Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Similar documents
The use of an unpleasant sound unconditional stimulus in an aversive conditioning procedure with 8- to 11- year-old children

OBJECTIVES. To examine the influence of conditioned fear on pain threshold

The renewal of extinguished conditioned fear with fearrelevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after extinction

Stimulus Typicality Determines How Broadly Fear Is Generalized

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

FEAR CONDITIONING AND EXTINCTION IN ANXIOUS AND NONANXIOUS YOUTH AND ADULTS: EXAMINING A NOVEL DEVELOPMENTALLY APPROPRIATE FEAR-CONDITIONING TASK

Learning. Learning. Habituation. Sensitization. Habituation and Sensitization

Conducting extinction in multiple contexts does not necessarily attenuate the renewal of shock expectancy in a fear-conditioning procedure with humans

Unit 6 Learning.

Baseline and affective startle modulation by angry and neutral faces in 4-8-year-old anxious and non-anxious children

The Development of Fear Learning and Generalization in 8 13 Year-Olds

A SYSTEM FOR CONTROL AND EVALUATION OF ACOUSTIC STARTLE RESPONSES OF ANIMALS

Chapter 3. Outline for this lecture

Theories of Learning

Version of record first published: 24 May Full terms and conditions of use:

CHAPTER 7 LEARNING. Jake Miller, Ocean Lakes High School

The effect of stimulus modality and task difficulty on attentional modulation of blink startle

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective

Abundant research has been devoted to understanding anxiety

PSYC 337 LEARNING. Session 4 Classical Conditioning II. Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information:

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

GCSE PSYCHOLOGY UNIT 2 LEARNING REVISION

3/7/2010. Theoretical Perspectives

I. Classical Conditioning

Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal

Associative Learning

Learning and conditioning

Learning Chapter 6 1

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015

The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselineswith and without noncontingent shock

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models. From Mechanisms of Memory, 2 nd Edition by J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans

Psychometric properties of startle and corrugator response in NPU, Affective Picture Viewing, and. Resting State tasks

Issues Surrounding the Normalization and Standardisation of Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs).

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 5

UPDATED META-ANALYSIS OF CLASSICAL FEAR CONDITIONING IN THE ANXIETY DISORDERS

Effect of Conditioned Stimulus Generalization on Regression of Fear Memory Fading

Outline. History of Learning Theory. Pavlov s Experiment: Step 1. Associative learning 9/26/2012. Nature or Nurture

PSYC 337 LEARNING. Session 3 Classical Conditioning. Lecturer: Dr. Inusah Abdul-Nasiru Contact Information:

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

27- Classical Conditioning 1 of 5

January 8. EQ- What are the major elements of classical conditioning?

Running head: INSTRUCTIONS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1. A review on the effects of verbal instructions in human fear conditioning: Empirical

operant, behaviors because most of our teaching challenges lie there. Companion parrots

Abnormalities in the Anxiety Disorders. A Thesis SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY. Brian Gregory van Meurs

Topics in Animal Cognition. Oliver W. Layton

Chapter Six. Learning. Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning Observational Learning

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

The Influence of Conditioned Fear on Human Pain Thresholds: Does Preparedness Play a Role?

Chapter 5: How Do We Learn?

A Unique Safety Signal: Social-Support Figures Enhance Rather Than Protect From Fear Extinction

The development of an attentional bias for angry faces following Pavlovian fear conditioning

Operant Conditioning B.F. SKINNER

Conditioning By Darren Burke & Caroline Tomiczek The word conditioning has two different meanings, only one of which is uncontroversial.

