Vision Research. Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Suzanne C. Belmore a, Steven K.

Similar documents
OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

Color scaling of discs and natural objects at different luminance levels

Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance

Variations in normal color vision. II. Unique hues

Asymmetries in the time-course of chromatic adaptation and the significance of contrast

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

Fitting the world to the mind: Transforming images to mimic perceptual adaptation

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations

Surface Color Perception under Different Illuminants and Surface Collections

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Decline of the McCollough effect by orientation-specific post-adaptation exposure to achromatic gratings

COLOUR CONSTANCY: A SIMULATION BY ARTIFICIAL NEURAL NETS

SENSES: VISION. Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014

Psychophysical estimates of opponent-process response functions*

Nonlocal interactions in color perception: nonlinear processing of chromatic signals from remote inducers

Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components

Adaptation and perceptual norms

Human colour perception and its adaptation

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2003

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

An empirical explanation of color contrast

Visual discrimination and adaptation using non-linear unsupervised learning

A THEORY OF MCCOLLOUGH EFFECT AND. CHUN CHIANG Institute of Physics, Academia Sinica

3/15/06 First Class Meeting, Pick Up First Readings, Discuss Course, MDF Overview Lecture (Chs. 1,2,3)

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II

On the Perception of Brightness and Contrast of Variegated Backgrounds

Biological Bases of Behavior. 6: Vision

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations

1.4 MECHANISMS OF COLOR VISION. Trichhromatic Theory. Hering s Opponent-Colors Theory

Adaptation and perceptual norms in color vision

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

Color perception PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 17. Importance of color

The cone inputs to the unique-hue mechanisms

Variations in normal color vision. V. Simulations of adaptation to natural color environments

= add definition here. Definition Slide

A MULTI-STAGE COLOR MODEL REVISITED: IMPLICATIONS FOR A GENE THERAPY CURE FOR RED-GREEN COLORBLINDNESS

Opponent theory PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 18. Trichromatic theory

The Perceptual Experience

Simulations of adaptation and color appearance in observers with varying spectral sensitivity

OPTO Physiology Of Vision II

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

Chromatic discrimination: differential contributions from two adapting fields

Supplementary Materials

Contrast magnitude and polarity effects on color filling-in along cardinal color axes

Toward the neural causes of human visual perception and behavior

Processing in the probed-sinewave paradigm is likely retinal

Supplemental Information: Task-specific transfer of perceptual learning across sensory modalities

Mechanisms of color constancy under nearly natural viewing (color appearance adaptation natural scenes retinex)

Senses are transducers. Change one form of energy into another Light, sound, pressure, etc. into What?

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Consciousness and Blindsight

Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS

The early stages in the processing of chromatic information by

SEMANTIC AND PERCEPTUAL REPRESENTATIONS OF COLOR

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Attention enhances feature integration

Perceived Color, Induction Effects, and Opponent-Response Mechanisms

Behavioral measurement of background adaptation in infants. RONALD M. HANSEN

The effect of contrast intensity and polarity in the achromatic watercolor effect

Response latencies to chromatic. and achromatic visual stimuli

Adaptation and the Phenomenology of Perception

Scotopic hue percepts in natural scenes

Surround articulation. I. Brightness judgments

Division of Social, Behavioral and Human Sciences University of Washington - Tacoma Tacoma, WA (253)

Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth

Dependence of Perceived Purity of a Chromatic Stimulus on Saturation Adaptation

LEA Color Vision Testing

Color Perception Is Mediated by a Plastic Neural Mechanism that Is Adjustable in Adults

Transducer model produces facilitation from opposite-sign flanks

A Psychophysical Technique For Measuring Cone Photopigment Bleaching

Unconscious Color Priming Occurs at Stimulus- Not Percept- Dependent Levels of Processing Bruno G. Breitmeyer, 1 Tony Ro, 2 and Neel S.

