Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Similar documents
Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Brainpotentialsassociatedwithoutcome expectation and outcome evaluation

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Ingroup categorization and response conflict: Interactive effects of target race, flanker compatibility, and infrequency on N2 amplitude

Sensitivity of Electrophysiological Activity from Medial Frontal Cortex to Utilitarian and Performance Feedback

Biological Psychology

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Development: Event-Related Brain Potential and Task Performance Indices of Executive Control in Preadolescent Children

Supporting Information

ERP Correlates of Feedback and Reward Processing in the Presence and Absence of Response Choice

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

Electrophysiological Analysis of Error Monitoring in Schizophrenia

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT

Reward positivity is elicited by monetary reward in the absence of response choice Sergio Varona-Moya a,b, Joaquín Morís b,c and David Luque b,d

International Journal of Psychophysiology

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

A study of the effect of auditory prime type on emotional facial expression recognition

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action

Cardiac and electro-cortical responses to performance feedback reflect different aspects of feedback processing

BRAIN RESEARCH 1104 (2006) available at

Retest reliability of event-related potentials: Evidence from a variety of paradigms

Response-selection Conflict Contributes to Inhibition of Return

Research Article Mindfulness Trait Predicts Neurophysiological Reactivity Associated with Negativity Bias: An ERP Study

Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

An EEG/ERP study of efficient versus inefficient visual search

Development of action monitoring through adolescence into adulthood: ERP and source localization

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Beware misleading cues: Perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex

SLEEP STAGING AND AROUSAL. Dr. Tripat Deep Singh (MBBS, MD, RPSGT, RST) International Sleep Specialist (World Sleep Federation program)

Affective-motivational influences on feedback-related ERPs in a gambling task

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Extraversion-Related Differences in Stimulus Analysis: Effectiveness of the Lateralized. Readiness Potential. Dianna Monteith. Saint Thomas University

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Individual Differences in the Regulation of Intergroup Bias: The Role of Conflict Monitoring and Neural Signals for Control

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

Error-related negativity predicts academic performance

The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Time-frequency theta and delta measures index separable components of feedback processing in a gambling task

Discovering Processing Stages by combining EEG with Hidden Markov Models

Supporting Information

EEG History. Where and why is EEG used? 8/2/2010

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events

Electrophysiological correlates of error correction

NeuroImage. Resting-state EEG power predicts conflict-related brain activity in internally guided but not in externally guided decision-making

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

Does Contralateral Delay Activity Reflect Working Memory Storage or the Current Focus of Spatial Attention within Visual Working Memory?

The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy

Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Visual Short-term Memory Capacity for Simple and Complex Objects

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

F E M M Faculty of Economics and Management Magdeburg

Drink alcohol and dim the lights: The impact of cognitive deficits on medial frontal cortex function

Generalization Effect of Conditioned Sadness: An Innovation of the Approach to Elicit Implicit Sadness

Frontal midline theta and N200 amplitude reflect complementary information about expectancy and outcome evaluation

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Perceptual properties of feedback stimuli influence the feedback-related negativity in the flanker gambling task

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

Presented at the 44th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, October 20-24, 2004, Santa Fe, New Mexico, USA.

What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3

Perceptual and cognitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory distraction

Error-preceding brain activity: Robustness, temporal dynamics, and boundary conditions $

Effects of Light-Emitting Diode Light on Human Electroencephalogram in Comparison with Fluorescent Light

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

Developmental Course of Auditory Event-Related Potentials in Autism

Effects of dynamic rotation on event-related brain potentials

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

WAVELET ENERGY DISTRIBUTIONS OF P300 EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS FOR WORKING MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN CHILDREN

ERP Correlates of Identity Negative Priming

Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene

Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks

To Bet or Not to Bet? The Error Negativity or Error-related Negativity Associated with Risk-taking Choices

