Using the Rey Osterrieth and Modified Taylor Complex Figures with Older Adults: A Preliminary Examination of Accuracy Score Comparability

Similar documents
OLDER ADULTS LEARNING, MEMORY, AND COPY PERFORMANCE ON THE REY-OSTERRIETH AND MODIFIED TAYLOR COMPLEX FIGURES

PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) ABSTRACT

Normative Data for the Rey Osterrieth and the Taylor Complex Figure Tests in Quebec-French People

Hubley Depression Scale for Older Adults (HDS-OA): Reliability, Validity, and a Comparison to the Geriatric Depression Scale

Trail Making Test part A part B. Mild Cognitive Impairment MCI MCI. Kennedy 1981 Bornstein Rey Osterrieth 3. Berry 1991.

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2018 Columbia University

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY Course Syllabus, Spring 2015 Columbia University

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Geriatric performance on the Neurobehavioral Cognitive Status Examination (Cognistat) What is normal?

COMPARISON OF THE BECK DEPRESSION INVENTORY-II AND GERIATRIC DEPRESSION SCALE AS SCREENS FOR DEPRESSION IN CARDIAC PATIENTS

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Detecting Dementia With the Mini-Mental State Examination in Highly Educated Individuals

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults

Comparison of assessments of fitness to drive for people with dementia

The Five-Point Test: Reliability, Validity and Normative Data for Children and Adults

A developmental study of scores of the Boston Qualitative Scoring System

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Key words : I. Broca, P 150. Broca. Vol. 19 No

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32

Minimizing Misdiagnosis: Psychometric Criteria for Possible or Probable Memory Impairment

SOCIABLE - NEXT GENERATION COGNITIVE TRAINING USING MULTI-TOUCH SURFACE COMPUTERS

Intro to Executive Functioning Across the Lifespan. Agenda. The Construct of Executive Functioning. Construct of Executive Functioning

Restrictions of the Mini-Mental State Examination in acute stroke

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

A semantic verbal fluency test for English- and Spanish-speaking older Mexican-Americans

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

Elderly Norms for the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised*

Recognizing Dementia can be Tricky

Healthy Children Get Low Scores Too: Prevalence of Low Scores on the NEPSY-II in Preschoolers, Children, and Adolescents

CHAPTER 2 CRITERION VALIDITY OF AN ATTENTION- DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER (ADHD) SCREENING LIST FOR SCREENING ADHD IN OLDER ADULTS AGED YEARS


Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

22 nd Alzheimer Europe Conference Changing perceptions, practice and policy Vienna (4-6 October 2012)

DSM-5 MAJOR AND MILD NEUROCOGNITIVE DISORDERS (PAGE 602)

Pain Assessment in Elderly Patients with Severe Dementia

Evaluation of Preventive Care Program for Cognitive Function Decline among Community-dwelling Frail Elderly People A Pilot Study

Quantitative analysis for a cube copying test

Korean-VCI Harmonization Standardization- Neuropsychology Protocol (K-VCIHS-NP)

Validity and Reliability Benton visual-retention test in braininjured diagnosis of 8 to 10 years old children in Tehran

Copyright 2002 American Academy of Neurology. Volume 58(8) 23 April 2002 pp

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Supplemental Information. Acquiring the Knowledge. of London s Layout. Drives Structural Brain Changes. Katherine Woollett and Eleanor A.

NEUROPSYCHOMETRIC TESTS

LOTCA Assessment review. Georgina Wrack. University of the Sunshine Coast

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE

Running head: BEHAVIORAL ASSESSMENT TOOLS TO IDENTIFY PAIN. Effectiveness of a Behavior Assessment Tool to Identify Pain in Patients with Dementia

Trail making test A 2,3. Memory Logical memory Story A delayed recall 4,5. Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT) 2,6

Rapidly-administered short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rd edition

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Presented By: Yip, C.K., OT, PhD. School of Medical and Health Sciences, Tung Wah College

INTERPRETING IRT PARAMETERS: PUTTING PSYCHOLOGICAL MEAT ON THE PSYCHOMETRIC BONE

Assessing cognition in ELDERLY drivers

Comparison of Predicted-difference, Simple-difference, and Premorbid-estimation methodologies for evaluating IQ and memory score discrepancies

William C Miller, PhD, FCAOT Professor Occupational Science & Occupational Therapy University of British Columbia Vancouver, BC, Canada