DEFINITION. Learning is the process of acquiring knowledge (INFORMATIN ) and new responses. It is a change in behavior as a result of experience

Effects of perceptual load on startle reflex modification at a long lead interval

Learning: Some Key Terms

Psychological Hodgepodge. Mr. Mattingly Psychology

Associative Learning

Startle modulation during emotional anticipation and perception

Classical Conditioning & Operant Conditioning

Affective reactions to briefly presented pictures

LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

Learning and Anxiety 1

Announcements. Next Aplia due Monday Exam Next Wednesday (April 2) Review session Next Monday (Mar 31) 5:30 pm,, room 130 ILC

1. Learning theory. Cambridge MRCPsych course Tue 23 Oct 2018

PROBABILITY OF SHOCK IN THE PRESENCE AND ABSENCE OF CS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

marijuana and the teen brain MARY ET BOYLE, PH. D. DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE UCSD

POST-TRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER

Spontaneous recovery. Module 18. Processes of Conditioning. Classical Conditioning (cont d)

Fear Conditioning in Panic Disorder: Enhanced Resistance to Extinction

Classical & Operant Conditioning. Learning: Principles and Applications

Learning = an enduring change in behavior, resulting from experience.

Teaching Classical and Operant Conditioning in a Laboratory-Based Course: Eight Effective Experiments

Classical Conditioning. Learning. Classical conditioning terms. Classical Conditioning Procedure. Procedure, cont. Important concepts

Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

Spacing extinction trials alleviates renewal and spontaneous recovery

Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP 1

The Most Important Thing I ve Learned. What is the most important thing you ve learned in your life? How did you learn it?

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov ( )

Standard Operating Procedure

TEMPORALLY SPECIFIC BLOCKING: TEST OF A COMPUTATIONAL MODEL. A Senior Honors Thesis Presented. Vanessa E. Castagna. June 1999

Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

Chapter 6/9: Learning

Classical Conditioning. AKA: Pavlovian conditioning

BRAIN PLASTICITY. Neuroplasticity:. Happens in at least 3 ways: - - -

Associative Learning

Learning. Learning: Problems. Chapter 6: Learning

Learning. Learning. Stimulus Learning. Modification of behavior or understanding Is it nature or nurture?

Learning. AP PSYCHOLOGY Unit 4

Some Parameters of the Second-Order Conditioning of Fear in Rats

To be diagnosed with panic disorder, a person must experience

Individual Differences in Fear Potentiated Startle in Behaviorally Inhibited Children

Psychology in Your Life

CS DURATION' UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO. in response suppression (Meltzer and Brahlek, with bananas. MH to S. P. Grossman. The authors wish to

Learning. Learning. Learning via Association 1/8/2012. Chapter Eight. Change in an organism s behavior or thought as a result of experience

an ability that has been acquired by training (process) acquisition aversive conditioning behavior modification biological preparedness

Transcription:

The use of an unpleasant sound as the unconditional stimulus in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments that involve children and adolescent participants Author Neumann, David, Waters, Allison, Westbury Ingham, Rae Published 2008 Journal Title Behavior Research Methods Copyright Statement This manuscript was accepted for publication in Behavior Research Methods, September 28, 2007. The copyright is held by Psychonomic Society Publications. This document may not exactly correspond to the final published version. Psychonomic Society Publication disclaims any responsibility or liability for errors in this manuscript. Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/10072/22452 Link to published version http://www.psychonomic.org/ Griffith Research Online https://research-repository.griffith.edu.au

B341 1 Running head: UNPLEASANT SOUND IN CONDITIONING The use of an unpleasant sound as the unconditional stimulus in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments that involve children and adolescent participants David L. Neumann, Allison M. Waters, & H. Rae Westbury School of Psychology Griffith University Queensland, Australia Corresponding author and address: David Neumann, School of Psychology, Griffith University (Gold Coast Campus), Mail: GRIFFITH UNIVERSITY QLD, Queensland, 4222, Australia, E-mail D.Neumann@griffith.edu.au, Facsimile +61(0)7 5552 8291, Telephone +61(0)7 5552 8052

