Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction. Paul A. Dudchenko

Similar documents
Place fields and the cognitive map

Navigation: Inside the Hippocampus

Head direction maps remain stable despite grid map fragmentation

FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE RAT HEAD-DIRECTION CIRCUIT

From the GPS to HM: Place Cells, Grid Cells, and Memory

Y a-t il un pilote dans le cerveau? Données récentes Sur les bases neuronales de l orientation spatiale

EDVARD MOSER AND THE GRID CELLS MICROSTRUCTURE OF A SPATIAL MAP IN THE ENTORHINAL CORTEX

Distinct Ensemble Codes in Hippocampal Areas CA3 and CA1

The Head Direction Signal: Origins and Sensory-Motor Integration

How do spatial learning and memory occur in the brain? Coordinated learning of entorhinal grid cells and hippocampal place cells

Persistent Firing Supported by an Intrinsic Cellular Mechanism in a Component of the Head Direction System

Chapter 6: Hippocampal Function In Cognition. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Backward Shift of Head Direction Tuning Curves of the Anterior Thalamus: Comparison with CA1 Place Fields

Some Thoughts on Place Cells and the Hippocampus

Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive

Cue Control and Head Direction Cells

SPATIAL PROCESSING IN THE BRAIN: The Activity of Hippocampal Place Cells

Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping

Space and Context in the Rodent Hippocampal Region

A Neural Systems Analysis of Adaptive Navigation

ASSOCIATIVE MEMORY AND HIPPOCAMPAL PLACE CELLS

Grid Cell Firing May Arise From Interference of Theta Frequency Membrane Potential Oscillations in Single Neurons

The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience

Encoding Spatial Context: A Hypothesis on the Function of the Dentate Gyrus-Hilus System

of specic visual patterns. The activity of these neurons implicitly represents properties like agent-landmark distance and egocentric orientation to v

Discordance of Spatial Representation in Ensembles of Hippocampal Place Cells

A. David Redish and David S. Touretzky y. Computer Science Department and. Carnegie Mellon University. Abstract

The Effects of Inhibiting Neurons in Layer II of the Medial Entorhinal Cortex on Hippocampal Place Cells in CA1 and CA3

Interaction between the Postsubiculum and Anterior Thalamus in the Generation of Head Direction Cell Activity

REPRESENTATION OF OBJECT-IN-CONTEXT WITHIN MOUSE HIPPOCAMPAL NEURONAL ACTIVITY. Herborg Nanna Ásgeirsdóttir. A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of

Theta-Modulated Head Direction Cells in the Rat Anterior Thalamus

Hector Penagos. Licenciatura en Ffsica, Universidad de las Americas-Puebla, 2000

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Place Cells, Grid Cells, and the Brain s Spatial Representation System

The cognitive architecture of spatial navigation: hippocampal and striatal contributions

Place cells, spatial maps and the population code for memory Stefan Leutgeb, Jill K Leutgeb, May-Britt Moser and Edvard I Moser

Benjamin J. Clark, Ph.D.

Head Direction Cells in the Postsubiculum Do Not Show Replay of Prior Waking Sequences During Sleep

Computation by Oscillations: Implications of Experimental Data for Theoretical Models of Grid Cells

Changes in reward contingency modulate the trial-to-trial variability of hippocampal place cells

Self-organizing continuous attractor networks and path integration: one-dimensional models of head direction cells

Informationsverarbeitung im zerebralen Cortex

Anatomy of the Hippocampus

Sachin S. Deshmukh, 1,3 * Jeremy L. Johnson, 1 and James J. Knierim 1,2,3 INTRODUCTION HIPPOCAMPUS 22: (2012)

Mapping Self-Motion Information in the Posterior Parietal Cortex Jeremiah Palmerston Dr. Douglas Nitz June, 2012

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

Mechanosensation. Central Representation of Touch. Wilder Penfield. Somatotopic Organization

Neural processing of Spatial Information: What we know about Place cells and what they can tell us about Presence

An Investigation of the. Postsubiculum s role in Spatial. Cognition

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators

A Computational Model of Rodent Spatial Learning and Some Behavioral. experiments.