Pathology of Visual Memory in Patients with Epilepsy

Laterality Differences in Quantitative and Qualitative Hooper Performance

Final Report of Activity February 21 st, 2006 to April 30 th, 2006 CHEO Grant 052

battery assessing general cognitive functioning (Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE),

Title. CitationAustralasian Journal on Ageing, 31(3): Issue Date Doc URL. Rights. Type. File Information


Running head: CPPS REVIEW 1

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

Assessment of Memory

Information Gathering Obtaining history is the most critical first step Patient-provided history may not be reliable Need info from relatives, friends

VALIDATION OF TWO BODY IMAGE MEASURES FOR MEN AND WOMEN. Shayna A. Rusticus Anita M. Hubley University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

COGNITION PART TWO HIGHER LEVEL ASSESSMENT FUNCTIONAL ASSESSMENT

Depression and Loneliness in Geriatric Communities. By: Vanessa Billingsley, MS4

examination in the initial assessment of overdose patients

The reliability of production strategy scores for the Ruff Figural Fluency Test

Performance profiles and cut-off scores on the Memory Assessment Scales

Recognition of Alzheimer s Disease: the 7 Minute Screen

Cognitive assessments in multicultural populations using the Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Table 2B: Summary of Select Screening and Initial Assessment Tools for Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients (Updated 2014)

The NART as an index of prior intellectual functioning: a retrospective validity study covering a 66-year interval

Table 7.2B: Summary of Select Screening Tools for Assessment of Vascular Cognitive Impairment in Stroke Patients

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES)

The relation of education and gender on the attention items of the Mini-Mental State Examination in Spanish speaking Hispanic elders

Relationship between Cognitive Impairment and Depressive Symptoms

DOWNLOAD PDF THE EFFECT OF AEROBIC EXERCISE ON INFORMATION PROCESSING IN OLDER ADULTS

Cognitive Screening in Risk Assessment. Geoffrey Tremont, Ph.D. Rhode Island Hospital & Alpert Medical School of Brown University.

The detection and management of pain in patients with dementia in acute care settings: development of a decision tool: Research protocol.

The Effectiveness of EEG Biofeedback and Cognitive Rehabilitation as Treatments for Moderate to Severe Traumatic Brain Injury

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised: Norms for Elderly African Americans

Quality of Life Among Adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Comparative Study Between the Three Presentations of ADHD

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

AGED SPECIFIC ASSESSMENT TOOLS. Anna Ciotta Senior Clinical Neuropsychologist Peninsula Mental Health Services

ADMS Sampling Technique and Survey Studies

Test Assessment Description Ref. Global Deterioration Rating Scale Dementia severity Rating scale of dementia stages (2) (4) delayed recognition

One-Month Test Retest Reliability of the ImPACT Test Battery

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Number perseveration in healthy subjects: Does prolonged stimulus exposure influence performance on a serial addition task?

William W. Hale III, 1 Quinten A. W. Raaijmakers, 1 Anne van Hoof, 2 and Wim H. J. Meeus 1,3. 1. Introduction

Meniere s Disease Case. Suzanne Beason-Hazen, Ph.D.

Neuropsychological Testing (NPT)

Global Processing Training to Improve Visuospatial Memory Deficits after Right-Brain Stroke

Chapter 7. Depression and cognitive impairment in old age: what comes first?

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist

منتخب بهترین مجلات در حوزه روانپزشکی تهیه و تنظیم:مهشید تسلیمی کارشناس ارزشیابی مرکز تحقیقات روان تنی

T. Rune Nielsen, PhD

Memory Retraining with Adult Male Alcoholics

Transcription:

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 Using the Rey Osterrieth and Modified Taylor Complex Figures with Older Adults: A Preliminary Examination of Accuracy Score Comparability Anita M. Hubley* Department of ECPS, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada *Corresponding author at: Department of ECPS, The University of British Columbia, 2125 Main Mall, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4. Tel.: þ1-604-822-9223; fax: þ1-604-822-3302. E-mail address: anita.hubley@ubc.ca (A.M. Hubley). Accepted 8 January 2010 Abstract Although considerable research has now shown that the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) and its original companion figure, the Taylor Complex Figure (TCF), are not comparable measures of visuospatial learning and memory, recent studies have provided evidence to suggest that the Modified TCF (MTCF) is a comparable measure to the ROCF. The primary aim of the present study was to examine the comparability of ROCF and MTCF accuracy scores with older adults using the traditional incidental learning procedure. A secondary aim was to examine whether performance on the two figures showed comparable gender effects and relationships with age and education. Comparable recall performance, but not copy performance, was found for the two figures in this sample of older adults. No gender differences were found on either figure and similar relationships with age and education were reported for the two figures. These findings are discussed within the context of previous research with consideration given to the clinical implications of the findings and future research recommendations. Keywords: Complex figure; Memory; Visuospatial abilities; Validity; Score equivalence; Older adults Introduction Complex figures, such as the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF; Rey, 1941), are commonly used to assess visuospatial, perceptual, and executive functioning in older adults (e.g., Gasparini et al., 2008; Grace et al., 2005; Ivgi, Beeri, Rabinowitz, & Davidson, 1999; Lincoln, Drummond, & Berman, 1997; Marshall et al., 2007; Qi-hao, Jun-chao, & Zhen, 2005; Robinson et al., 2007; Uc, Rizzo, Anderson, Qian, et al., 2006, Uc, Rizzo, Anderson, Sparks, et al., 2005). At times, one may simply want to use an alternate figure to which the person has not been exposed (e.g., due to a previous assessment, clinical training, or a breach of test security). More commonly, there are a number of situations (e.g., surgery, drug treatment, rehabilitation) in which a clinician or researcher may want to evaluate visuospatial, perceptual, and/or executive functioning more than once. Complex figures are often used in these situations but, to minimize practice effects associated with using the same complex figure twice (or multiple times as required in some situations), it is recommended procedure to use a different, but equally difficult, complex figure (or figures). At one time, the Taylor Complex Figure (TCF; Taylor, 1969) was treated as though it was comparable to the ROCF and the figures were used interchangeably in both clinical and research settings. However, subsequent research has clearly demonstrated that the TCF is less difficult to learn and remember than the ROCF (e.g., Delaney, Prevey, Cramer, & Mattson, 1992; Hamby, Wilkins, & Barry, 1993; Strauss & Spreen, 1990; Tombaugh, Faulkner, & Hubley, 1992; Yamashita, 2006). Since then, the TCF was revised to make it more difficult (Hubley, 1996; Hubley, Tombaugh, & Hemingway, 2003) and several studies have demonstrated that the Modified TCF (MTCF) provides comparable accuracy scores to the ROCF (Hubley & Jassal, 2006; Hubley & Tremblay, 2002; Yamashita, 2006). # The Author 2010. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org. doi:10.1093/arclin/acq003 Advance Access publication on 4 February 2010

