouraging responsible gambling: Optimal wording for warning signs and public health campaigns

Alcohol mixed with energy drinks: Is there an evidence-base for future policy reform and regulation?

Minimum alcohol price policies in action: The Canadian Experience

The Society has considered the proposals contained in the consultation document and makes the following principal comments:

Know your limits: Labelling interventions to reduce alcohol consumption

1. Would you prefer $54 today, or $55 in 117 days? [ ] larger reward in the specified number of days

Tobacco dependence: Implications for service provision

Sophomore = Wise Fool? The Examination of Alcohol Consumption Throughout Class Years

Alcohol Portrayal on Television: A Study of Prime Time Television Programming

Addiction & Dependency Services in Bridgend County

Voluntary Pregnancy Warning Labels on Alcohol Presentation for the Australasian Drug and Alcohol Strategy Conference, 2-5 May 2017

Ocular Imaging of Attentional Bias Among College Students: Automatic and Controlled Processing of Alcohol- Related Scenes*

A Comprehensive Model of Stepped Care for Substance Misuse Prevention on a College Campus

SPECIAL ISSUE: THE MESSAGE AND THE MEDIA An Experimental Study on Imitation of Alcohol Consumption in Same-Sex Dyads

The Student s Guide to Social Neuroscience

THE ROLE OF THE CONTROLLED DRINKING GOAL IN THE RESPONSE TO ALCOHOL-RELATED HARM

Alcohol marketing and problem drinking. The effects of binge drinking and the starting age of consumption

AUSTRALIAN CHRONIC DISEASE PREVENTION ALLIANCE. Submission to Senate Standing Committee on Community Affairs

Higher Psychology RESEARCH REVISION

Submission to the Oireachtas Joint Committee on Health and Children

UKnowledge. University of Kentucky. Ramey G. Monem University of Kentucky, Recommended Citation

European Sponsorship Association

POSITION PAPER ON: HEALTH WARNING MESSAGES ON ALCOHOLIC BEVERAGES

Help us prioritise research into alcohol-related liver disease

Steady Ready Go! teady Ready Go. Every day, young people aged years become infected with. Preventing HIV/AIDS in young people

Alcohol use Among Adolescents in Europe

Renfrewshire Health Improvement Team. Training Programme

Transcription:

Evaluating the interactive effects of responsible drinking messages and attentional bias on actual drinking behaviours February 2017 Key findings Responsible drinking posters do not attract significant attention in bar environments Responsible drinking posters had no effect on the consumption of beer among study participants Researchers Daniel Frings, PhD, Guleser Eskian, BSc, Ian Albery, PhD and Tony Moss, PhD. All authors are affiliated with London South Bank University, School of Applied Sciences, Division of Psychology. Background Responsible drinking messages [s] are widely viewed as a key tool in reducing alcohol-related harms, commonly taking the form of posters displayed in places such as bars, bus stops and toilet cubicles. However, research suggests they may not be effective in reducing problematic consumption. Evaluations have found such messages can improve knowledge around alcohol and responsible drinking, and may also lead to responsible drinking intentions (Kalsher, et al., 1993; Fenaughty & MacKinnon, 2011; York et al., 2012). However, a review found little evidence that s changed actual drinking behaviour (Wakefield et al., 2010). Furthermore, recent research suggests that, in some contexts, viewing s can actually increase volumes consumed (e.g. Moss et al., 2015). This study looked at whether the context in which responsible drinking messages appeared influenced their effectiveness. It did so by observing how participants responded to posters in a bar environment compared a traditional psychology laboratory. Methods After completing measures of cognitive attentional bias, alcohol expectancies and prior drinking behaviour, participants were moved (with their eyes closed) to either a traditional psychology laboratory or to a purpose-built facility which simulates a public house. There, they were asked to complete a word search puzzle. In both contexts, participants could see bottles of beer (in reality, non-alcoholic) and a poster containing either a responsible drinking or a general fitness message (see Figure 1). Participants eye-movements were followed using a mobile eye-tracker which measured the amount of visual attention directed to the word search, the drinks bottles and the poster. After completing the word search, participants consumed as much or as little of the beer as they liked, purportedly as part of a taste preference task.

