A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Similar documents
Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

The (Dynamic) Mind in the Cave: Representational Space of Cave Paintings and Petroglyphs

Cast shadow can modulate the judged final position of a moving target

The locus of memory displacement is at least partially perceptual: Effects of velocity, expectation, friction, memory averaging, and weight

Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Representational Momentum Beyond Internalized Physics

Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel. Ph.D. in Cognitive Psychology 1999.

The path of visual attention

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

EFFECT OF SPEED AND ACCURACY INSTRUCTIONS ON CONFIDENCE JUDGMENTS FOR MAGNITUDE ESTIMATES*.

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

The eyes fixate the optimal viewing position of task-irrelevant words

Attention maintains mental extrapolation of target position: Irrelevant distractors eliminate forward displacement after implied motion

Dartmouth College (Hanover, NH) Experimental Psychology Ph.D

Main Study: Summer Methods. Design

Categorical Perception

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Visual Transformation of Size

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

Supplementary Materials

Head Up, Foot Down. Object Words Orient Attention to the Objects Typical Location. Research Report

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Range effects of an irrelevant dimension on classification

SENSATION AND PERCEPTION KEY TERMS

ID# Exam 2 PS 325, Fall 2003

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

TICK-MARKS, AXES, AND LABELS: THE EFFECTS OF ADDING CONTEXT TO AUDITORY GRAPHS

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

PERCEPTION OF AUDITORY-VISUAL SIMULTANEITY CHANGES BY ILLUMINANCE AT THE EYES

Auditory Physiology PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 30. Organ of Corti

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

Cross-modal interaction between vision and touch: the role of synesthetic correspondence

Perception Lecture 1

Key questions about attention

ON THE ROLE OF IMPUTED VELOCITY IN THE AUDITORY KAPPA EFFECT. Molly J. Henry. A Thesis

Encoding of Information in Auditory Displays: Initial Research on Flexibility and Processing Codes in Dual-task Scenarios

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

Random visual noise impairs object-based attention

The number line effect reflects top-down control

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition

Examining the locus of the attentional attraction effect

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Exploring a brightness-drag illusion. Author. Published. Journal Title DOI. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Griffith Research Online

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

Sound Waves. Sensation and Perception. Sound Waves. Sound Waves. Sound Waves

HEARING AND PSYCHOACOUSTICS

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

Perceptual asynchronies between color and motion at the onset of motion and along the motion trajectory

EFFECTS OF RHYTHM ON THE PERCEPTION OF URGENCY AND IRRITATION IN 1 AUDITORY SIGNALS. Utrecht University

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

Spatial Extent of Attention to Letters and Words

On perfect working-memory performance with large numbers of items

Distractor Interference During Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements

Perceptual Learning of Categorical Colour Constancy, and the Role of Illuminant Familiarity

Psychology of Perception PSYC Spring 2017 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Grouped Locations and Object-Based Attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994)

Auditory Scene Analysis

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

THE SPATIAL REPRESENTATION OF NON-SYMBOLIC NUMERICAL QUANTITIES

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

The Simon effect in vocal responses

The influence of irrelevant information on speeded classification tasks*

MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

Spatial Prepositions in Context: The Semantics of near in the Presence of Distractor Objects

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Dikran J. Martin. Psychology 110. Name: Date: Making Contact with the World around Us. Principal Features

Perceptual Fluency Affects Categorization Decisions

Representational momentum in perception and grasping: Translating versus transforming objects

Effect of spectral content and learning on auditory distance perception

Object-based attention in Chinese readers of Chinese words: Beyond Gestalt principles

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Going, Going, Gone: Localizing Abrupt Offsets of Moving Objects

How does attention spread across objects oriented in depth?

Seeing Sound: Changing Visual Perception Through Cross-Modal Interaction. Tracey D. Berger. New York University. Department of Psychology

Running head: PERCEPTUAL GROUPING AND SPATIAL SELECTION 1. The attentional window configures to object boundaries. University of Iowa

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations

Object-based selection in the two-rectangles method is not an artifact of the three-sided directional cue

Spatial distortions within the Poggendorff figure and its variants: A parametric analysis

Exploring the link between time to collision and representational momentum

3-D Sound and Spatial Audio. What do these terms mean?

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

Reflexive Spatial Attention to Goal-Directed Reaching

PARAMETERS OF ECHOIC MEMORY

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

Rhythm Categorization in Context. Edward W. Large

Are In-group Social Stimuli more Rewarding than Out-group?

INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 103 (2), February /98/103(2)/1080/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 1080

Transcription:

In D. Algom, D. Zakay, E. Chajut, S. Shaki, Y. Mama, & V. Shakuf (Eds.). (2011). Fechner Day 2011: Proceedings of the 27 th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 89-94). Raanana, Israel: International Society for Psychophysics. A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E. Ruppel 2 1 Department of Psychology, Texas Christian University timothyleehubbard@gmail.com 2 Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Upstate sruppel@uscupstate.edu Abstract Memory for the initial pitch of an auditory target sequence that increased or decreased in frequency was displaced forward in the direction of pitch motion, and this is consistent with a Fröhlich effect. Displacement was decreased (not eliminated) by an auditory cue presented before the target was presented, and displacement was smaller if a valid auditory cue was presented than if an invalid auditory cue was presented. A visual cue that indicated whether the initial pitch would be relatively low or relatively high did not influence memory for the initial pitch. Displacement was larger for descending pitch motion than for ascending pitch motion, and this is consistent with an influence of representational gravity. The data suggest that the mental representation of auditory frequency space exhibits some of the same spatial biases that are exhibited in the representation of visual physical space. Displacement of the remembered initial location of a moving visual target from the actual initial location of that target can be in the direction of target motion or in the direction opposite to target motion, and these two types of displacement are referred to as a Fröhlich effect (for review, Kerzel, 2010) or an onset repulsion effect (Thornton, 2002), respectively. Surprisingly, there has been only one published report examining whether memory for a moving auditory target exhibits a Fröhlich effect or an onset repulsion effect (Getzmann, 2005), and that report suggested a Fröhlich effect occurred. On a more abstract level, auditory pitch is often referred to in spatial terms, with pitches resulting from faster or slower frequencies referred to as higher and lower, respectively, and there is a consistent mapping between higher locations in the visual picture plane and faster auditory frequencies (e.g., Melara & Marks 1990; Rusconi et al., 2006; Shepard, 1982). Whether a Fröhlich effect or an onset repulsion effect occurs in memory for the initial pitch of an auditory target moving in frequency space is not known. If the representation of location in auditory frequency space is similar to the representation of location in visual physical space, then a larger displacement for descending motion than for ascending motion (resulting from representational gravity, Hubbard, 1997) and a smaller displacement following cueing of the initial position (for a Fröhlich effect, Müsseler & Aschersleben, 1998; for an onset repulsion effect, Hubbard & Ruppel, in press) should occur. The experiments reported here presented an auditory target that increased or decreased in frequency, and memory for the initial pitch was examined. Experiment 1 Participants were presented with auditory targets that ascended or descended in pitch. A probe pitch was then presented, and participants judged whether the probe was the same pitch as the remembered initial pitch of the target. On half of the trials, an auditory cue that indicated the

initial pitch of the auditory target was presented prior to the presentation of the target, and on half of the trials, no cue was presented. Method Participants. The participants were 17 undergraduates from the University of South Carolina, Upstate who were naïve to the hypotheses. Apparatus. The stimuli were generated by and the data collected with a Gateway desktop computer equipped with a 15-inch color monitor (resolution of 1024 x 768 pixels). Auditory stimuli were presented over the built-in speakers in the desktop computer. Stimuli. The targets and probes were auditory tones. On each trial, there were five successive presentations of the target that implied ascending motion or descending motion of the target in frequency space, and these are referred to as inducing stimuli (see Figure 1). Each inducing stimulus was presented for 250 msec, and there was a 250 msec interval between successive inducing stimuli. Each inducing stimulus differed by 700 cents (a perfect fifth) from the preceding inducing stimulus, and this ensured a constant perceived velocity. For ascending motion, inducing stimuli were 250, 375, 562.5, 843.75, and 1265.63 Hz, and for descending motion, inducing stimuli were 1265.63, 843.75, 562, 375, and 250 Hz. The auditory probe was one of nine frequencies relative to the initial frequency of the target: -80, -60, -40, -20, 0, +20, +40, +60, or +80 cents (there are 100 cents in a semitone [half step]). Probe positions denoted by a minus sign indicated the frequency of the probe was shifted backward (in the direction opposite to target motion) from the initial frequency of the target by the indicated number of cents, and probe positions denoted by a plus sign indicated the frequency of the probe was shifted forward (in the direction of target motion) from the initial frequency of the target by the indicated number of cents; the zero probe position was the same as the initial frequency of the target. The auditory cue was a tone of the same frequency as the first inducing stimulus (250 or 1265.63 Hz). Trials in which the first auditory tone was the cue rather than the target were signaled by a visual cross presented in the middle of the display at the beginning of the trial. The horizontal and vertical arms of the cross were each 10 pixels in length and 4 pixels in thickness. Each participant received 216 trials (2 cues [present, absent] x 9 probes [-80, -60, Cue (if present) Target Probe or audible until 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec participant responds 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec 250 msec Figure 1. The structure of a trial in Experiments 1, 2, and 3. In Experiments 1 and 2, the cue was an auditory tone, and in Experiment 3, the cue was a visual cross.