198 A.M. Hubley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 Surprisingly little research comparing alternate forms of complex figures has been conducted with older adults (Hubley & Tombaugh, 2003) and no studies comparing the ROCF and MTCF have focused solely on this age group. One reason for comparing the ROCF and MTCF in a sample comprised only of older adults is that age effects have been documented consistently in the recall of complex figures and less consistently in the reproduction (i.e., copy) of these figures (Hartman & Potter, 1998; Hubley & Tombaugh, 2003; Knight, 2003). Thus, is performance on the two figures comparable when examined solely in older adults? The issue here is about the level of difficulty of these figures for older adults given age-related changes to executive functioning, learning, and memory. On the one hand, any potential differences between the ROCF and MTCF might be masked by the increased difficulty of the task for older adults (for an example of this phenomenon with children, see Sadeh, Ariel, & Inbar, 1996). On the other hand, it is possible that any subtle differences in difficulty reproducing or recalling the two figures (given the individual features or the gestalt) might become more apparent with older adults (see the findings for children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder [ADHD] in Sadeh et al., 1996, and commentary by Hubley & Tombaugh, 2003). For clinicians and researchers working with older adults and in situations in which alternate forms of the complex figure test are needed (e.g., diagnosis of, and tracking cognitive change in, cases such as mild cognitive impairment and dementia), a comparison of performance on the ROCF and MTCF specifically with an older sample will be informative. The primary aim of the present study was to determine if MTCF accuracy scores were comparable to those on the ROCF using an incidental learning procedure with older adults. Given past research comparing the ROCF and MTCF, it was hypothesized that there would be no statistically significant differences in mean accuracy scores obtained on the MTCF and ROCF on copy, immediate recall (IR), or delayed recall (DR) trials. A secondary aim of this study was to examine whether performance on the ROCF and MTCF showed comparable gender effects and relationships with age and education. It was hypothesized that similar gender effects and similar patterns of relationships with age and education would be obtained with the two figures. Materials and Methods Participants Study participants were recruited using (a) posters or recruitment tables placed in various locations in the community (e.g., shopping malls, pharmacies, grocery stores, seniors groups), (b) a list of individuals from the community who had volunteered to take part in future research, (c) advertisements in various local newsletters to seniors, and (d) word of mouth. No honoraria were provided for participation. Inclusionary criteria were that study participants had to be at least 55 years of age, living independently in the community, and speak and read English as a first language. Exclusionary criteria were that the participants could not have a history of stroke, seizures, multiple heart attacks, or serious head injury; nor could they have a current diagnosis of dementia or screen positive for depression or cognitive impairment. Participants consisted of 64 predominantly White community-dwelling older adults (16 men and 48 women) aged 55 78 years (M ¼ 65.5, SD ¼ 6.33) living in a small northern Canadian city. Their years of education ranged from 9 to 18 (M ¼ 12.9, SD ¼ 2.37). Nearly all (93.8%; n ¼ 60) were right-handed. A between-groups design was used wherein participants received either the ROCF (n ¼ 32) or the MTCF (n ¼ 32). Participants in the two groups were individually matched for age (within 4 years; ROCF: M ¼ 65.6, SD ¼ 6.23; MTCF: M ¼ 65.5, SD ¼ 6.53; t (62) ¼ 0.06, p ¼.95, d ¼ 0.02) and education (within 2 years; ROCF: M ¼ 12.9, SD ¼ 2.48; MTCF: M ¼ 12.8, SD ¼ 2.29; t (62) ¼ 0.16, p ¼.88, d ¼ 0.04). There were 7 men and 25 women in the ROCF condition and 9 men and 23 women in the MTCF condition. A x 2 test for independence (using Yates Continuity Correction and Fisher s Exact Test) indicated no significant association between gender and figure, x 2 (1, n ¼ 64) ¼ 0.083, p ¼.77, phi ¼ 20.07. Participants were asked about the following medical history conditions and, with the exception of diabetes, showed similar numbers (ROCF:MTCF) of participants reporting such conditions: Heart attack and other heart conditions (3:3), hypertension (11:10), vascular disease or stroke (0:0), thyroid problems (8:6), seizures or epilepsy (0:0), diabetes (1:5), head injury/concussion (2:3), diagnosed dementia (0:0), other notable medical conditions (0:0), and no medical conditions (14:12). All participants scored.24 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; M ¼ 28.6, SD ¼ 1.28), indicating no significant cognitive impairment on this screen, and 14 on the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; M ¼ 4.1, SD ¼ 3.26), indicating no significant depressive symptomatology on this screen. Materials A demographic form recording sex, birthdate, age, handedness, years of education, and medical history was completed via interview. The tests administered included the MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), GDS (Yesavage et al., 1983), and either the ROCF or MTCF. The standard 36-point scoring system developed by Osterrieth (1944) and refined by Taylor (1959)