Integration of the Sensory Inputs to Place Cells: What, Where, Why, and How?

NST II Psychology NST II Neuroscience (Module 5)

Sensation, Part 1 Gleitman et al. (2011), Chapter 4

The motor regulator. 2) The cerebellum

A sense of direction in human entorhinal cortex

Brain anatomy and artificial intelligence. L. Andrew Coward Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia

MAHARISHI VEDIC ARCHITECTURE BACKGROUND AND SUMMARY OF SCIENTIFIC RESEARCH

Neural Representation of Space in Rats and Robots

The Role of the Hippocampus in Solving the Morris Water Maze

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Two cortical systems for memoryguided

Hippocampal global remapping for different sensory modalities in flying bats

A cognitive map for object memory in the hippocampus

Robust self-localisation and navigation based on hippocampal place cells

A sketch of the central nervous system and its origins. MIT 9.14 Classes 31

Modelling spatial recall, mental imagery and neglect

9.14 Class 32 Review. Limbic system

CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation

WHERE AM I? WHERE AM I GOING?

An Allocentric Spatial Model for the Hippocampal Cognitive Map

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Hippocampal diencephalic cingulate networks for memory and emotion: An anatomical guide

Neuroscience Letters

Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Humboldt University of Berlin, Philippstr. 13, Haus 6, Berlin, Germany 2

The motor regulator. 2) The cerebellum

Spatial View Cells and the Representation of Place in the Primate Hippocampus

Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper. Today: Wolfger von der Behrens

Neuroscience Optional Lecture. The limbic system the emotional brain. Emotion, behaviour, motivation, long-term memory, olfaction

Motor Functions of Cerebral Cortex

Memory: Computation, Genetics, Physiology, and Behavior. James L. McClelland Stanford University

What do you notice?

Objects and Landmarks: Hippocampal Place Cells Respond Differently to Manipulations of Visual Cues Depending on Size, Perspective, and Experience

Supplementary Information Supplementary Table 1. Quantitative features of EC neuron dendrites

Beta Oscillations and Hippocampal Place Cell Learning during Exploration of Novel Environments Stephen Grossberg 1

Limbic system outline

Spatial Learning and Localization in Animals: A Computational Model and Behavioral Experiments

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory

Toward a spiking model of the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex

Hippocampal Place Cells, Context, and Episodic Memory

Development of a Touch-Screen-Based Paradigm for Assessing Working Memory in the Mouse

Neurophysiology of systems

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

Measuring distributed properties of neural representations beyond the decoding of local variables implications for cognition

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Brain Mechanisms of Emotion 1 of 6

Synaptic plasticity and hippocampal memory

Hippocampal and Neocortical Interactions During Context Discrimination: Electrophysiological Evidence From the Rat

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Transcription:

Space and brain: a tabula rasa for the sense of direction Paul A. Dudchenko University of Stirling Psychology, School of Natural Sciences Stirling, FK9 4LA United Kingdom Phone: +44 131 650 3531 p.a.dudchenko@stir.ac.uk mailto:p.a.dudchenko@stir.ac.uk

