Repetition priming and the time course of object recognition: an fmri study

Similar documents
Does stimulus quality affect the physiologic MRI responses to brief visual activation?

Research Paper Correspondence: Thomas W. James Received: 1 June 2000 Revised: 8 July 2000 Accepted: 12 July 2000

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

The functional anatomy of the McCollough contingent colour aftereffect

Attentional suppression of activity in the human visual cortex

Attention to Form or Surface Properties Modulates Different Regions of Human Occipitotemporal Cortex

Human primary visual cortex and lateral geniculate nucleus activation during visual imagery

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of Human Visual Cortex during Face Matching: A Comparison with Positron Emission Tomography

Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making

Supplementary Note Psychophysics:

Supplemental Information

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

MR Advance Techniques. Vascular Imaging. Class II

Summary. Multiple Body Representations 11/6/2016. Visual Processing of Bodies. The Body is:

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Object and spatial alternation tasks with minimal delays activate the right anterior hippocampus proper in humans

Flexible Retinotopy: Motion-Dependent Position Coding in the Visual Cortex

Experimental Design. Thomas Wolbers Space and Aging Laboratory Centre for Cognitive and Neural Systems

Magnetic Resonance Angiography

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 159 ( 2014 ) WCPCG 2014

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Reproducibility of Visual Activation During Checkerboard Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging at 4 Tesla

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

Scratching Beneath the Surface: New Insights into the Functional Properties of the Lateral Occipital Area and Parahippocampal Place Area

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Kenneth F. Valyear a, Jody C. Culham a, Nadder Sharif b, David Westwood c, Melvyn A. Goodale a,

Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus

Event-related fmri analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison

MEDICAL REVIEW Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: From Acquisition to Application

How the brain perceives causality: an eventrelated

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Perceptual Learning of Motion Direction Discrimination with Suppressed and Unsuppressed MT in Humans: An fmri Study

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus

Sub-exemplar Shape Tuning in Human Face-Related Areas

The BOLD fmri refractory effect is specific to stimulus attributes: evidence from a visual motion paradigm

Why do we prefer looking at some scenes rather than others?

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients

Supplementary Information

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

PHYSICS OF MRI ACQUISITION. Alternatives to BOLD for fmri

Functional Topography of a Distributed Neural System for Spatial and Nonspatial Information Maintenance in Working Memory

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

P2 Visual - Perception

Purely temporal gure±ground segregation

Mental chronometry using latency-resolved functional MRI

Evidence for false memory before deletion in visual short-term memory

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval

Supplemental Information. Neural Representations Integrate the Current. Field of View with the Remembered 360 Panorama. in Scene-Selective Cortex

fmri: What Does It Measure?

Behavioural Brain Research

fmri and visual brain function Dr. Tessa Dekker UCL Institute of Ophthalmology

Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Brain Imaging Investigation of the Impairing Effect of Emotion on Cognition

Attention and Scene Perception

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Perception of Faces and Bodies

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Supporting Information

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Dissociation of Mnemonic and Perceptual Processes During Spatial and Nonspatial Working Memory Using fmri

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

fmri and visual brain function

Ventral and Dorsal Pathways Differently Related to Visual Awareness of Body Postures under Continuous Flash Suppression

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

Brain activity related to integrative processes in visual object recognition: bottom-up integration and the modulatory influence of stored knowledge

Lag-Sensitive Repetition Suppression Effects in the Anterior Parahippocampal Gyrus

Human cortical gustatory areas: A review of functional neuroimaging data

Face encoding and recognition in the human brain

Shape Modeling of the Corpus Callosum for Neuroimaging Studies of the Brain (Part I) Dongqing Chen, Ph.D.

ERP Studies of Selective Attention to Nonspatial Features

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

The neural basis of object perception Kalanit Grill-Spector

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Stimulus Repetition and Hemodynamic Response Refractoriness in Event-Related fmri

A Stable Topography of Selectivity for Unfamiliar Shape Classes in Monkey Inferior Temporal Cortex

International Journal of Innovative Research in Advanced Engineering (IJIRAE) Volume 1 Issue 10 (November 2014)

Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex

35 Gedney Rd

The Tools: Imaging the Living Brain

An fmri Version of the Farnsworth Munsell 100-Hue Test Reveals Multiple Color-selective Areas in Human Ventral Occipitotemporal Cortex

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Transcription:

Brain Imaging 10, 1019±1023 (1999) WE investigated the effects of repetition priming on the time course of recognition in several visual areas of the brain using fmri. We slowed down recognition by gradually revealing the stimuli, in order to prolong the pre-recognition phase. Activation was lower for primed than for non-primed objects overall in both the occipitotemporal region (OTR) and the intraparietal region (IPR). A difference was found between primed and nonprimed objects in the rate of increase of OTR activation. We concluded that the IPR, in addition to the OTR, was affected by repetition priming, and that this effect was different from that seen in the OTR. Neuro- Report 10:1019±1023 # 1999 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Key words: Human; Magnetic resonance imaging; Memory; Object recognition; Reaction time; Visual awareness; Visual cortex Repetition priming and the time course of object recognition: an fmri study Thomas W. James, CA G. Keith Humphrey, Joseph S. Gati, 1 Ravi S. Menon 1 and Melvyn A. Goodale Psychology Department, The University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada N6A 5C2; 1 The John P. Robarts Research Institute, 100 Perth Dr., London, ON, Canada N6A 5K8 CA Corresponding Author Introduction Objects that we encounter regularly are more easily recognized than objects that are less familiar. This difference in behavior is re ected in the activity of high-level visual areas of the primate brain, particularly in the ventral (or perceptual) stream of cortical visual processing, that projects from area V1 to the temporal cortex [1]. Thus, the response of single units in the inferior temporal cortex (IT) of the monkey is reduced with repeated presentations of the same visual stimulus; a phenomenon called repetition suppression [2]. In humans, event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded from the occipitotemporal region also show a reduced response to repeated (or primed) stimuli [3]. The advent of functional neuroimaging (PET and fmri) has resulted in a number of studies showing that repetition priming [4] leads to a reduction of activity in the ventral stream, particularly the region surrounding the occipitotemporal junction [5±7]. Because ERPs and single unit responses are recorded at a much higher temporal resolution than either fmri or PET, these techniques provide a more detailed look at the pro le of functional brain activation. Both ERP and single unit studies have found that responses to primed and non-primed stimuli begin to differ before any overt response occurred [3,8]. Despite its poor temporal resolution, fmri provides much higher spatial resolution than do ERPs. In the present study, we took advantage 0959-4965 # Lippincott Williams & Wilkins of the high spatial speci city of fmri to study where in the visual pathways the effects of object priming occurred, but at the same time we used a new paradigm that allowed us to examine the time course of object recognition. We made it dif cult for participants to recognize objects quickly by revealing the objects only gradually over time, thereby making high temporal resolution unnecessary. We predicted that activation to primed objects would be lower than activation to non-primed objects in high-level visual areas of the ventral stream. We also predicted that differences would begin to appear before an overt response occurred. Materials and Methods Participants were all graduate students attending the University of Western Ontario, were right-handed, spoke English as their rst language, and had no history of neuropsychological disorder. There were eight participants in total, four men and four women, with ages ranging from 22 to 27 years. All imaging was performed with a 4 T, whole body MRI system (Varian, Palo Alto, USA; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) and a quadrature head coil. The eld of view was 19.2 3 19.2 3 5.0 cm, with an in-plane resolution of 64 3 64 pixels and 10 contiguous scan planes per volume, resulting in a voxel size of 3 3 3 3 5 (mm). Slices were oriented perpendicular to the calcarine sulcus and spanned from the occipital pole to the posterior aspect of the Vol 10 No 5 6 April 1999 1019