A.M. Hubley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 199 and Taylor (1989, as cited in Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006) was used to score the ROCF. A very similar 36-point scoring system, developed by Hubley (1998) that also focuses on accuracy and placement, was used to score the MTCF. (The MTCF figure and an abbreviated version of the scoring system can be viewed in Lezak, Howieson, and Loring [2004]orStrauss et al. [2006]; a detailed version of the scoring system can be obtained from the author upon request.) Two scorers (the author, as primary scorer, and a senior undergraduate psychology student research assistant, SJ) independently scored all the complex figure reproductions for each study participant. Finally, a series of questionnaires about aging (e.g., knowledge of aging, anxiety about aging, social value of the elderly, subjective age) not relevant to the present study question were used as filler tasks during the DR interval of the complex figure test. Design and Procedure Each participant was tested individually by trained senior undergraduate student research assistants. Participants first were administered the MMSE and the GDS. Next, they were shown the complex figure and asked to copy the design as accurately as they could while observing the figure. This task employed the traditional incidental learning procedure in which the participants were not informed in advance that there would be IR and DR trials following the copy trial. Immediately after their copies were completed and removed, participants were asked to reproduce the complex figure from memory (IR). Upon completion of this task, participants spent the next 20 min completing a series of questionnaires on aging as filler tasks. Participants who had not completed the questionnaires within the delay interval were asked to put the questionnaires aside momentarily and then reproduced the complex figure one last time (DR). Finally, participants completed the questionnaires on aging, if they had not already done so. Results Inter-scorer reliabilities for the ROCF and MTCF are reported using both Pearson s r and the intraclass correlation (ICC). The ICC reported here uses a two-way random effects model that provides a measure of inter-scorer agreement (rather than consistency) when the scores from a single (rather than averaged) scorer will be used for subsequent analyses (Laschinger, 1992; McGraw & Wong, 1996; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). The use of absolute agreement provides a stricter criterion because scores that differ in absolute value are treated as disagreements. Table 1 shows the obtained Pearson s r and ICC values. Only the scores from the primary scorer were used in subsequent analyses. Gender differences were examined on each of the ROCF and MTCF trials. Given some non-normality in the data, nonparametric statistics were conducted. A series of Mann Whitney U-tests revealed that no significant differences in accuracy score performance were found between men and women on the ROCF trials (copy: U ¼ 70.50, z ¼ 20.78, p ¼.43, r ¼.14; IR: U ¼ 79.50, z ¼ 20.37, p ¼.72, r ¼.06; DR: U ¼ 53.50, z ¼ 21.55, p ¼.12, r ¼.27) or the MTCF trials (copy: U ¼ 96.50, z ¼ 20.30, p ¼.76, r ¼.05; IR: U ¼ 86.50, z ¼ 20.71, p ¼.48, r ¼.13; DR: U ¼ 100.50, z ¼ 21.26, p ¼.90, r ¼.02). Copy and recall trial performance for the ROCF and MTCF is shown in Table 2. Again, given some non-normality in the data, nonparametric statistics were conducted. The results of a Mann Whitney U-test revealed a statistically significant difference between the figures in performance on copy, U ¼ 310.50, z ¼ 22.75, p ¼.006, r ¼.34, with moderately higher performance on the MTCF (Median ¼ 35.0) than the ROCF (Median ¼ 32.0). There were no significant differences in performance between the two figures on IR, U ¼ 416.50, z ¼ 21.28, p ¼.20, r ¼.16, or DR, U ¼ 468.00, z ¼ 20.59, p ¼.55, r ¼.07. Given that the statistically significant difference between the figures on the copy trial could impact recall performance, Mann Whitney U-tests were conducted on percent-retained scores (i.e., recall scores divided by copy scores and multiplied by 100). These tests revealed no significant differences between the figures in percent retained performance at IR, U ¼ 454.0, z ¼ 20.78, p ¼.44, r ¼.10, or DR, U ¼ 511.0, z ¼ 20.01, p ¼.99, r ¼.002. Table 1. Inter-scorer reliability estimates for the ROCF and MTCF Trial ROCF MTCF Pearson s r ICC Pearson s r ICC Copy.86.81.84.73 IR.97.97.98.97 DR.97.97.98.98 Notes: ROCF ¼ Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure; MTCF ¼ Modified Taylor Complex Figure; ICC ¼ intraclass correlation; IR ¼ immediate recall; DR ¼ delayed recall.

200 A.M. Hubley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 Table 2. Performance on the ROCF and MTCF using an incidental procedure Figure Trials Copy IR 20-min DR ROCF (n ¼ 32) M 32.1 15.6 16.3 SD 2.69 4.80 4.14 MTCF (n ¼ 32) M 33.7 18.0 17.5 SD 2.66 6.94 6.06 Notes: ROCF ¼ Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure; MTCF ¼ Modified Taylor Complex Figure; IR ¼ immediate recall; DR ¼ delayed recall. Table 3. Correlations of the ROCF and MTCF with age and education Age Education Copy ROCF 2.18.32 MTCF 2.30.17 Fisher s Z 0.49 0.61 IR ROCF.18.22 MTCF 2.14.21 Fisher s Z 1.23 0.04 DR ROCF.14.33 MTCF 2.31.37 a Fisher s Z 1.76 0.17 Notes: ROCF ¼ Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure; MTCF ¼ Modified Taylor Complex Figure; IR ¼ immediate recall; DR ¼ delayed recall; Education ¼ years of education; critical values for Fisher s Z ¼ 1.96 for p,.05 and 2.58 for p,.01; ROCF: n ¼ 32; MTCF: n ¼ 32. a p,.05. Correlations between performance on the complex figures and each of age and education were examined to determine whether the pattern of correlations were similar for the ROCF and MTCF. The results are shown in Table 3. Using Fisher s Z-tests to compare these coefficients, none of the correlations were statistically significantly different between the ROCF and MTCF. Overall, the correlations tended to be small (ranging between r ¼ 2.31 and.37) and nonsignificant. Discussion The primary purpose of the present study was to examine the comparability of accuracy score performance between the ROCF and MTCF using an incidental learning procedure with older adults. On the basis of previous research, it was hypothesized that there would be no statistically significant differences in mean accuracy scores obtained on the MTCF and the ROCF. This hypothesis was only partially supported. There were no significant differences in IR and DR trial performance on the ROCF and the MTCF, which suggest that these two figures do provide comparable accuracy scores when assessing visuospatial memory using an incidental procedure in older adults. This finding is consistent with previous research comparing the ROCF and the MTCF in samples of younger adults (Hubley & Tremblay, 2002; Yamashita, 2006) and in a mixed age adult sample (Hubley & Jassal, 2006). Surprisingly, however, the MTCF was found to be significantly easier than the ROCF to copy (i.e., visuospatial construction) in the present sample of older adults. There was an average difference of 1.6 points (out of 36) between the mean copy scores on the two figures, which is a low moderate effect and on the borderline of being clinically meaningful. This unexpected finding is contrary to all previous research comparing the ROCF and the MTCF (Hubley & Jassal, 2006; Hubley & Tremblay, 2002; Yamashita, 2006) and even research that has been conducted with older adults comparing the ROCF and the original and easier TCF (Berry, Allen, & Schmitt, 1991; Kuehn & Snow, 1992; Vingerhoets, Lannoo, & Wolters, 1998). At present, the current study supports the comparability of the two figures when used to assess visuospatial recall when used with older adults, but calls into question the comparability of the two figures when used to assess visuospatial construction in this group. Further research is needed to determine whether this is a robust finding with older adults or reflects a Type I error. If the finding is