The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention. *Corresponding Author. Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, UK

linkedin.com/in/lizziehillsleeptechservices 1

Neural correlates of perceptual separation-induced enhancement of prepulse inhibition of startle in humans

Learning from delayed feedback: neural responses in temporal credit assignment

PD233: Design of Biomedical Devices and Systems

An investigation of the effect of preparation on response execution and inhibition in the go/nogo task

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

ERROR PROCESSING IN CLINICAL POPULATIONS IN CONTRAST TO ADHD J.J.VAN DER MEERE

No Behavioral or ERP Evidence for a Developmental Lag in Visual Working Memory Capacity or Filtering in Adolescents and Adults with ADHD

NeuroImage 51 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception

Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence

See what I mean? An ERP study of the effect of background knowledge on novel object processing

Social class affects neural empathic responses

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Transcription:

Supplementary materials 1 Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling analyses placed the source of the No Go N2 component in the dorsal ACC, near the ACC source identified for the ERN component reported in the main text. (b) Scalp distribution corroborates maximal deflection at the vertex.

Supplementary materials 2 Neurocognitive Correlates of Liberalism and Conservatism David M. Amodio, John T. Jost, Sarah L. Master, and Cindy M. Yee Procedure Upon arrival to the laboratory, participants provided their informed consent and were prepared for physiological recording. The participant was seated in a dimly-lit, sound-proofed room in a comfortable chair, approximately one meter from a computer monitor. The experimenter explained that, following baseline recordings, the participant would complete a simple computer task. Experimenters were blind to participants political attitudes. Political attitudes questionnaire. A measure of political attitudes was embedded in a larger set of personality and attitudes surveys completed at the every beginning of the experimental session. Participants completed these questionnaires in private. Participants were instructed to not make any identifying marks on the questionnaires and, upon completion, to place the questionnaires into a large envelope. These questionnaires remained in these envelopes until the completion of the study, at which time they were entered into a computer database. The critical political orientation item asked participants to indicate their political orientation on a scale ranging from Extremely Liberal ( 5) to Extremely Conservative (+5), with neutral corresponding to 0. This single item has been shown to provide a valid and reliable measure of political orientation that is very strongly predictive of intended and actual behavior (e.g., voting decisions). 1,2 EEG recording. Participants were fitted with a stretch-lycra cap, and EEG was collected from 29 scalp sites using Ag/AgCl electrodes positioned according to the 10-10 system. These sites included midline, frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital locations. The active reference electrode was placed on the left earlobe, and a ground electrode was placed on the forehead. EEG was also collected from the right earlobe for offline rereferencing. Vertical and horizontal

Supplementary materials 3 electrooculogram (EOG) was collected to permit the reduction of artifact due to eye movements. Electro-Gel (Eaton, OH) was used as the conductive medium, and impedances were below 5kΩ at each scalp site (below 10kΩ at EOG sites). EEG was recorded through a 0.1 100 bandpass filter and digitized at 500 Hz using a 32-channel Synamps amplifier (Neuroscan Labs, El Paso, TX). Offline, EEG was manually scored to remove portions of data containing eye or muscle movement, and rereferenced to the average earlobe. Go/No Go task. On each trial of the Go/No Go task, either the letter M or W was presented in the center of a computer monitor screen, following Nieuwenhuis et al. 3 Half of the participants were instructed to make a Go response when they saw M but to make no response when they saw W ; the remaining participants completed a version in which W was the Go stimulus and M was the No Go stimulus; assignment to either version of the task was random. Responses were registered on a computer keyboard placed in the participants laps. Each trial began with a fixation point, presented for 500 ms. The target then appeared for 100 ms, followed by a blank screen. Participants were instructed to respond within 500 ms of target onset. A Too slow! warning message appeared after responses that exceeded this deadline, and Incorrect feedback was given after erroneous responses. The task consisted of 500 trials, of which 80% consisted of the Go stimulus and 20% consisted of the No Go stimulus. As in past research, the high frequency of Go stimuli induced a prepotent Go response, enhancing the difficulty of successfully inhibiting a response on No Go trials. Participants received a two-minute break halfway through the task, which took approximately 15 minutes to complete. Following completion of the task, participants were debriefed, thanked, paid or awarded credit, and dismissed. Preliminary analyses of task behavior revealed that participants made a significantly higher percentage of errors on No Go trials (M = 39%, SD =.16) than on Go trials (M = 0.01 %,