-40, -20, 0, +20, +40, +60, +80] x 2 directions [ascending, descending] x 6 replications) in a different random order. Procedure. Participants were first given a practice session consisting of 10 practice trials randomly drawn from the experimental trials. If a cue was presented, the visual cross immediately appeared and was visible for 250 msec before vanishing. There was a blank interval of 250 msec, and the auditory cue was presented for 250 msec. After the auditory cue was presented, there was an interval of 250 msec before the first inducing stimulus appeared. If a cue was not presented, there was a blank interval of 1000 msec before the first inducing stimulus appeared. After the final inducing stimulus vanished, there was a retention interval of 250 msec before the probe was presented. Participants pressed a designated key to indicate if the pitch of the probe was the same as or different from the initial pitch of the target. Results and Discussion Probabilities of a same response are shown in Figure 2. Estimates of displacement were determined by calculating a weighted mean (WM) for each participant for each condition. The sign of a WM indicated direction (a minus sign indicated displacement in the direction opposite target motion; a plus sign indicated displacement in the direction of target motion), and the absolute value of a WM indicated magnitude (a larger absolute value indicated a larger displacement). If the cue was present, WMs for ascending motion (M = 7.46), t(16) = 2.85, p <.02, and for descending motion (M = 16.00), t(16) = 6.41, p <.0001, were larger than zero. If the cue was absent, WMs for ascending motion (M = 15.83), t(16) = 4.86, p <.0002, and for descending motion (M = 25.24), t(16) = 7.84, p <.0001, were larger than zero. Cue influenced displacement, F(1,16) = 19.46, p <.001, and displacement was smaller if a cue was present (M = 11.73) than if a cue was absent (M = 20.54). Thus, a Fröhlich effect occurred in all conditions, and presence of the cue decreased but did not eliminate the Fröhlich effect. Direction influenced displacement, F(1,16) = 8.62, p <.01, with ascending motion (M = 11.65) exhibiting a smaller Fröhlich effect than did descending motion (M = 20.62), and this is consistent with representational gravity. Experiment 2 Figure 2. The probability of a same response as a function of probe position in Experiment 1. If effects of the cue on displacement in Experiment 1 were due to specific pitch information in the cue, then a Fröhlich effect should be smaller if a cue was valid (i.e., if the cue

indicated the initial frequency of the target) then if a cue was invalid (i.e., if the cue indicated a frequency different from the initial frequency of the target). In Experiment 2, a cue was always presented before the target was presented. On 75% of the trials, the cue was valid, and on 25% of the trials, the cue was invalid. Method Participants. The participants were 16 undergraduates from the same participant pool as in Experiment 1, and none had participated in the previous experiment. Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as in Experiment 1. Stimuli. The targets and probes were the same as in Experiment 1. The auditory cue was the same as in Experiment 1, with the following exceptions: On 75% of the trials, the frequency of the cue was the same as the frequency at which the target on that trial would appear (valid cues; 250 and 1265.63 Hz for ascending and descending motion, respectively), and on 25% of the trials, the frequency of the cue was the same as the starting frequency for auditory motion in the opposite direction (invalid cues; 250 and 1265.63 Hz for descending and ascending motion, respectively). Each participant received 288 trials (2 validities [valid, invalid] x 9 probes [-80, -60, -40, -20, 0, +20, +40, +60, +80] x 2 directions [ascending, descending] x 8 replications [6 of valid trials, 2 of invalid trials]) in a different random order. Procedure. The procedure was the same as for trials in which the cue appeared in Experiment 1, except: The visual cross was not presented, and there was a 500 msec blank interval between when participants initiated a trial and when the auditory cue was presented. Results and Discussion Probabilities of a same response are shown in Figure 3, and WMs were calculated as in Experiment 1. If the cue was valid, WMs for ascending motion (M = 5.52), t(15) = 3.21, p <.006, and for descending motion (M = 17.59), t(15) = 6.29, p <.0001, were larger than zero. If the cue was invalid, WMs for ascending motion (M = 12.88), t(15) = 3.34, p <.005, and for descending motion (M = 22), t(15) = 6.28, p <.0001, were larger than zero. Thus, a Fröhlich effect occurred in all conditions. Validity influenced displacement, F(1,15) = 8.21, p <.02, and displacement was smaller with valid cues (M = 11.50) than with invalid cues (M = 17.86). A valid cue resulted in a smaller Fröhlich effect than did an invalid cue, and this is consistent with findings that valid cues facilitate and invalid cues interfere with judgment Figure 3. The probability of a same response as a function of probe position in Experiment 2.