A.M. Hubley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 201 robust, then either a modification needs to be applied to MTCF design (which would require an exploration of the cause of the differences in copy performance in the figures across studies) or a correction needs to be applied to older adults copy scores on the MTCF. A secondary purpose of this study was to examine whether performance on the ROCF and the MTCF showed comparable gender effects and relationships with age and education. It was hypothesized that similar gender effects and similar patterns of relationships with age and education would be obtained with the two figures. These hypotheses were supported. No gender differences in accuracy scores were found on either the ROCF or the MTCF. This finding is consistent with the majority of research on gender differences in complex figures (see summary by Hubley & Tombaugh, 2003). The few studies that have reported gender differences (e.g., Bennett-Levy, 1984; Tombaugh, Faulkner, et al., 1992) have found that these differences were negligible. Correlations between figure performance and age were small (ranging from r ¼ 2.31 to.18) and nonsignificant on both figures. This finding is contrary to that of previous research (e.g., Berry et al., 1991; Boone, Lesser, Hill-Gutierrez, Berman, & D Elia, 1993; Chiulli, Haaland, LaRue, & Garry, 1995; Gallagher & Burke, 2007; Hubley & Jassal, 2006; Knight, 2003; Rosselli & Ardila, 1991; Vingerhoets et al., 1998) but may reflect the restricted age range present in a sample of older adults. Correlations between figure performance and education were small and positive (ranging from r ¼.17 to.39) and, with one minor exception, nonsignificant. These findings seem to reflect much of the inconsistency in the literature; that is, some research has found that education has little relationship with complex figure performance (e.g., Berry et al., 1991; Boone et al., 1993; Delaney et al., 1992; Hubley & Jassal, 2006), whereas other research reports positive correlations between education and either copy or recall performance (e.g., Ardila & Rosselli, 2003; Frazier, Adams, Strauss, & Redline, 2001; Vingerhoets et al., 1998). More importantly, in the present study, none of the correlations between complex figure performance and each of age and education were statistically significantly different between the ROCF and the MTCF. Only three studies appear to have conducted comparability studies by examining the respective patterns of correlations between different complex figure scores and demographic variables (Frazier et al., 2001; Hubley & Jassal, 2006; Vingerhoets et al., 1998). In the only previous study comparing the ROCF and the MTCF, Hubley and Jassal (2006) also found a similar pattern of correlations between the ROCF and the MTCF. The present study has focused on examining the comparability of ROCF and MTCF accuracy scores in a cognitively intact community sample of older adults using a between-groups design and an incidental procedure. In exploring the comparability of the ROCF and the MTCF, there are other types of scores, samples, research designs, and administration procedures that can also be used and should be explored in future research. The vast majority of complex figure comparability studies have focused on accuracy scores. However, many clinicians and researchers believe that information about the process (e.g., strategies, sequencing, errors) used in the construction or recall of a complex figure provides additional unique information (Hubley, 2006; Poreh, 2006). Information about the comparability of ROCF and MTCF process scores may also inform research on the comparability of accuracy scores for the two figures. Whereas the present study focused on a community sample of older adults, future research should explore the comparability of ROCF and MTCF accuracy and process scores in clinical samples of older adults (e.g., dementia, stroke). The present study also used a between-groups design and an incidental procedure in the administration of the ROCF and MTCF. Between group designs are commonly used to assess the comparability of different complex figures and have particular advantages (e.g., easier sample recruitment, less sample attrition, less restrictive statistical assumptions) but also some disadvantages (e.g., less control over individual differences between groups). Future research should also consider using a within-subjects design with counterbalancing. Notably, although the incidental procedure used in the present study is the most commonly used method of administering the complex figure test, there are a variety of potential problems with this approach, especially when the complex figures are administered in a within-subjects (test retest) design (Hubley & Tombaugh, 2003; Tombaugh, Schmidt, & Faulkner, 1992). For example, a participant who has been administered the complex figure using an incidental procedure during the test portion often expects the memory component when a similar figure is presented during the retest portion and thus shifts to a more intentional approach for the second figure. Research examining the comparability of ROCF and MTCF accuracy (and process) scores in older adults should also be conducted using an intentional learning procedure, in which the study participant is informed in advance that there is a memory component to the test and multiple learning trials can be incorporated. Acknowledgements I would like to thank Linda Lundquist, Kim Scoffield, and Loriann Quinlan for their assistance with data collection and Surita Jassal for her scoring of the ROCF and MTCF to provide interscorer reliability data.