B341 2 Abstract Ethical considerations can prohibit the use of traditional unconditional stimuli (USs), such as electric shock or loud tone, when children or adolescents participate in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments. The present study evaluated whether an unpleasant sound provides a viable alternative. Fifteen boys and girls aged 13 to 17 years completed a differential Pavlovian conditioning procedure in which a conditional stimulus (CS) was followed by the sound of metal scraping on slate. Acquisition of conditioned responses was found in startle blink magnitude, expectancy judgments of the sound, and skin conductance responses. Extinction of conditioned responses was found in all measures when the CS was no longer followed by the unpleasant sound. Subjective ratings and skin conductance responses indicated that the sound was unpleasant because of its qualitative features, rather than its intensity. The results support the use of an unpleasant sound as a low risk alternative to traditional USs in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments with children and adolescents. Keywords: Pavlovian conditioning; unconditional stimulus; adolescents; children; sounds

B341 3 Introduction In aversive Pavlovian conditioning, a neutral conditional stimulus (CS) is paired with an aversive unconditional stimulus (US). In research with healthy adults, researchers have traditionally used a US that is mildly painful or intense, most commonly electric shock and loud tones (see Grillon, 2002; Lissek et al., 2005). However, these stimuli present several ethical limitations when the participants are healthy children and adolescents or are individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. An electric shock requires the individual to determine an intensity that is unpleasant, but not painful during a work up procedure. Children and adolescents may not be sufficiently self-aware to make this judgement. Loud tones (e.g., 100 db presented for 1 s) may cause pain and discomfort in children and adolescents sensitive to loud stimuli, particularly those with Fragile X syndrome, autistic spectrum disorders, or some anxiety disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In addition, loud tones may be associated with poor acquisition of conditioned responses (CRs) in experiments with children (e.g., Liberman, Lipp, Spence, & March, 2006). Unpleasant odours (e.g., Flor, Birbaumer, Hermann, Ziegler, & Patrick, 2002) and air puffs (e.g., Suboski, 1967) are alternatives to traditional USs, but require special stimulus delivery systems and obtaining compliance may be difficult in some children (e.g., avoiding movement). Another stimulus that may function as a viable US in studies with children could be a sound that is unpleasant, not because of its intensity, but because of its inherent characteristics. The psychoacoustic properties of a range of sounds were studied by Halpern, Blake, and Hillenbrand (1986) in a sample of adults. The sounds included a white noise, a blender motor, compressed air sounds, and rubbing two pieces of Styrofoam together, among others. The sound that was rated as most unpleasant was produced by dragging a three-pronged garden tool across slate. Neumann and Waters (2006) made a recording of this type of sound and confirmed its effectiveness when used as a US in a Pavlovian conditioning procedure with adult college students. In addition, the unpleasant sound was rated as more unpleasant than an electric shock

B341 4 and loud tone, although the shock elicited larger skin conductance defence responses than the sound. It was suggested that the sound was effective because of its unpleasant qualitative features rather than its high intensity. The unpleasant sound may present several advantages in aversive Pavlovian conditioning experiments with children and adolescents. For example, it is presented at only a moderate intensity, does not require subjective calibration via a work up procedure, and uses equipment found in the typical laboratory (computer and headphones/speakers). However, it needs to be empirically demonstrated that children and adolescents will perceive the sound to be unpleasant and that it can serve as a viable US in an aversive Pavlovian conditioning procedure in this age group. For example, it is possible that younger participants may perceive the sound of metal scraping on slate to be less unpleasant than adults because of their shorter learning history and less opportunity for prior exposure. Evaluation of the unpleasant sound in a Pavlovian conditioning procedure To evaluate the effectiveness of the unpleasant sound in a Pavlovian conditioning procedure, seven boys and eight girls aged between 13 and 17 years (M = 16.00 years, SD = 1.19) participated in a protocol approved by the Griffith University Human Research Ethics committee. The methods followed those described by Neumann and Waters (2006). A differential conditioning procedure was used (Öhman, Hamm, & Hugdahl, 2000) by pairing one CS (CS+) with the unpleasant sound and presenting another CS (CS-) alone during conditioning. Both were presented alone during a subsequent extinction phase. The CSs were outlines of a diamond and triangle presented for 8 s. The US was a 3 s recording of a three pronged garden fork scraped over slate (see Neumann & Waters, 2006) and did not exceed a peak intensity of 83 db(a). The resulting sound resembled that of fingernails running down a chalkboard. Pavlovian CRs were indexed by (a) modulation of the startle blink reflex that was elicited by a 50 ms white noise presented at 110 db(a), (b) self-reported US expectancy, and (c) skin conductance responses (SCRs). These measures were selected because they are among the most commonly employed to