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

The New Colour Scales based on Saturation, Vividness, Blackness and Whiteness

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

Temporal properties of the chromatic and achromatic Craik O Brien Cornsweet effect

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

Gaze direction modulates visual aftereffects in depth and color

The Color Between Two Others

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Construction of the Visual Image

2. METHODS. 2.1 Apparatus

Effects of attention on motion repulsion

The perception of motion transparency: A signal-to-noise limit

Practice Test Questions

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Latency differences and the flash-lag effect

Lighta part of the spectrum of Electromagnetic Energy. (the part that s visible to us!)

Electronic Letters to: Electronic letters published: Multiple Mechanisms in the VEP 23 July von :24

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

Transcription:

Vision Research 51 (2011) 362 366 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Suzanne C. Belmore a, Steven K. Shevell a,b,c, a Psychology, The University of Chicago, 940 East 57th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, USA b Ophthalmology & Visual Science, The University of Chicago, 940 East 57th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, USA c Institute for Mind & Biology, The University of Chicago, 940 East 57th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, USA article info abstract Article history: Received 19 August 2010 Received in revised form 23 November 2010 Available online 3 December 2010 Keywords: Chromatic adaptation Equilibrium yellow Equilibrium hue Long-term adaptation Very-long-term (VLT) chromatic adaptation results from exposure to an altered chromatic environment for days or weeks. Color shifts from VLT adaptation are observed hours or days after leaving the altered environment. Short-term chromatic adaptation, on the other hand, results from exposure for a few minutes or less, with color shifts measured within seconds or a few minutes after the adapting light is extinguished; recovery to the pre-adapted state is complete in less than an hour. Here, both types of adaptation were combined. All adaptation was to reddish-appearing long-wavelength light. Shifts in unique yellow were measured following adaptation. Previous studies demonstrate shifts in unique yellow due to VLT chromatic adaptation, but shifts from short-term chromatic adaptation to comparable adapting light can be far greater than from VLT adaptation. The question considered here is whether the color shifts from VLT adaptation are cumulative with large shifts from short-term adaptation or, alternatively, does simultaneous short-term adaptation eliminate color shifts caused by VLT adaptation. The results show the color shifts from VLT and short-term adaptation together are cumulative, which indicates that both short-term and very-long-term chromatic adaptation affect color perception during natural viewing. Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Chromatic adaptation from viewing light of selective wavelengths causes reversible changes in color appearance. Chromatic adaptation for a few seconds or minutes (short-term adaptation) or for days or weeks (very-long-term adaptation) has been studied experimentally, but the work here is the first to study shifts in color appearance when both types of adaptation are combined. Short-term adaptation is a well-studied phenomenon. It results from exposure of 15 min or less to a chromatic light. The adapting effect decays within seconds or minutes (Jameson, Hurvich, & Varner, 1979; Rinner & Gegenfurtner, 2000). Many studies support a two-process model of short-term chromatic adaptation (Cicerone, Krantz, & Larmier, 1975; Drum, 1981; Guth, Massof, & Benzschawel, 1980; Hayhoe & Wenderoth, 1991; Jameson & Hurvich, 1972; Larimer, 1981; Shevell, 1978; Ware & Cowan, 1982). Very-long-term (VLT) chromatic adaptation has been little studied until recently. It results from exposure to an altered chromatic environment for an hour or more each day over many days or weeks. Several adapting techniques have been used, including Corresponding author at: Institute for Mind & Biology, The University of Chicago, 940 East 57th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, USA. E-mail address: shevell@uchicago.edu (S.K. Shevell). natural viewing with chromatically selective (typically longwave-transmitting) lenses (Eisner & Enoch, 1982; Neitz, Carroll, Yamauchi, Neitz, & Williams, 2002; Yamauchi et al., 2002) or with a spectrally filtered illuminant that predominantly emits long wavelengths (Belmore & Shevell, 2008; Neitz et al., 2002). VLT adaptation can be induced also by viewing a long-wavelength grating on a video display for an hour each day (a technique suggested to us by Dr. J. Neitz). Different VLT-adapting methods and durations produce similar shifts in unique yellow, which are maintained for days or even weeks after the end of the adapting period (Belmore & Shevell, 2008; Neitz et al., 2002). The color shifts from VLT adaptation cannot be fully explained by a gain (von Kries) theory of adaptation (Belmore & Shevell, 2008). As with short-term chromatic adaptation, VLT adaptation causes shifts in unique yellow consistent with a two-process model. VLT and short-term adaptation often have been distinguished by the temporal properties of the color shifts resulting from the adaptation. VLT adaptation is characterized by color changes (i) that can increase in magnitude with recurring exposure to the adapting light over several days or longer and (ii) that persist for tens of hours, days or even weeks after chromatic adaptation ends. Short-term adaptation typically is assumed to reach its asymptotic impact within an hour or less of viewing the adapting stimulus, 0042-6989/$ - see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.11.011