202 A.M. Hubley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 Funding This study was supported by a research grant from the University of British Columbia. Conflict of Interest A.M.H. is the developer of, and holds the copyright on, the Modified Taylor Complex Figure (MTCF). References Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (2003). Educational effects on ROCF performance. In J. A. Knight, & E. F. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure usage: Clinical and research applications (pp. 659 667). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Bennett-Levy, J. (1984). Determinants of performance on the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure Test: An analysis, and a new technique for single-case assessment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 109 119. Berry, D. T. R., Allen, R. S., & Schmitt, F. A. (1991). Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure: Psychometric characteristics in a geriatric sample. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 143 153. Boone, K. B., Lesser, I. M., Hill-Gutierrez, E., Berman, N. G., & D Elia, L. F. (1993). Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure performance in healthy, older adults: Relationship to age, education, sex, and IQ. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 22 28. Chiulli, S. J., Haaland, K. Y., LaRue, A., & Garry, P. J. (1995). Impact of age on drawing the Rey Osterrieth Figure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 219 224. Delaney, R. C., Prevey, M. L., Cramer, J., Mattson, R. H., & the VA Epilepsy Cooperative Study #264 Research Group. (1992). Test retest comparability and control subject data for the Rey-Auditory Verbal Learning Test and the Rey Osterrieth/Taylor Complex Figures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7, 523 528. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state : A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189 198. Frazier, T. W., Adams, N. L., Strauss, M. E., & Redline, S. (2001). Comparability of the Rey and Mack forms of the Complex Figure Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 337 344. Gallagher, C., & Burke, T. (2007). Age, gender, and IQ effects on the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46, 35 45. Gasparini, M., Masciarelli, G., Vanacore, N., Ottaviani, D., Salati, E., Talarico, G., et al. (2008). A descriptive study on constructional impairment in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer s disease. European Journal of Neurology, 15, 589 597. Grace, J., Amick, M. M., D Abreu, A., Festa, E. K., Heindel, W. C., & Ott, B. R. (2005). Neuropsychological deficits associated with driving performance in Parkinson s and Alzheimer s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 766 775. Hamby, S. L., Wilkins, J. W., & Barry, N. S. (1993). Organizational quality on the Rey Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figure tests: A new scoring system. Psychological Assessment, 5, 27 33. Hartman, M., & Potter, G. (1998). Sources of age differences on the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 513 524. Hubley, A. M. (1996). Modification of the Taylor Complex Figure: A comparable figure to the Rey Osterrieth Figure? Edgeworth Series in Quantitative Behavioural Science, Paper No. ESQBS-96-7. University of Northern British Columbia, Prince George, BC, Canada. Hubley, A. M. (1998). Scoring guidelines for the Modified Taylor Complex Figure (MTCF). Unpublished manuscript, University of Northern British Columbia, Prince George, BC, Canada. Hubley, A. M. (2006). Quantified process scoring of complex figures. In A. Poreh (Ed.), The quantified process approach to neuropsychological assessment. Lisse, the Netherlands: Psychology Press. Hubley, A. M., & Jassal, S. (2006). Comparability of the Rey Osterrieth and Modified Taylor Complex Figures using total scores, completion times, and construct validation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 1482 1497. Hubley, A. M., & Tombaugh, T. N. (2003). Comparability of the Rey Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures. In J. A. Knight, & E. F. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure usage: Clinical and research applications (pp. 283 296). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Hubley, A. M., Tombaugh, T. N., & Hemingway, D. (2003). A modification of the Taylor Figure and the development of new figures for older adults. In J. A. Knight, & E. F. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure usage: Clinical and research applications (pp. 297 311). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Hubley, A. M., & Tremblay, D. (2002). Comparability of total score performance on the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure and a Modified Taylor Complex Figure. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 370 382. Ivgi, M., Beeri, M. S., Rabinowitz, J., & Davidson, M. (1999). A naturalistic study comparing the efficacy of a memory enhancement course to a general academic course. International Psychogeriatrics, 11, 281 287. Knight, J. A. (2003). ROCF psychometric characteristics and normative data. In J. A. Knight, & E. F. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure usage: Clinical and research applications (pp. 193 253). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Kuehn, S. M., & Snow, W. G. (1992). Are the Rey and Taylor figures equivalent? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7, 445 448. Laschinger, H. K. S. (1992). Intraclass correlations as estimates of interrater reliability in nursing research. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 14, 246 251. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Lincoln, N., Drummond, A. E. R., & Berman, P. (1997). Perceptual impairment and its impact on rehabilitation outcome. Disability and Rehabilitation: An International Multidisciplinary Journal, 19, 231 234. Marshall, S. C., Molnar, F., Man-Son-Hing, M., Blair, R., Brosseau, L., Finestone, H. M., et al. (2007). Predictors of driving ability following stroke: A systematic review. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 14, 98 114. McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods, 1, 30 46.