B341 5 measure aversive Pavlovian conditioning in experiments with children, adolescents, and adults (e.g., Lissek et al., 2005) and encompass both signal-based and affective learning measures. The startle blink data from two participants were not used due to non-responsiveness. Participants were instructed that they would be presented with pictures of geometric shapes and an unusual sound. They were asked to try and predict when the sound might occur and to indicate this by using an expectancy dial placed on the table. The expectancy dial could be moved about two extremes of certain sound will occur and certain sound will not occur and a middle uncertain region. During conditioning, there were 8 positive trials followed by the unpleasant sound US, interspersed with 8 negative trials. Intertrial intervals (ITIs) randomly varied between 20, 25, and 30 s. Extinction trials for both trial types followed immediately and were interspersed in a similar manner. Half of the trials (7500 ms following the CS onset) and half of the ITIs contained a blink-eliciting stimulus. Startle blinks were measured as the maximum response occurring 20 200 ms following the onset of the blink-eliciting stimulus. Trials that did not contain a blink-eliciting stimulus were used to measure SCR magnitude as the SCR-CR (latency window of 1-4 s) and SCR-UR (latency window of 9-13 s). To examine the strength of conditioning, the dependent measures were arranged into two blocks of trials during each of the conditioning and extinction phases and analysed with separate CS x Block ANOVAs. Table 1 shows the relevant statistical effects obtained for each dependent measure. Conditioning of startle blink reflexes and SCR-CRs was confirmed by significantly larger responses during the CS+ than during the CS-. The difference between the CS+ and CSvaried across blocks for expectancy judgments and SCR-URs. The interaction for expectancy reflected that the expectancy of the US increased across blocks for CS+, t = 4.62, p <.01, whereas it decreased for CS-, t = 2.99, p <.05. The interaction for SCR-URs reflected that there was a significant decline across blocks for CS+, t = 10.18, p <.01, but not for CS-, t = 2.40, p >.05. The extinction of startle blink reflexes, SCR-CRs, and SCR-URs was confirmed by no significant differences between the CS+ and CS-. The extinction of expectancy judgments was reflected in

B341 6 higher expectancy of the US during CS+ than during CS- in Block 1, t = 9.73, p <.01, but not in Block 2, t = 2.16, p >.05. ------------------------------- Insert Table 1 about here ------------------------------- The results showed that an unpleasant sound of metal scraping on slate supported the acquisition and extinction of CRs in the entire sample of 13 to 17 year old boys and girls. Conditioned responding was established in measures typically regarded as representative of fear learning (modulation of startle blink), autonomic conditioning (skin conductance responses), and expectancy learning (self-reported ratings). In addition, startle blink potentiation, often described as fear potentiated startle (Grillon, 2002), was observed only during the CS paired with the unpleasant sound. The effect sizes (η 2 p ) obtained for the difference between the CS+ and CSduring conditioning for the startle blink, expectancy, and skin conductance measures were, respectively, 0.42, 0.95, and 0.39, indicating that the unpleasant sound produced a moderate to large conditioning effect. Moreover, these values are comparable to and even exceed those obtained by Neumann and Waters (2006) in their experiment with adults (0.40, 0.80, and 0.33, respectively). Neumann and Waters (2006) also reported that the effect sizes obtained with an unpleasant sound US were similar to that obtained with electric shock and loud tone USs. Taking the prior results and the present findings together, an unpleasant sound would seem to provide a reliable US for researchers who wish to study aversive Pavlovian conditioning in children and adolescents. Evaluation of subjective ratings to the unpleasant sound To examine the subjective properties of the unpleasant sound, we also asked the same sample of participants to provide ratings to the unpleasant sound in comparison to an electric shock, loud tone (100 dba for 1000 ms), and a blink reflex eliciting stimulus (110 dba for 50 ms). The 200 ms shock was set individually for each participant to be unpleasant, but not