S.C. Belmore, S.K. Shevell / Vision Research 51 (2011) 362 366 363 and any effect of adaptation is presumed (though seldom demonstrated) to dissipate within an hour. The different time courses for the influence of VLT versus short-term adaptation suggest distinct neural mechanisms, which may be isolated to a significant degree with carefully chosen adapting durations and testing periods. Both mechanisms, however, conceivably may drive a common, higherlevel neural pathway that mediates the color shifts caused by either type of adaptation. Even though each type of adaptation may be initiated by a separate mechanism with distinct temporal properties, if they share a later nonlinear neural pathway then one type of adaptation may completely dominate the other. If so, VLT adaptation may not affect color perception in natural viewing when both short-term and VLT adaptation occur simultaneously. In general, the relative strength of VLT versus short-term chromatic adaptation is not easily determined. Under some comparable conditions, for example exposure to a long-wavelength adapting light in the laboratory, short-term chromatic adaptation can produce a shift in equilibrium (neither reddish nor greenish) yellow far greater than that produced by VLT adaptation. Consider a test stimulus that is an admixture of 540 and 660 nm light. With the level of the 540 nm light fixed, the observer sets the radiance of the 660 nm component so the test appears equilibrium yellow. Introducing short-term long-wavelength chromatic adapting light can increase the required 660 nm radiance by a full log unit, but VLT adaptation may raise the 660 nm level by as little as 0.1 log unit. Laboratory measurements of VLT adaptation, however, do not extend for a sufficiently long period of time to examine VLT-adapting mechanisms that may depend on light stimulation over months or longer. Also, other color-measurement paradigms may yield a different relative contribution from short-term versus VLT adaptation. A challenge, therefore, is to assess whether color shifts from short-term and VLT adaptation are cumulative without having to determine a baseline state of color perception without VLT adaptation. The aim of this study was to assess whether the color shifts found in laboratory experiments of VLT adaptation are important for natural viewing. This can be determined without knowing the absolute magnitude of the color changes that may result from VLT adaptation. The critical question is whether the color shifts induced by VLT laboratory adaptation are the same, with or without short-term adaptation that alone causes large color shifts; that is, are the color shifts from short-term and VLT adaptation cumulative even when short-term adaptation causes a much larger change in color appearance than VLT adaptation? If so, then laboratory studies of VLT adaptation reveal a neural process of color perception whose effect survives even in the presence of a far larger color shifts due to short-term adaptation, and thus they reveal an important neural process in natural viewing. 2. Methods 2.1. Overview of procedure The experiment was divided into two contiguous time periods: a baseline period followed by VLT chromatic adaptation (Fig. 1a and b). During both periods, the observer made equilibrium-yellow measurements at approximately the same time each day. Two sets of baseline measurements were taken during the first 6 days of the experiment. One set immediately followed dark adaptation, and then another set was taken during short-term chromatic adaptation. At the end of the testing session on the sixth day, the observer began the VLT chromatic adaptation, which lasted 1 h. On the following (seventh) day, testing began at least 22 h after the end of the previous day s VLT adaptation. This sequence of taking equilibrium-yellow measurements and then, immediately afterward, 1 h of VLT adaptation continued for 12 or 14 days. 2.2. Adapting stimuli The stimulus for VLT chromatic adaptation was a grating pattern (Fig. 2a) presented for 1 h each day on a cathode ray tube (CRT) video display in an otherwise dark, windowless room. The screen displayed randomly located, parallel lines of some orientation; a new set of lines at a different randomly chosen orientation was presented every 5 s. The width of each line was approximately 10 min of arc. The line density covered 25% of the area of the display. Judd chromaticity coordinates for the lines were (x = 0.60, y = 0.35) at 22.4 cd/m 2. The display, a carefully calibrated Sony Trinitron monitor (GDM-F520), was controlled by a Macintosh G4 computer. The CRT display was set via software for 1360 1024 pixel resolution and a refresh rate of 75 Hz noninterlaced. Short-term chromatic adaptation took place during the testing session as part of the test-stimulus presentation, using a Maxwellian-view optical system. The short-term adapting field was a 660 nm, 2.7 diameter disk at 100 td. At this light level, the additive and gain components that account for color perception under short-term adaptation are well differentiated (Shevell, 1982). Initially, the observer viewed this adapting field for 3 min; later, it (a) Two time periods of experiment (b) 6 days Baseline Fixed start of daily cycle 12-14 days Very-long-term adaptation Fixed start of daily cycle Testing (includes short-term adaptation) Testing (includes short-term adaptation) VLT CA (1 hour) Normal viewing ( 23 hours) Normal viewing ( 22 hours) Fig. 1. (a) The two consecutive experimental time periods during which equilibrium-yellow measurements were obtained. (b) The daily cycle of testing, VLT adaptation and normal viewing. The daily cycle for each observer began at approximately the same time each day throughout the experiment.

