Tania D. Signal 1 and Nicola Taylor Attitudes to Animals: Demographics Within a Community Sample

Similar documents
Empathy and attitudes to animals

Human-Animal Violence research. A/Prof Nik Taylor & Dr Tania Signal

A Different Cut? Comparing Attitudes toward Animals and Propensity for Aggression within Two Primary Industry Cohorts Farmers and Meatworkers

Community participation in Tokyo and its suburbs: the importance of savings and interaction effects

The Swinburne National Technology and Society Monitor. Australian Centre for Emerging Technologies and Society 2006 Monitor

Nik Taylor a & Tania D. Signal b a Department of Sociology, Flinders University, Adelaide, Australia b Department of Behavioural & Social Sciences,

MARK SCHEME MAXIMUM MARK: 60

Setting The setting was the community. The economic study was conducted in Yokohama city, Japan.

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

Project RISCO Research Summary

THE EFFECTS OF OWNING A PET ON SELF-ESTEEM AND SELF-EFFICACY OF MALAYSIAN PET OWNERS

POSTGRADUATE RESEARCH EXPERIENCE A Report of the Postgraduate Research Experience Questionnaire

Table of contents. Part I. Gender equality: The economic case, social norms, and public policies

NONRESPONSE ADJUSTMENT IN A LONGITUDINAL SURVEY OF AFRICAN AMERICANS

European Data Supplement

AN INVESTIGATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TRAFFIC ENFORCEMENT AND THE PERCEIVED RISK OF DETECTION FOR DRIVING OFFENCES

The Australian Homicide Project:

Learning Objectives. Outline. The Dyadic Adjustment Scale: Utility for Rural Marital Assessment

Ethical Ideology and Moral Persuasion: Personal Moral. Philosophy, Gender, and Judgments of Pro- and Anti- Animal Research Propaganda

Holas, Jakub et al. Regionální kriminalita a její odraz v kvalitě života obyvatel Regional crime and its impact on quality of life Summary

SELF-REPORTED HISTORY OF SEXUAL COERCION AND RAPE NEGATIVELY IMPACTS RESILIENCE TO SUICIDE AMONG WOMEN STUDENTS

CHAPTER 3. Research Methodology

Your Community in Profile: Halton-Peel

2018 Homeless Enumeration Bruce County

A study of animal hoarding in NSW

What motivates volunteer participation: a summary of the Threatened Bird Network survey

Preliminary Observations of Digit Retention in Pakistani Population

Sexism 11/15/2011. Sex and Gender Biological sex Anatomical sex Genetic sex. Gender Gender roles & expectations. Gender Identity.

Wellbeing, community connections and resilience of dairy farmers

The Work Practice Questionnaire:

How accurately does the Brief Job Stress Questionnaire identify workers with or without potential psychological distress?

CHAPTER-5. Family Disorganization & Woman Desertion by Socioeconomic Background

Students Perception and Attitude Toward Hiring a Former Offender

SAQ-Adult Probation III & SAQ-Short Form

Trends in food availability in MALTA the DAFNE V project

Links between gambling availability, participation and harm a 28 year New Zealand case study and relevant findings from Sweden and Victoria

Postgraduate Research Experience 2012 A report on the experience of recent higher degree research graduates

RESEARCH METHODOLOGY 3.1 OBJECTIVES OF THE STUDY. 2. To analyze the attitude and behavior of Indian consumers towards green products

Attitudes Toward Smoking Restrictions in Work Sites, Restaurants, and Bars Among North Carolinians. Abstract

Report. 1. Introduction. 2. Background. 3. Results for England

Two-in-three are dieting or exercising In the Battle of the Bulge, More Soldiers Than Successes

'If you don't manage diabetes, it will manage you': Type two diabetes self-management in rural Australia

CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY

INSECURITY. Food. Though analyses at the regional and national levels

MAKING AN IMPACT ON GENDER EQUALITY LET S UNCOVER THE ISSUES. LET S CREATE SOLUTIONS.

Health Behavioral Patterns Associated with Psychologic Distress Among Middle-Aged Korean Women

MINNESOTA S ORAL HEALTH WORKFORCE October 2015

Social Issues in Nonmetropolitan Nebraska: Perceptions of Social Stigma and Drug and Alcohol Abuse: 2018 Nebraska Rural Poll Results

RHCs in Accountable Care Organizations (ACOs)

SOC-SOCIOLOGY (SOC) SOC-SOCIOLOGY (SOC) 1

The Relative Importance of Per Capita Expenditure on Animal Protein by Different Income Categories in Egypt's Urban and Rural Sectors

Stigma in Patients Using Mental Health Services

National Surveys of Mental Health Literacy and Stigma and National Survey of Discrimination and Positive Treatment

Fit to play but goalless: Labour market outcomes in a cohort of public sector ART patients in Free State province, South Africa

Methodological Considerations to Minimize Total Survey Error in the National Crime Victimization Survey

Justice Data Lab Re offending Analysis: Prisoners Education Trust

Perceptions & Predictors of Inequality among Afghan & Pakistani Women

Lecturer: Dr. Paul Narh Doku Contact: Department of Psychology, University of Ghana

Vermonters Choose Healthy Eating Habits: Children and Time Impact Eating Choices Most Vermonter Poll March, 2008 Michele C.