Supplementary materials 4 SD = 0.02), t(42) = 15.33, P < 0.001, indicating that the Go/No Go task was successful in eliciting a habitual response pattern that was difficult to inhibit. ERP processing ERN. Frequencies below 1 Hz and above 15 Hz were digitally filtered (96 db, zero phase shift). An 800 ms response-locked epoch of EEG signal, centered on the time of response within each trial, was selected for each artifact-free trial. Baseline correction procedures subtracted the average voltage occurring from 400 ms to 50 ms prior to response from the entire epoch. Epochs associated with incorrect responses on No Go trials and correct responses on Go trials were averaged within their respective trial types. The ERN was scored as the peak negative deflection occurring between 50 and 150 ms, relative to response onset, at the frontocentral scalp site (Fcz), as in previous research. 3 The ERN component refers to the average amplitude associated with incorrect Go responses on No Go trials. N2. Frequencies below 1 Hz and above 15 Hz were digitally filtered (96 db, zero-phase shift). A 1000 ms epoch of EEG signal, beginning 200 ms prior to target onset, was selected for each artifact-free trial. Baseline correction procedures subtracted the average voltage occurring 100 to 200 ms relative to target onset within each epoch from the entire epoch. Epochs associated with correct and incorrect responses on No Go trials were averaged within their respective trial types. The N2 was scored as the peak negative deflection occurring between 200 and 400 ms, relative to target onset, at the vertex site (Cz), where it is typically maximal. The No Go N2 component refers to the average N2 amplitude associated with correct No Go responses. Source localization. Source localization analyses were conducted using SOURCE (Neuroscan Labs, El Paso, TX) dipole modeling software (see ref #4). A single equivalent current dipole model was estimated from the peak of the ERP wave, for both the ERN (reported

Supplementary materials 5 in the main text) and the N2 (Fig. 1). As in Van Veen and Carter (2002), we estimated the source from the peak maximum of the difference waveform, in which the waveform for the correct Go responses was subtracted, point by point, from the critical No Go waveform, for each subject. 5 Difference waveforms were then averaged across subjects for localization analyses. A single-dipole model of the ERN peak (44 ms post-response) identified a source in the dorsal region of the ACC (Mean Global Field Power [MGFP] = 4.66 μv; PAN coordinates [mm]: x = 1.0, y = 35.7, z = 88.2; dipole strength = 167.8 nam), which accounted for 90.3 % of the variance of the signal (see Fig. in main text). A single dipole model of the N2 peak (270 ms post-stimulus) identified a source in the same region of the ACC (MGFP = 2.36 μv; PAN coordinates [mm]: x = 4.1, y = 38.8, z = 88.7; dipole strength = 83.22 nam), which accounted for 91.4 % of the variance of the signal.

Supplementary materials 6 References 1. Knight, K., in Measures of Political Attitudes, J.P. Robinson, P.R. Shaver, & L.S. Wrightsman, Eds. (Academic Press, San Diego, 1999), pp. 59-158. 2. Jost, J., Am. Psychol. 61, 651-670 (2006). 3. Niewenhuis, S., Yeung, N., Van Den Wildenberg, W., Ridderinkhof, K. R. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosc. 16, 805 (2003). 4. Fuchs, M., Wagner, M., Kohler, T., Wischmann, H. A. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 16, 267-95 (1999). 5. van Veen, V., Carter, C. S. J Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 593-602 (2002).