364 S.C. Belmore, S.K. Shevell / Vision Research 51 (2011) 362 366 (a) 2.3. Test stimulus (b) 2.7 λ540 0.8 1.3 i.d. o.d. λ540 The test field for assessing equilibrium yellow was an annulus of inner outer diameter 0.8 1.3 presented using the Maxwellian-view system. The observer viewed the field with the right eye. The test was composed of an admixture of 540 nm and 660 nm lights. The observer adjusted the level of the 660 nm light so the test appeared equilibrium (neither reddish nor greenish) yellow. The 540 nm component was fixed in retinal illuminance on each trial; the levels of 540 nm tested within each session were 0.5, 1.0, 1.5 and 2.0 log td (a 2.5 log td level was added for the combined-adaptation measurements). For the combined-adaptation condition, or short-term adaptation alone, short-term chromatic adaptation was maintained with a repeating 4 s cycle of stimulus presentation. The test field was presented alone for 1 s and then was presented superimposed on the adapting field for 3 s. The observer judged the color of the test during only the 1 s test-alone presentation. The stimulus presentation sequence is diagrammed in Fig. 2b. 2.4. Procedure 3 minutes dark adaptation 3 minutes short-term chromatic adaptation 1 second equilibrium yellow judgment time 3 seconds readapt cycle repeats Fig. 2. (a) Example of CRT display for very-long-term chromatic adaptation (see text). (b) Test-stimulus presentation sequence for conditions that included shortterm chromatic adaptation (see text). was presented as part of the test-field presentation sequence (see Section 2.3 below), which ensured that short-term adaptation was maintained throughout those equilibrium-yellow measurements. log td 3.4 3.2 3 2.8 2.6 2.4 2.2 2 1.8 1.6 1.4 1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Obs: GA 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2 2.2 2.4 2.6 λ540 log td In testing sessions, unique yellow measurements began at the lowest 540 nm light level and ended with the highest. The observer used a game pad to adjust the radiance of the 660 nm light in the admixture to achieve a percept that was neither reddish nor greenish. There were five trials at each 540 nm level in the test; measurements from the five trials were averaged. This protocol is similar to one used by Shevell (1982) in studies of color perception with a short-term chromatic adapting field. Both observers were practiced at setting equilibrium yellow before the start of data collection. Each set of measurements began with 5 min of dark adaptation. Then measurements were taken for VLT adaptation alone (or, during the baseline period, for dark adaptation). There was 1 min of further dark adaptation before measurements were taken at each new level of 540 nm light in the test field. An additional 3 min of 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8 2 2.2 2.4 2.6 λ540 log td Short- + very- long-term chromatic adaptation Short-term chromatic adaptation alone Very-long-term chromatic adaptation alone Dark adaptation Obs: MO Fig. 3. Average measurements of equilibrium yellow during the baseline and very-long-term chromatic adaptation experimental periods. Baseline measurements: dark adaptation (solid line) and short-term chromatic adaptation alone (dotted line). Shifts in unique yellow due to introducing very-long-term adaptation: VLT adaptation alone (dashed line); VLT together with short-term adaptation (dash-dot line). The amount of 660 nm test light (vertical axis) needed to establish equilibrium yellow is given as a function of the amount of 540 nm test light (horizontal axis).