B341 7 painful (final mean level was 5.77 ma, SD = 2.07). Participants were presented with each stimulus in random order and after each presentation they were asked to make ratings on 9-point linear scales for pleasantness (0 = very unpleasant, 8 = very pleasant), arousal (0 = very calm, 8 = very arousing), and interest (0 = very boring, 8 = very interesting). The SCRs elicited 1-4 s after each stimulus were also measured. ------------------------------- Insert Table 2 about here ------------------------------- As shown in Table 2, the stimuli differed significantly in all subjective ratings and in SCRs as assessed by a one-way ANOVA. The sample of boys and girls perceived the unpleasant sound to be in fact unpleasant as shown by a mean pleasantness rating of 2.03 on a scale of 0 (very unpleasant) to 8 (very pleasant). Moreover, the unpleasant sound was rated as more unpleasant than the loud tone and electric shock, both ts > 2.50, p <.05. On the other hand, the shock was rated as more arousing than the loud tone, t = 4.39, p <.05, but the unpleasant sound did not differ from any stimuli, all p >.05. The unpleasant sound and shock were both rated as more interesting than the loud tone, both ts > 4.89, p <.05, and the shock was also rated as more interesting than the blink-eliciting stimulus, t = 4.13, p <.05. Finally, skin conductance defence responses elicited by the shock were larger than those elicited by the unpleasant sound, tone, and blink-eliciting stimulus, all ts > 4.19, p <.01, which themselves did not differ, all p >.05. The magnitude differences seen in the SCRs may reflect the intensity of the stimulus in that the electric shock was set at a level determined by each individual to be unpleasant, but not painful. This level was set during a shock work-up procedure and is the standard method to set the intensity of this stimulus in Pavlovian conditioning experiments. Because the shock intensity was unpleasant, but not painful, it may have been perceived to be unpleasant because it is presented at just below the pain threshold. In contrast, the moderate intensity unpleasant sound may have been perceived to be unpleasant due to its qualitative features. Neumann and Waters (2006) observed a similar pattern

B341 8 of results in a sample of adult college students with the subjective ratings to that found in the present research with a mean pleasantness rating of 1.46 on the same rating scale. The similar ratings suggest that the sound is perceived to be unpleasant to a similar degree across a wide age range. Conclusions The stimulus of an unpleasant sound of metal on slate was shown to support conditioning. Although it was subjectively rated as unpleasant, it was tolerable and presented fewer ethical concerns than other commonly used aversive stimuli such as electric shock and loud tone. On the basis of the strength of the conditioning effects observed in this study, it would seem likely that the application of the unpleasant sound to examine conditioning in very young children or children and adolescents with neurodevelopmental disorders would be successful.

B341 9 References American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Flor, H., Birbaumer, N., Hermann, C., Ziegler, S., & Patrick, C. J. (2002). Aversive Pavlovian conditioning in psychopaths: Peripheral and central correlates. Psychophysiology, 39, 505-518. Grillon, C. (2002). Startle reactivity and anxiety disorders: Aversive conditioning, context, and neurobiology. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 958-975. Halpern, D. L., Blake, R., & Hillenbrand, J. (1986). Psychoacoustics of a chilling sound. Perception and Psychophysics, 39, 77-80. Liberman, L. C., Lipp, O. V., Spence, S. H., & March, S. (2006). Evidence for retarded extinction of aversive learning in anxious children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1491-1502. Lissek, S., Powers, A. S., McClure, E. B., Phelps, E. A., Woldehawariat, G., Grillon, C., & Pine, D. S. (2005). Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1391-1424. Neumann, D. L., & Waters, A. M. (2006). The use of an unpleasant sound as an unconditional stimulus in a human aversive Pavlovian conditioning procedure. Biological Psychology, 73, 175-185. Öhman, A., Hamm, A., & Hugdahl, K. (2000). Cognition and the autonomic nervous system: Orienting, anticipation, and conditioning. In: Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.) (pp. 533-575). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Suboski, M. D. (1967). UCS intensity and the latency of the classically conditioned eyelid response. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 31-35.