Figure 1: and control posters Findings Attention to Poster: Participants were less likely to look at the responsible drinking posters in the bar than in the traditional laboratory. This was not the case for the general fitness posters. Table 1: Mean visual attention directed at the poster across condition. Context condition Poster condition Number of glances at stimuli Lab 16.67 (21.89) Number of longer (>2 second) glances 0.83 (2.04) Total glance duration (seconds) 12.21 (13.61) 23.72 (20.35) 1.50 (1.5) 16.40 (21.11) Bar 23.54 (13.05) 0.96 (2.05) 12.39 (5.86) 14.00 (18.24) 0.24 (0.44) 5.89 (6.61) Attention to drinks and task: Participants were less likely to look at their drinks in the bar than in the traditional laboratory. When in a bar, they were even less likely to look at their drinks if a responsible drinking message was displayed. Table 2: Mean visual attention directed at the drinks across condition. Context condition Poster condition Number of glances at stimuli Lab 71.38 (55.48) Number of longer (>2 second) glances 4.79 (4.68) Total glance duration (seconds) 50.36 (36.66) 99.95 (74.83) 7.47 (10.29) 74.84 (93.51) Bar 62.24 (42.98) 1.90 (2.49) 74.44 (104.39) 33.77 (33.76) 1.86 (2.66) 32.85 (67.51)

Context, Poster and Consumption. Despite variations in attention, neither context nor poster type affected actual consumption. The data does suggest that viewing any poster may be linked to lower consumption; however, this is not conclusive. Strand 2: Positive alcohol expectancies were associated with the number of longer glances at the drinks. This was the only visual attention index that showed a statistically significant correlation with the alcohol expectancies scale. The total number of glances and the number of longer glances aimed at the drinks both positively correlated with higher AUDIT scores. However, neither AUDIT, AEQ scores nor alcohol Stroop Test scores predicted the amount of alcohol participants actually consumed. Increased attentional bias scores were associated with lower AUDIT scores. Table 4; Zero order correlations (full sample). 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1. TPT consumption ---.13 -.17 -.07 -.18 -.14 -.19.18.08.08 2. Drinks (all glances) --- -.07.11.01 -.15 -.01.52*** -.16 -.016 3. Drinks (longer glances) ---.15 -.03.34***.21 -.03 -.04 -.06 4. Drinks (total glance time) --- -.13 -.12 -.10 -.07.14 -.07 5.Poster (all of glances) ---.41***.53***.01.06.03 6. Poster (longer glances) ---.68*** -.15 -.12.14 7. Poster (total glance time) --- -.04 -.02.07 8. Wordsearch (all glances) ---.22*** -.10 9. Wordsearch (longer glances) ---.67*** 10. Wordsearch (total glance time) --- Notes: *** = p <.001; = p <.10 Implications From an applied perspective, these findings suggest responsible drinking posters will attract greater attention if placed in relatively simple contexts (e.g. toilet cubicles or bus shelters) rather than complex alcohol cue-laden environments such as bars. This may also apply to other behavioural messages, such as responsible gambling posters, which are often displayed on the side of gambling and fixed odds betting machines. However, this study found no evidence that posters affected actual consumption. This is in contrast with some of our own previous work, which suggested they might increase consumption (Moss et al., 2015). In the previous study the posters were visually complex, and contained messages from which the goal of responsible drinking needed to be inferred. The current study disentangled these effects by presenting a simpler poster, where only the text was manipulated and the message to drink responsibly was clear. The lack of substantive effects in this case suggests that the behavioural effects of responsible drinking messages may depend the content of those messages. The lack of contextual effects in this study is striking. Previous literature on the effects of context on, for example, expectancies has shown a mixed picture - with alcohol cues sometimes leading to more positive expectancies and sometimes leading to no difference. The current work highlights the challenges in understanding how, when and for whom contextual alcohol cues affect attention, expectancies and behaviour. Further work is clearly needed in this area.