S.C. Belmore, S.K. Shevell / Vision Research 51 (2011) 362 366 365 dark adaptation preceded the following measurements that included short-term chromatic adaptation. An initial 3 min period of exposure to the short-term chromatic adapting field preceded short-term-adaptation measurements. 2.5. Observers Observers were two University of Chicago undergraduate students. They were naïve about the purpose of the experiment and were paid for their participation. Both observers had normal color vision as assessed by Rayleigh matching using a Neitz anomaloscope. Consent forms were completed in accordance with the policy of the University of Chicago s institutional review board. 3. Results Either VLT or short-term chromatic adaptation shifted color perception. Following either type of adaptation, the amount of 660 nm light in the test field had to be increased for the percept of equilibrium yellow, compared to dark adaptation. These measurements are shown in Fig. 3 for each level of 540 nm light in the test field (horizontal axis), for dark adaptation (solid line), VLT adaptation alone (dashed line) and short-term adaptation alone (dotted line). Each plotted value is the average of measurements made on many days. Error bars show standard errors of the mean, though most of them are smaller than the plotted point so are not visible. Each panel shows results for a different observer. The main aim of this experiment was to compare the color shifts with combined VLT and short-term chromatic adaptation to shifts with either type of adaptation alone. Measurements with combined-adaptation (dash-dot line, Fig. 3) fall above the shifts found with only short-term chromatic adaptation (dotted line); the results show that adding VLT chromatic adaptation increased the color shifts beyond those due to short-term adaptation alone (p < 0.01 for each observer by Tukey HSD test, comparing the four types of adaptation: dark, short-term alone, VLT alone and combined). VLT adaptation alone (dashed line) also caused a color shift compared to dark adaptation (solid line; p < 0.01 for each observer by Tukey HSD test). Furthermore, the magnitude of the average shift from VLT adaptation alone compared to dark adaptation (0.12 log td for Obs. G.A., 0.09 log td for M.O.) was comparable to the shift from combined-adaptation compared to short-term adaptation alone (0.08 log td for G.A., 0.09 log td for M.O.). Thus, the shifts in equilibrium yellow produced by short-term and VLT adaptation together were cumulative; this shows that VLT adaptation can significantly affect color perception in natural viewing where, of course, VLT and short-term adaptation operate simultaneously. 4. Discussion To our knowledge, there is no previous comparison of the color shifts from VLT chromatic adaptation combined with short-term adaptation, to the color shifts from short-term adaptation alone. The results here showed that the color shifts caused by short-term and VLT chromatic adaptation together were cumulative. The incremental shift due to VLT adaptation was very similar whether or not the observer also was exposed to short-term adaptation. While the results from short-term and VLT adaptation are additive, this additivity does not imply separate, independent neural mechanisms. Consider for example a single linear process, such that introducing adapting stimulus A results in a color shift of magnitude S, regardless of the initial adapted state (and thus of any color shift already induced) when A is introduced. This single mechanism could account for the cumulative color shifts found with combined short-term and VLT adaptation. Nonetheless, distinct short-term and VLT neural mechanisms are supported by other properties of the color shifts they induce. Color shifts from VLT