corpus collosum. Images were collected using a T2 -weighted, segmented (navigator corrected), interleaved EPI acquisition (TE ˆ 15 ms, TR ˆ 500 ms, ip angle ˆ 458, four segments/plane) for BOLDbased imaging [9]. Each volume (10 planes) required 2.0 s to acquire. Functional activation data were superimposed onto high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images (3D magnetization prepared (MP) turbo FLASH acquisition using an inversion time (TI) of 500 ms; TE ˆ 6 ms, TR ˆ 11 ms, ip ˆ 118). Participants were placed within the magnet in a supine position, with their head rmly supported by foam padding within the RF coil. Participants viewed a rear projection screen though a mirror within the coil. The screen was placed such that it straddled the participant's waist and was 60 cm from the mirror. Images were projected onto the rear projection screen using an NEC LCD projector. The maximum viewing area of the screen was 208 horizontal by 158 vertical. Checkerboard viewing: To help distinguish between areas of the brain that were processing lowlevel and high-level object information, we used two different stimuli to determine regions of interest (ROIs) in our images, a checkerboard stimulus and an object stimulus (described below). We used a circular checkerboard stimulus that was contrast reversing, ickered at 8 Hz and subtended 158 of visual angle. The checkerboard was presented to participants for 24 s, followed by a black screen for 24 s, while the participant xated a central cross. This sequence was repeated three times. T. W. James et al. Passive object viewing: This phase of the experiment served two purposes. First, it allowed us to identify voxels in our images that were involved in processing objects. Second, it allowed us to preexpose the participants to speci c objects before the recognition phase of the experiment. In this way, we were able to prime some of the objects and not others. Objects were presented in the center of the screen one at a time for 2 s each, resulting in 24 s of object presentation. Each presentation of the series of objects (see Fig. 2A) was followed by a 24 s period during which only a homogeneous grey screen was shown to the participant. This sequence was repeated four times, resulting in each object (distracters and primes) being presented four times. Participants were informed before the experiment began that some of these objects would be used later in the recognition experiment and that they should study them in order to make the objects easier to recognize later. Participants were instructed to xate on a cross in the center of the screen throughout. Gradual presentation task: A schematic of this task is shown in Fig. 1C. The presentation of each object began with 12 s of baseline, a homogeneous grey screen with a xation cross in the center. The object then began to be revealed, as if from behind vertical venetian blinds that were opening slowly. This was accomplished by superimposing six vertically oriented virtual panels over the object and gradually shrinking the width of the panels. The object was revealed over a 48s period, at the conclusion of which, 80% of the object was showing. Participants A 2 s 24 s B C % Revealed Baseline Button Press when object is recognized Passive Viewing 4 12 20 28 36 44 52 60 Time (s) FIG. 1. (A) Objects used during passive object viewing. Each object was presented for 2 s in the sequence indicated. Four primes, chosen from (B), were presented along with the distracters. B) Objects used during the gradual presentation task. Objects were in two groups and for each participant, only one group of four objects was presented during passive object viewing as primes. (C) A schematic of the gradual presentation task. Twelve seconds of baseline was followed by the object being gradually revealed over a 48 s period until 80% of the object was showing. Participants pressed a button when recognition occurred and these times were recorded. All objects were greyscale, rendered views of familiar, nameable objects. The long axis of each object subtended 8±108 of visual angle. 1020 Vol 10 No 5 6 April 1999

Time course of priming were instructed to xate on a cross in the center of the screen, and press a button when they recognized the object. After the participant pressed the button, the object continued to be revealed while the participant continued to view it. In this way, each object trial required 60 s (12 s baseline, 48 s of object presentation) to complete. These trials were run in two 4 minute blocks. The eight objects that were used for these trials, and the order that they were presented in, are shown in Fig. 1B. All of the functional imaging data were analyzed using Stimulate [10] software. Regions of interest (ROIs) were determined for each participant individually by analyzing the data from the checkerboard and passive object viewing phases of the experiment. A cross-correlation technique [11] was used to nd voxels in the images that responded signi cantly more to the stimulus state (checkerboard or objects) than to the homogeneous grey state. All voxels had to meet at least two criteria to be included in an activation map, a minimum correlation coef cient value of r ˆ 0.30, and that it be a member of a cluster of contiguous voxels of a size not less than six [12]. These criteria produced an effective signi cance level per voxel between p, 0.002 and p, 0.02. Functional images that were collected during the gradual presentation task were analyzed for excessive motion by creating animation loops and centerof-intensity pro les. None of the eight participants produced suf cient motion artifacts to warrant exclusion from further analyses. Time courses were then extracted from voxels within the ROIs. These data were converted into percent signal change scores, using the average signal intensity of the four baseline conditions within each run to establish a baseline level. Time courses were then normalized (using interpolation), setting recognition time for each trial to the averaged recognition time for primed or non-primed objects. Performing this normalization allowed comparisons of the pre- and post-recognition periods across participants and objects. Results The early visual ROI (EVR) was de ned on the checkerboard activation map. Voxels were included in this ROI if they were within the grey matter surrounding the calcarine sulcus in the two most posterior, consecutive slices that showed activation. This procedure for de ning early visual areas assumed the inclusion of areas V1 and V2 in the ROI [13,14]. The occipitotemporal ROI (OTR) was de- ned on the object activation map. Voxels were included in this ROI if they were within the fusiform or inferior temporal gyri or the collateral sulcus, all areas that have been shown to be involved in the high-level processes underlying object recognition [15-21]. The voxels also had to be in the three most anterior, consecutive slices that showed activation. The intraparietal ROI (IPR) was also de ned on the object activation map. Seven of eight participants showed one or more foci of activation along the intraparietal sulcus, though there was considerable individual variation in terms of where along the sulcus the activation occurred. Parietal activation during object recognition has been described in several studies [18,20,21], and is most likely associated with the processing of metric information about objects [1,22]. The averaged primed and non-primed time courses for the OTR are shown in Fig. 2. Time courses from the other two ROIs were similar. Averaging the signal across pre- and post-recognition phases showed signi cant differences between the primed and non-primed conditions in the OTR (t(7) ˆ 1.90, p, 0.05) and the IPR (t(7) ˆ 3.08, p, 0.01), with a non-signi cant trend in the EVR (t(7) ˆ 1.73, p, 0.1). We then analyzed the pre-recognition data points in isolation, in order to determine if the activation functions were different for the primed and nonprimed objects. Regression lines were tted to the pre-recognition data points for each participant and then the slopes were compared. There was a signi cant difference between the primed and non-primed slopes of the time courses extracted from the OTR (Fig. 3), but not from the IPR or EVR. The signal increased more rapidly in the primed condition than in the non-primed condition in the OTR (t(7) ˆ 2.06, p, 0.05). Peak activations for primed and non-primed objects were also analyzed. The peak activation for each participant was determined as the maximum 2.5 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 Hemodynamic delay Baseline Pre-recognition Post-recognition 0 4 8 12 16 20 24 28 32 36 40 44 48 52 56 60 FIG. 2. Percentage signal change as a function of time for the occipitotemporal region (OTR). Participants recognized the primed objects (M ˆ 34.5 s, s ˆ 2.2) signi cantly sooner than the non-primed objects (M ˆ 46.0 s, s ˆ 1.8; t(7) ˆ 2.7, p, 0.05). Symbols: lled ˆ non-primed, hollow ˆ primed. Vol 10 No 5 6 April 1999 1021