Two recent studies have shown that neurons which fire in a compass-like way head direction cells are present before rat pups open their eyes. Upon eye opening, the firing direction of these cells is anchored rapidly to visual landmarks. Let us then suppose the mind to be, as we say, white paper, void of all characters, without any ideas: How comes it to be furnished? Locke (1690) [1] A central question in philosophy has recently become a question for neuroscience: what do neural circuits look like before experience with the world? For Locke, the mind before experience was a tabula rasa a blank slate. For Bjerknes et al. [2] and Tan et al. [3], the mammalian brain possesses a representation of direction even before the animal sees the outside world. Rats are functionally blind until about two weeks after birth, at which point their eyes open. Bjerknes et al. and Tan et al. exploited this transition to record from a neural representation of direction in the rat pup brain both immediately before and after the animal opened its eyes. The representation they recorded from, the head direction cell system, is comprised of neurons whose firing is tuned to specific directions in the environment. An individual head direction (HD) cell typically fires in one direction in the animal s environment, with a range of approximately 90 degrees, and does not fire when the animal faces other directions [4]. However, different HD cells fire in different preferred directions, and these cells appear to form a ring attractor that is anchored to landmarks in the environment [5-7]. Previous work has shown that spatially tuned neuronal activity appears early in the rat s development [8, 9]. Specifically, HD cells show stable directionality at post-natal day 16 of development. Place cells neurons in the hippocampus that fire in specific locations in the 2

environment are also present from day 16 of development. Grid cells neurons in the entorhinal cortex and adjacent brain regions that show grid-like tuning fields in the environment appear at around the same time, but their spatial tuning becomes much sharper by days 24-30 of development. The observation that stable place cell firing precedes stable grid cell fields implies that the former do not necessarily require the latter [9, 10]. Do head direction cells require experience to exhibit their compass-like firing? These neurons show a remarkable signal-to-noise ratio, and are found in an interconnected series of brain regions extending from the dorsal tegmental nuclei to the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) [11, 12]. Changes in their firing direction correlate with changes in spatial behaviour [13, 14], and damage to the brain regions in which head direction cells are found produce impairments in spatial cognition [15, 16]. The results Bjerknes et al. and Tan et al. show that direction firing precedes visual experience. Bjerkenes et al. recorded from the presubiculum, parasubiculum, and medial entorhinal cortex of different rat pups between post-natal days 11-15, prior to the opening of their eyes. They found head direction cells in each of these brain areas. The head direction cells, however, showed significant drift in firing directions across the first and second half of a 10 minute recording session, and between adjacent recording sessions. Thus, directionally tuned firing was evident, but was not tied strongly to the environment. Simultaneous recordings of more than one HD cell showed that the drift in the preferred direction of one cells was similar to that of any other cell. Thus, the cells behaved in the same way, consistent with the idea that head direction cells form an attractor network [17]. 3

Next Bjerkenes et al. recorded from the HD cells just after the rat pups opened their eyes. A basic property of HD cells is that their preferred directions are anchored to visual landmarks in the environment. The author found that this was also the case with the rat pups, indicating that the head direction cell system can be anchored to visual landmarks as soon as they are visible. Tan and colleagues found similar results. They recorded from the dorsal presubiculum and the anterodorsal thalamus in rat pups aged post-natal day 12-20. Before the pups eyes opened, head direction cells were observed in both brain areas. The cells showed some stability in firing directions within sessions, but this stability was much better once the eyes opened. Interestingly, the percentage of cells showing directional firing increased dramatically once the eyes opened, particularly in the anterodorsal thalamus. Finally, Tan and colleagues also observed that the firing direction of head direction cells was anchored to a visual landmark in a reliable way about a day after the eyes opened. These results suggest that the head direction circuit may rely on some type of non-visual information prior to visual experience, but that visual experience stabilises the representation, and may be critical for the development of a subset of the head direction cells found in the anterodorsal thalamus. Together, these two studies make several contributions. First, they show that the head direction cell system does not require visual input to form. HD cells develop prior to the animal s ability to see the world, and this is consistent with the view that vestibular or other non-visual inputs contribute to the basic directional tuning of these cells [18]. Second, it s clear the link between HD cells is evident from the start, thus suggesting that the cells function as a coherent network. Third, before visual access to the world the stability of the 4

directional firing, while not entirely absent, is poor. Fourth, when vision does come online, the stability of the cells is much improved, and some HD cells particularly those in the anterodorsal thalamus appear to develop directional firing. Finally, the capacity for HD cells to form an associational anchor to visual landmarks appears within a day of access to these landmarks. Thus, a blank slate or compass is present in the mammalian brain prior to experience with the visual world. With vision, this directional slate is rapidly honed and an associational process allows this internal representation to become linked to the contents of the outside world. 5