adaptation, but not short-term adaptation, increase with additional adaptation over many days; and the color shifts from VLT adaptation, but not short-term adaptation, persist for days after the adaptation ends (Neitz et al., 2002). The question of independent mechanisms for each type of adaptation, however, is not the focus here, because even fully independent mechanisms at one stage of the visual system may interfere with each other. As mentioned earlier, if short-term and VLT chromatic-adaptation mechanisms converge on a later nonlinear neural mechanism or some other bottleneck, then short-term chromatic adaptation could saturate or otherwise inhibit neural responses that mediate color shifts from VLT adaptation alone. Different processes of adaptation have been described at many levels of the visual system. A classical distinction is photochemical (pigment bleaching; Wyszecki & Stiles, 1980) versus neural processes of adaptation, and of course there is a variety of different neural mechanisms. For example, two distinct mechanisms are posited to explain color shifts that persist for a longer duration following a 10 s-on/10 s-off chromatic adapting cycle (0.05 Hz), compared to presenting the same adapting light continuously (Jameson et al., 1979). Within the realm of neural adaptation, some mechanisms depend on the space-average or time-average of the adapting stimulus (Fairchild & Lennie, 1992; Valberg & Lange-Malecki, 1990; Webster & Wilson, 2000), while other mechanisms are driven by the variation in the adapting stimulus over space (Brown & MacLeod, 1997; Monnier & Shevell, 2003; Shevell & Wei, 1998) or time (Webster & Mollon, 1991, 1994). Additional evidence in support of multiple mechanisms comes from the observation that a given long-wavelength adapting light can shift color appearance toward either greenness or redness, depending on whether the light is presented to the same eye as the test, as is typical, or to only the contralateral eye (Shevell & Humanski, 1984); presumably, the contralateral stimulus drives only a central neural mechanism while the same-eye adapting field affects also retinal mechanisms of adaptation that influence the neural representation of the test field. In addition, separate mechanisms are revealed by their distinct time scales (Vul, Krizay, & MacLeod, 2008; Webster & Leonard, 2008). Multiple, hierarchical mechanisms of adaptation are posited even for short-term adaptation to a steadily presented adapting field in the same eye as the test (Rinner & Gegenfurtner, 2000). Given the broad evidence for multiple mechanisms of adaptation, it may not be surprising that short-term and VLT chromatic adaptation act cumulatively. Isolating distinct mechanisms, however, does not guarantee that each one has an influence on color perception when other mechanisms are also active. The findings here show the importance of understanding VLT adaptation because the color shifts it causes persist even with strong, simultaneous short-term chromatic adaptation. Acknowledgment Supported by NIH Grant EY-04802. References Belmore, S. C., & Shevell, S. K. (2008). Very-long-term chromatic adaptation: Test of gain theory and a new method. Visual Neuroscience, 25, 411 414. Brown, R. O., & MacLeod, D. I. A. (1997). Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors. Current Biology, 7, 844 849. Cicerone, C. M., Krantz, D. H., & Larmier, J. (1975). Opponent-process additivity III: Effects of moderate chromatic adaptation. Vision Research, 15, 1125 1135. Drum, B. (1981). Additive effect of backgrounds in chromatic induction. Vision Research, 21, 959 961. Eisner, A., & Enoch, J. M. (1982). Some effects of 1 week s monocular exposure to long-wavelength stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 31, 169 174.