2.5 2.0 1.5 1.0 Non-primed Primed 0.5 16 20 24 28 32 36 40 44 48 FIG. 3. Percentage signal change as a function of time for pre-recognition data points from the occipitotemporal region (OTR). The primed signal increased more rapidly than the non-primed signal. value of each time course averaged across trials. Peak activation was signi cantly lower for the primed than for the non-primed objects in the OTR (t(7) ˆ 2.36, p, 0.03) and IPR (t(7) ˆ 2.07, p, 0.05), but not in the EVR. The OTR difference can be observed in Fig. 2, where the peak for the primed objects occurred at 32 s and the peak for the nonprimed objects occurred at 42 s. In both the primed and non-primed conditions, the peak activation occurred just prior to the time of the recognition response. Discussion By gradually revealing the visual stimuli, we were able to increase the time that it took participants to recognize the stimuli. Combining the use of fmri with this method of presentation allowed us to examine the process of object recognition with higher spatial and temporal resolution than has been possible before. In particular, it allowed us to examine whether there was a difference in activation to primed (previously viewed) and non-primed objects during the `pre-recognition' period (before the participants indicated they had recognized the stimulus) in three ROIs, the EVR, OTR and the IPR. Overall, the level of activation in the OTR was lower for primed than for non-primed objects. This nding replicates the results of other studies of repetition priming that have found lower activation levels in ventral stream structures with primed stimuli, even though these earlier studies used short stimulus presentation times [5±7]. However, we also found that overall activity in the IPR was lower for primed than for non-primed stimuli. To our knowledge, an effect of repetition priming has never been T. W. James et al. reported in this posterior parietal (PP) area, even though studies have utilized many different types of stimuli [5±7]. It has been suggested that recognition of a degraded image of an object may require the recruitment of structures in PP cortex that are involved in spatial reconstruction and feature binding [23]. In the present experiment, spatial reconstruction could have been facilitated by prior exposure to the non-degraded object, resulting in reduced activation in the IPR. As can be seen in Fig. 2, the overall differences just described were due mostly to large post-recognition differences between primed and non-primed signals. This would have been the portion of the time course that was measured in most other neuroimaging studies of repetition priming. The present data showed that the non-primed signal continued to climb while the primed signal dropped (at 34 s). Thus, past studies of repetition priming could have reported differences in activation that were due simply to faster recognition times for the primed stimuli. The present data showed that the pattern of activation was different between primed and nonprimed stimuli. Even after recognition of nonprimed stimuli occurred, the signal did not drop to the level of the primed stimuli. The levels of activation observed in EVR did not differ signi cantly between primed and non-primed visual stimuli, and this was true for all the measures that we used. The absence of any effect of repetition priming on the activity of early visual areas was consistent with many other studies that have looked at activity in primary visual cortex (for review see [5]). Nevertheless, there was a hint that overall activity might be lower with primed objects and it is possible that a more detailed analysis will reveal subtle effects of priming on early visual areas. Certainly, slowing down the whole recognition process, as we did in this study, offers a way of examining this possibility. The differences in fmri signal to primed and non-primed stimuli in the OTR and IPR were also seen in peak activation, which was lower for primed stimuli. From other functional imaging studies, we know that primed stimuli produce less activation on average than non-primed stimuli [5±7]. We also know that primed objects are recognized faster than non-primed objects [4,24] and our study was no exception. Taken together, these results could be interpreted as meaning that primed stimuli require a lower level of activation for recognition to occur if primed and non-primed objects have the same rate of increase of activation in the period leading up to recognition. The pattern of activation observed in the IPR is consistent with this explanation; in other words, the level of activation achieved before recog- 1022 Vol 10 No 5 6 April 1999