References 1. Locke, J. (1690). An essay concerning human understanding. (Adelaide: ebooks@adelaide). 2. Bjerknes, T.L., Langston, R.F., Kruge, I.U., Moser, E.I., Moser, M-B (2015). Coherence among head direction cells before eye opening in rat pups. Current Biology 25, 1-6. 3. Tan, H.M., Bassett, J.P., O Keefe, J., Cacucci, F., Wills, T.J. (2015). The development of the head direction system before eye-opening in the rat. Current Biology (in press). 4. Taube, J.S., Muller, R.U., Ranck, J.B. (1990). Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis. Journal of Neuroscience 10, 420-435. 5. Skaggs, W.E., Knierim, J.J., Kudrimoti, H.S., McNaughton, B.L. (1995). A model of the neural basis of the rat's sense of direction. In: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems Vol 7. 6. Redish, A.D., Elga, A.N., Touretzky, D.S. (1996). A coupled attractor model of the rodent head direction system. Network. Comput. Neural Syst. 7, 671-685. 7. Zhang, K. (1996). Representation of spatial orientation by the intrinsic dynamics of the head-direction ensemble: a theory. Journal of Neuroscience 16, 2112-2126. 8. Langston, R.F., Ainge, J.A., Couey, J.J., Canto, C.B., Bjerknes, T.L., Witter, M.P., Moser. E.I., Moser, M-B. (2010). Development of the spatial representation system in the rat. Science, 238, 1576-1580. 6

9. Wills, T.J., Cacucci, F., Burgess, N., O Keefe, J. (2010). Development of the hippocampal cognitive map in preweanling rats. Science 328, 1573-1576. 10. Bush, D., Barry, C., Burgess, N. (2014). What do grid cells contribute to place cell firing? Trends in Neuroscience 37, 136-145. 11. Taube, J.S. (2007). The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30, 181-207. 12. Boccara, C.N., Sargolini, F., Thoresen, V.H., Solstad, T., Witter, M.P., Moser, E.I., Moser, M-B. (2010). Grid cells in the pre- and parasubiculum. Nature Neuroscience 13, 987-994. 13. Dudchenko, P.A., Taube, J.S. (1997). Correlation between head direction cell activity and spatial behavior on a radial arm maze. Behavioral Neuroscience 111, 3-19. 14. van der Meer, M., Richmond, Z., Braga, R.M., Wood, E.R., Dudchenko, P.A. (2010). Evidence for the use of an internal sense of direction in homing. Behavioral Neuroscience 124, 164-169. 15. Vann, S.D. (2010). Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory. Neuropsychologia 48, 2316-2327. 16. Vann, S.D., Aggleton, J.P. (2004). The mammillary bodies: two memory systems in one? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5, 35-44. 17. Yoganarasimha, D., Yu, X., Knierim, J.J. (2006). Head direction cell representations maintain internal coherence during conflicting proximal and distal cue rotations: comparison with hippocampal place cells. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 622-631. 7

18. Stackman, R.W., Taube, J.S. (1997). Firing properties of head direction cells in the rat anterior thalamic nucleus: dependence on vestibular input. Journal of Neuroscience 17, 4349-4358. 8

Figure legend Figure 1. Development of the head direction cell system. The results of Bjerknes et al. [2] and Tan et al. [3] show that rat pups have head direction cells before the eyes are opened. Though directional, these cells appear to show withinrecording session drift in firing directions. Once the eyes are opened, these cells are much more stable, and are anchored to visual landmarks in the environment. 9

beginning of session 90 o end of session 90 o 180 o 0 o 180 o 0 o eyes closed 270 o 90 o 270 o 90 o 180 o 0 o 180 o 0 o eyes open 270 o 270 o