B341 10 Author Note Requests for reprints should be sent to David Neumann, School of Psychology, Griffith University (Gold Coast Campus), Mail: GRIFFITH UNIVERSITY QLD, Queensland, 4222, Australia, E- mail or D.Neumann@griffith.edu.au. A computer file copy of the unpleasant sound used in this research may obtained via e-mail on request to the authors. Thanks to Stephen Brandis and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on earlier drafts of this article.

B341 11 Table 1 Mean startle blink magnitude (µv), ratings of unconditional stimulus (US) expectancy (%), skin conductance ( µs) conditioned response (SCR-CR) and unconditional response (SCR-UR) or its omission for the CS+ and CS- in both trial blocks during the conditioning and extinction phases and their associated statistical effects Measure CS+ CS- ANOVA Effect size Block 1 Block 2 Block 1 Block 2 M SD M SD M SD M SD Conditioning Startle Blink 141.2 84.19 103.7 78.16 120.2 92.78 80.98 75.39 CS, F (1, 12) = 8.59, p <.01 η 2 p =.42. Expectancy 29.13 28.10 98.37 5.92-38.93 37.68-83.73 45.51 CS x Block, F (1, 14) = 29.05, p <.0005 η 2 p =.68 SCR-CR 1.02 0.56 0.97 0.70 0.70 0.52 0.71 0.57 CS, F (1, 14) = 8.84, p <.01 η 2 p =.39 SCR-UR 1.92 0.59 0.90 0.63 0.44 0.24 0.17 0.25 CS x Block, F (1, 14) = 31.13, p <.0005 η 2 p =.69 Extinction Startle Blink 100.5 70.09 67.83 62.87 92.31 74.97 65.39 51.80 CS, F (1, 12) = 0.39, p >.05 η 2 p =.03

B341 12 Expectancy 34.30 34.37-65.00 48.17-55.57 40.34-85.27 31.25 CS x Block, F (1, 14) = 28.44, p <.0005 η 2 p =.67 SCR-CR 0.89 0.45 0.72 0.59 0.83 0.50 0.51 0.41 CS, F (1, 14) = 0.43, p >.05 η 2 p =.03 SCR-UR 0.49 0.59 0.37 0.45 0.30 0.38 0.33 0.37 CS, F (1, 14) = 1.01, p >.05 η 2 p =.09 Note Expectancy ratings range on a scale where +100 indicates maximum expectation of the US and 100 indicates maximum expectation of no US. Skin conductance responses (SCRs) for the conditional response (SCR-CR) and unconditional response (SCR-UR) were square root transformed prior to analyses to normalise the distributions

B341 13 Table 2 Mean subjective ratings for pleasantness, arousal, and interest, and mean skin conductance defence responses ( µs) for the unpleasant sound, shock, loud tone, and blink-eliciting stimulus and their associated statistical effects (standard deviations are in parentheses) Measure Stimulus ANOVA Effect size Sound Shock Loud tone Blink-eliciting stimulus M SD M SD M SD M SD Pleasantness rating 2.03 2.11 3.47 1.85 3.73 1.16 3.17 1.94 F (3, 42) = 3.39, p <.039 η 2 p =.19 Arousal rating 4.20 2.81 5.70 1.44 2.97 2.02 4.50 2.61 F (3, 42) = 6.51, p <.002 η 2 p =.32 Interest rating 4.73 2.40 6.00 1.60 1.73 1.62 3.47 2.17 F (3, 42) = 17.67, p <.0005 η 2 p =.56. Skin conductance response 1.04 0.59 2.09 0.95 0.74 0.81 0.97 0.63 F (3, 42) = 11.63, p <.0005 η 2 p =.45. Note Anchors for the ratings were for Pleasantness: 0 = very unpleasant to 8 = very pleasant; for Arousal: 0 = very calm to 8 = very arousing; and for Interest 0 = very boring to 8 = very interesting.