A.M. Hubley / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 25 (2010) 197 203 203 Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d une figure complexe. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 206 356. Poreh, A. M. (2006). Quantification and validation of the Rey Osterrieth global-local indices. In A. Poreh (Ed.), The quantified process approach to neuropsychological assessment. Lisse, the Netherlands: Psychology Press. Qi-hao, G., Jun-chao, L., & Zhen, H. (2005). Research for differences between neurodegenerative and vascular mild cognitive impairment with neuropsychological assessment. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13, 405 407. Rey, A. (1941). L examen psychologie dan les cas d encéphalopathie traumatique (Les problèmes). Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286 340. Robinson, R. G., Paradiso, S., Mizrahi, R., Fiedorowicz, J. G., Kouzoukas, D. E., & Moser, D. J. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates of normal variation in emotional response to visual stimuli. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 112 118. Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1991). Effects of age, education, and gender on the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, 370 376. Sadeh, M., Ariel, R., & Inbar, D. (1996). Rey Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures: Equivalent measures of visual organization and visual memory in ADHD and normal children. Child Neuropsychology, 2, 63 71. Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420 425. Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Strauss, E., & Spreen, O. (1990). A comparison of the Rey and Taylor Figures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 417 420. Taylor, E. M. (1959). Psychological appraisal of children with cerebral deficits. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Taylor, L. B. (1969). Localisation of cerebral lesions by psychological testing. Clinical Neurosurgery, 16, 269 287. Tombaugh, T. N., Faulkner, P., & Hubley, A. M. (1992). Effects of age on the Rey Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures: Test retest data using an intentional learning paradigm. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 647 661. Tombaugh, T. N., Schmidt, J. P., & Faulkner, P. (1992). A new procedure for administering the Taylor Complex Figure: Normative data over a 60-year age span. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 63 79. Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Qian, S., Rodnitzky, R. L., & Dawson, J. D. (2005). Visual dysfunction in Parkinson disease without dementia. Neurology, 65, 1907 1913. Uc, E. Y., Rizzo, M., Anderson, S. W., Sparks, J. D., Rodnitzky, R. L., & Dawson, J. D. (2006). Impaired visual search in drivers with Parkinson s disease. Annals of Neurology, 60, 407 413. Vingerhoets, G., Lannoo, E., & Wolters, M. (1998). Comparing the Rey Osterrieth and Taylor Complex Figures: Empirical data and meta-analysis. Psychologica Belgica, 38, 109 119. Yamashita, H. (2006). Comparability of the Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure, the Taylor Complex Figure, and the Modified Taylor Complex Figure in a normal sample of Japanese speakers. Psychological Reports, 99, 531 534. Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression rating scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17, 37 49.