Theoretical Implications The second strand of this research aimed to address theoretical questions about the extent to which attentional bias (measured here using the alcohol Stroop Test) affects visual attention to responsible drinking messages and alcohol consumption. It found that greater levels of attentional bias were linked to more long glances towards drinks. However, there was no relationship between this and shorter glances at drinks. As longer glances are likely to reflect a more conscious processing system (maintenance of attention) and shorter glances a more automated mode (initial attentional orientation), it appears that attentional biases may reflect a more conscious processing of relevant cues (see Field, Munafo and Franken, 2009; Noel et al, 2006; Field et al, 2004). Conclusions This project raises new questions for researchers, in particular, around the varied relationship between attentional bias scores and attention times given to alcohol cues, messages and actual drinks. From an applied perspective, it suggests that both placement and content influence the amount of attention responsible drinking messages receive. However, while less cue-laden environments may be more effective locations in terms of visual attention, in the real world such locations are usually not places where people typically drink. Therefore, the potential impact on behaviour which is already weak may be further lessened. Overall, while this study does not replicate earlier findings that posters may increase consumption, neither does it provide evidence that they are likely to meaningfully reduce the amount people drink. Therefore, even if levels of attention are affected by context and content, there remains little evidence that posters have any significant impact on behaviour. Further Information All correspondence to the first author via fringsd@lsbu.ac.uk or London South Bank University 102 Borough Road, London, SE1 0AA Acknowledgements The research team would like to thank Thomas Wilcox for his assistance early in the project, Ergoneers for advice on the D-lab software used to analyse eye-tracking data and, finally, acuity eye tracking solutions for their support with the eye-tracking hardware involved. References Brown, S.A., Christiansen, B.A. and Goldman, M.S.,1987. The Alcohol Expectancy Questionnaire: an instrument for the assessment of adolescent and adult alcohol expectancies. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48(5), pp.483-491. Field, M., Duka, T., Eastwood, B., Child, R., Santarcangelo, M., and Gayton, M. 2007. Experimental manipulation of attentional biases in heavy drinkers: Do the effects generalise? Psychopharmacology, 192(4), pp. 593 608. Kalsher, M.J., Clarke, S.W., and Wogalter, M.S. 1993. Communication of alcohol facts and hazards by a warning poster. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 13, pp. 78 90. Fenaughty, A.M., and MacKinnon, D.P. 1993. Immediate effects of the Arizona alcohol warning poster. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 12, pp. 69 77

Monk, R. L., and Heim, D. 2013. Environmental context effects on alcohol-related outcome expectancies, efficacy, and norms: a field study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(3), pp. 814-818. Morrison, P.M., Noel, N.E. and Ogle, R.L., 2012. Do angry women choose alcohol?. Addictive Behaviors, 37(8), pp.908-913. Moss, A.C., and Albery, I.P. 2009. A Dual-Process Model of the Alcohol Behaviour Link for Social Drinking. Psychological Bulletin, 135, pp. 516-530. Moss, A.C., Albery, I.P., Dyer, K.R., Frings, D., et al. 2015. The effects of responsible drinking messages on attention allocation and drinking behaviour. Addictive Behaviors, 44, pp. 94-101. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.035 Saunders, J.B., Aasland, O.G., Babor, T.F., de la Fuente, J.R. and Grant, M. 1993. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption. II. Addiction, 88, pp. 791-804. Schoenmakers, T.M., and Wiers, R.W. 2009. Craving and attentional bias respond differently to alcohol priming: A field study in the pub. European Addiction Research,16(1), pp. 9 16 Sharma, D., Albery, I.P. and Cook, C., 2001. Selective attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli in problem drinkers and non-problem drinkers. Addiction, 96(2), pp.285-295. York, V.K., Brannon, L.A., and Miller, M.M. 2012. Increasing the effectiveness of messages promoting responsible undergraduate drinking: Tailoring to personality and matching to context. Health Communication, 27(3), pp. 302 309. Download the Final Report Alcohol Research UK works to reduce levels of alcohol-related harm by ensuring that policy and practice can always be developed on the basis of research-based evidence. We are a lead funder of high quality research into the causes, impact and prevention of alcohol-related harm and are the only organisation exclusively dedicated to building an evidence base in this area. Read more reports at www.alcoholresearchuk.org Alcohol Research UK, 83 Victoria Street, London SW1H 0HW Tel: 020 3585 4155 Registered charity 1140287