of subsequent stimuli (e.g., Posner et al., 1977). Direction influenced displacement, F(1,15) = 10.92, p <.005, with ascending motion (M = 9.15) exhibiting a smaller Fröhlich effect than did descending motion (M = 20.21), and this is consistent with representational gravity. Experiment 3 Experiment 3 presented the same auditory targets and auditory probes as in Experiment 1, but on half of the trials, a visual cue indicating whether the initial pitch of the target would be relatively low or relatively high was presented, and on half of the trials, no cue was presented. Method Participants. The participants were 15 undergraduates from the same participant pool as in Experiment 1, and none had participated in the previous experiments. Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as in Experiment 1. Stimuli. The targets and probes were the same as in Experiment 1. The cue was the visual cross used in Experiment 1. If auditory motion ascended, the cue appeared near the bottom of the display, and if auditory motion descended, the cue appeared near the top of the display. Each participant received 216 trials (2 cues [present, absent] x 9 probes [-80, -60, -40, -20, 0, +20, +40, +60, +80] x 2 directions [ascending, descending] x 6 replications) in a different random order. Procedure. The procedure was the same as for trials in which the cue appeared in Experiment 1, except: If a cue was presented, there was a 500 msec blank interval between when the participants initiated a trial and when the visual cue was presented. The visual cue was visible for 250 msec, then there was a blank interval of 250 msec before the target appeared. If a cue was not presented, there was a 1000 msec blank interval between when the participant initiated a trial and when the target appeared. After participants judged the probe, they pressed a designated key to indicate if the visual cue had been located near the top or bottom of the display or if a visual cue had not been presented. Results and Discussion Probabilities of a same response are shown in Figure 4, and WMs were calculated as in Experiment 1. If the cue was present, WMs for ascending motion (M = 6.45), t(14) = 2.68, p <.02, and for descending motion (M = 21.33), t(14) = 4.66, p <.0005, were Figure 4. The probability of a same response as a function of probe position in Experiment 3.

larger than zero. If the cue was absent, WMs for ascending motion (M = 8.18), t(14) = 3.55, p <.004, and for descending motion (M = 17.63), t(14) = 7.84, p <.002, were larger than zero. As in Experiments 1 and 2, a Fröhlich effect occurred. Cue did not influence displacement, F(1,14) = 0.82, p >.38, and there was no influence of whether a cue was present (M = 13.89) or absent (M = 12.91). Direction influenced displacement, F(1,14) = 7.70, p <.02, with ascending motion (M = 7.32) exhibiting a smaller Fröhlich effect than did descending motion (M = 19.48), and this is consistent with representational gravity. If the cue was present, participants correctly indicated the location of the cue on 93% of the trials; if the cue was absent, participants correctly indicated the absence of the cue on 97% of the trials. General Discussion Memory for the initial pitch of an auditory target that ascended or descended in frequency space was displaced in the direction of pitch motion. This is consistent with a Fröhlich effect in memory for the initial location of a moving visual target in physical space. Displacement for descending pitch motion was larger than displacement for ascending pitch motion, and this is consistent with effects of representational gravity on memory for ascending or descending visual targets (Hubbard, 1997). Displacement was decreased if an auditory cue that indicated the initial pitch of the target was presented prior to appearance of the target in Experiments 1 and 2, and this is consistent with findings regarding visual cueing of initial location on the Fröhlich effect for visual targets (Müsseler & Aschersleben, 1998), but displacement was not influenced by a visual cue presented prior to appearance of the target in Experiment 3. Some of the same spatial biases found in the representation of visual targets in physical space appear to occur in the representation of auditory targets moving in frequency space. References Getzmann, S. (2005). Shifting the onset of a moving sound source: A Fröhlich effect in spatial hearing. Hearing Research, 210, 104-111. Hubbard, T. L. (1997). Target size and displacement along the axis of implied gravitational attraction: Effects of implied weight and evidence of representational gravity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1484-1493. Hubbard, T. L., & Ruppel, S. E. (in press). Effects of spatial cueing on the onset repulsion effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. Kerzel, D. (2010). The Fröhlich effect: Past and present. In R. Nijhawan & B. Khurana (Eds.), Space and time in perception and action. New York: Cambridge University Press. Melara, R. D., & Marks, L. E. (1990). Processes underlying dimensional interactions: Correspondences between linguistic and nonlinguistic dimensions. Memory & Cognition, 18, 477-495. Müsseler, J., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: The Fröhlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 683-695. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1977). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In H. L. Pick & I. J. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113-129. Shepard, R. N. (1982). Geometrical approximations to the structure of musical pitch. Psychological Review, 89, 305-333. Thornton, I. M. (2002). The onset repulsion effect. Spatial Vision, 15, 219-243.