366 S.C. Belmore, S.K. Shevell / Vision Research 51 (2011) 362 366 Fairchild, M. D., & Lennie, P. (1992). Chromatic adaptation to natural and incandescent illuminants. Vision Research, 32, 2077 2085. Guth, S. L., Massof, R. W., & Benzschawel, T. (1980). Vector model for normal and dichromatic color vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 70, 197 212. Hayhoe, M., & Wenderoth, P. (1991). Adaptation mechanisms in color and brightness. In A. Valberg & B. B. Lee (Eds.), From Pigments to Perception (pp. 353 367). New York: Plenum. Jameson, D., & Hurvich, L. M. (1972). Color adaptation: Sensitivity, contrast and afterimages. In D. Jameson & L. M. Hurvich (Eds.). Handbook of sensory physiology (Vol. VII/4, pp. 568 581). Berlin, Germany: Springer. Jameson, D., Hurvich, L. M., & Varner, F. D. (1979). Receptoral and postdoctoral visual processes in recovery from chromatic adaptation. Proceedings National Academy of Sciences USA, 76, 3034 3038. Larimer, J. (1981). Red/green opponent colors equilibria measured on chromatic adapting fields: Evidence for gain changes and restoring forces. Vision Research, 14, 1127 1140. Monnier, P., & Shevell, S. K. (2003). Large shifts in color appearance from patterned chromatic backgrounds. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 801 802. Neitz, J., Carroll, J., Yamauchi, Y., Neitz, M., & Williams, D. R. (2002). Color perception is mediated by a plastic neural mechanism that is adjustable in adults. Neuron, 15, 783 792. Rinner, O., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2000). Time course of chromatic adaptation for color appearance and discrimination. Vision Research, 40, 1813 1826. Shevell, S. K. (1978). The dual role of chromatic backgrounds in color perception. Vision Research, 18, 1649 1661. Shevell, S. K. (1982). Color perception under chromatic adaptation: Equilibrium yellow and long-wavelength adaptation. Vision Research, 22, 279 292. Shevell, S. K., & Humanski, R. A. (1984). Color perception under contralateral and binocularly fused chromatic adaptation. Vision Research, 24, 1011 1019. Shevell, S. K., & Wei, J. (1998). Chromatic induction: Border contrast or adaptation to surrounding light? Vision Research, 38, 1561 1566. Valberg, A., & Lange-Malecki, B. (1990). Colour constancy in mondrian patterns: A partial cancellation of physical chromaticity shifts by simultaneous contrast. Vision Research, 30, 371 380. Vul, E., Krizay, E., & MacLeod, D. I. A. (2008). The McCollough effect reflects permanent and transient adaptation in early visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 8(12), 1 12. Ware, C., & Cowan, W. B. (1982). Changes in perceived color due to chromatic interactions. Vision Research, 22, 1035 1062. Webster, M. A., & Leonard, D. (2008). Adaptation and perceptual norms in color vision. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 25, 2817 2825. Webster, M. A., & Mollon, J. D. (1991). Changes in colour appearance following postreceptoral adaptation. Nature, 349, 235 238. Webster, M. A., & Mollon, J. D. (1994). The influence of contrast adaptation on color appearance. Vision Research, 34, 1993 2020. Webster, M. A., & Wilson, J. A. (2000). Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance. Vision Research, 40, 3801 3816. Wyszecki, G., & Stiles, W. S. (1980). High-level trichromatic color matching and the pigment-bleaching hypothesis. Vision Research, 20, 23 37. Yamauchi, Y., Williams, D. R., Brainard, D. H., Roorda, A., Carroll, J., Neitz, M., et al. (2002). What determines unique yellow, L/M cone ratio or visual experience? In Paper presented at the 9th congress of the international colour association, proceedings of SPIE, vol. 4421, pp. 275 278.