Time course of priming nition occurred with the primed objects was lower than for non-primed objects, but the rates of increase of activation during the pre-recognition phase were the same. Although peak activation was lower in the OTR for primed stimuli, it appears that this was not the only factor that in uenced faster recognition of primed as opposed to non-primed stimuli. Activation in the OTR increased more rapidly for primed objects than for non-primed objects after stimulus onset. Thus, even if (hypothetically) the same level of activation was required for the recognition of primed and non-primed stimuli, recognition would still have occurred sooner if the rate of increase of activation was greater for primed stimuli. In short, mechanisms in the OTR that participate in the more rapid recognition of primed stimuli may do so by virtue of a combination of an elevated rate of increase in activation and a lower threshold of required activation. Conclusions We analyzed the time course of recognition for primed (previously viewed) and non-primed objects by slowing down recognition by gradually revealing the stimuli. Primed objects produced less activation than non-primed objects in the occipitotemporal region, an area implicated in processing the perceptual properties of stimuli in order to determine identity. Primed objects also produced less activation in the intraparietal region, an area implicated in the spatial reconstruction of degraded images. Activation in the occipitotemporal region increased at a faster rate for primed objects than for non-primed objects prior to recognition, a difference in the pattern of activation that was not seen in the intraparietal region. This new paradigm for studying the effects of prior experience on object recognition not only allows us to identify regions that show differences in activation before recognition takes place, but also allows us to chart differences in the pattern of activation observed in these different regions across the entire time course of recognition. References 1. Milner AD and Goodale MA. The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1995. 2. Desimone R. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93, 13494±13499 (1996). 3. Zhang XL, Begleiter H, Porjesz B et al. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 102, 200±215 (1997). 4. Tulving E and Schacter DL. Science 247, 301±306 (1990). 5. Buckner RL, Goodman J, Burock M et al. Neuron 20, 285±296 (1998). 6. Squire LR, Ojemann JG, Miezin FM et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89, 1837±1841 (1992). 7. Ungerleider LG. Science 270, 769±775 (1995). 8. Li L, Miller K and Desimone R. J Neurophysiol 69, 1918±1929 (1993). 9. Ogawa S, Menon RS, Tank DW et al. Biophys J 64, 803±812 (1993). 10. Strupp JP. Neuroimage 3, S607 (1996). 11. Bandettini PA, Jesmanowicz A, Wong EC et al. Magn Res Med 30, 161±173 (1993). 12. Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M et al. Magn Res Med 33, 636±647 (1995). 13. Van Essen DC, Drury HA, Joshi S et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95, 788±795 (1998). 14. DeYoe EA and Van Essen DC. Trends Neurosci 11, 219±226 (1988). 15. Kanwisher N, Chun MM, McDermott J et al. Cogn Brain Res 5, 55±67 (1996). 16. Malach R, Reppas JB, Benson RR et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92, 8135±8139 (1995). 17. Sergent J, Ohta S and Macdonald B. Brain 115, 15±36 (1992). 18. Kraut M, Hart JJr, Soher BJ et al. Neurology 48, 1416±1420 (1997). 19. Schacter DL, Reiman E, Uecker A et al. Nature 376, 587±590 (1995). 20. Vanni S, Revonsuo A and Hari R. J Neurosci 17, 7141±7147 (1997). 21. Price CJ, Moore CJ, Humphreys GW et al. Proc R Soc Lond B 263, 1501±1507 (1996). 22. Faillenot I, Toni I, Decety J et al. Cerebr Cortex 7, 77±85 (1997). 23. Dolan RJ, Fink GR, Rolls E et al. Nature 389, 596±599 (1997). 24. Schacter DL and Buckner RL. Neuron 20, 185±195 (1998). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS: This research was supported by the Medical Research Council of Canada. Received 19 January 1999; accepted 7 February 1999 Vol 10 No 5 6 April 1999 1023