Research Questions and Survey Development

Words and Deeds of Generosity. Are Decisions About Real and Hypothetical Money Really Different? DRAFT COMMENTS WELCOME.

TIMELINE CONTENT SKILLS ASSESSMENT NJCCCS February. Monitor class discussion and 6.1-A

CHAPTER 14 ORAL HEALTH AND ORAL CARE IN ADULTS

Internet Dependency among University Entrants: A Pilot Study

Minnesota s Dental Hygienist Workforce,

UNEQUAL ENFORCEMENT: How policing of drug possession differs by neighborhood in Baton Rouge

Animal Welfare: A Key Component of Social Justice

Supplement DS1 Search strategy. EMBASE Search Strategy

Animal-Assisted Interventions and Psychiatric Disorders: Knowledge and Attitudes among General Practitioners, Psychiatrists, and Psychologists

The Personal Profile System 2800 Series Research Report

Knowledge Building Part I Common Language LIVING GLOSSARY

2017 Sociology. Higher. Finalised Marking Instructions

Minnesota s Dental Therapist Workforce, 2016 HIGHLIGHTS FROM THE 2016 DENTAL THERAPIST SURVEY

Inequalities in childhood immunization coverage in Ethiopia: Evidence from DHS 2011

Media, Discussion and Attitudes Technical Appendix. 6 October 2015 BBC Media Action Andrea Scavo and Hana Rohan

Workplace stress in South African mineworkers

At the Israel Electric Company: Israel Railways

STATUS REPORT: OVERVIEW. Prepared by The Center for Applied Research and Analysis The Institute for Social Research University of New Mexico

A Study on Effectiveness of Clinical Psychologist in De-Addiction Centers

Oral health trends among adult public dental patients

Finding Cultural Differences and Motivation Factors of Foreign Construction Workers

Gender Profile: Rwanda

Statistics Anxiety among Postgraduate Students

Factor Structure of the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale: Two and Three factor solutions. Kevin Williams, Craig Nathanson, & Delroy Paulhus

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

Summary and conclusions

[Type text] Alabama. The Burden of Asthma. Alabama Department of Public Health 201 Monroe Street Montgomery, AL

Re: Response to discussion points raised at Allied Health Professions Australia (AHPA) Board meeting 20 June 2013 with regard to HWA

Hanover Welfare Services. Quantitative Research Report. October, 2006

CHARACTERISTICS OF SURVEY RESPONDENTS 3

AWARENESS ON AYURVEDIC SYSTEM OF MEDICINE IN CHENNAI CITY

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

Running Head: CAREER MATURITY IN INTERCOLLEGIATE ATHLETES 1. Career Maturity and Identity Foreclosure in Intercollegiate Student Athletes:

BEHIND CLOSED DOORS: UNDERSTANDING THE HUMAN SIDE OF HOARDING

Sex Differences and Promotion Prospects in Clinical Psychology in Scotland

Effectiveness of planned teaching programme on knowledge regarding ill effects of alcohol consumption.

Clinical Psychologists in Aged Care in Australia: A Question of Attitude or Training? E. Helmes & D. A. Koder James Cook University

PRISM Brain Mapping Factor Structure and Reliability

Practice tests Weblinks Flashcards

Transcription:

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 147 Tania D. Signal 1 and Nicola Taylor Attitudes to Animals: Demographics Within a Community Sample ABSTRACT The results of various studies have suggested a range of demographic and personality variables that may affect attitudes toward the treatment of nonhuman species; however, the literature has reached little consensus. Various limited populations have used The Attitude to Animals Scale (AAS), developed initially by Herzog, Betchart, and Pittman (1991), as a quantitative measure of attitudes toward the treatment of nonhuman species. The current study administered the AAS to a large community sample within Australia, resulting in approximately 600 respondents. The study found demographic variables such as age, educational level, presence of children in the current dwelling, current, and past companion animal ownership to have no statistically significant effect on AAS scores. The study found both occupation and income to have an effect on AAS scores.this paper examines and discusses all of these variables and their effects (or lack thereof ). An acceptance of the idea that attitudes toward nonhuman animals may affect attitudes toward humans opens the door to many possibilities. As awareness increases concerning the links between attitudes to humans and nonhuman animals, there is a need to determine which variables influence attitudes toward the treatment of animals and the nature of the relation. To date, the majority of research investigating Society & Animals 14:2 (2006) Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2006

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 148 this has been conducted within specific populations (students) and/or with relatively small samples (Matthews & Herzog, 1997; Taylor & Signal, 2005). Within this literature, variables suggested to affect attitudes toward the treatment of nonhuman animals include the following: 1. gender (Herzog, Betchart & Pittman, 1991); 2. experience of companion animals both as a child and an adult (Paul & Serpell, 1993; Taylor & Signal, 2005); 3. age, income, and educational level (Kellert, 1980); 4. marital status (Kafer, Lago, Wamboldt, & Harrington, 1992); 5. ethical ideology (Galvin & Herzog, 1992); religious persuasion (Bowd & Bowd, 1989; Kellert, 1980); and 6. race and whether the respondent resides in a rural or urban environment (Kellert, 1980). Overall, however, the literature lacks agreement about the effect on attitudes toward the treatment of animals of these various demographic and personality-related variables. One of the more consistent findings is that females tend to score higher than males on measures of attitudes toward the treatment of animals; that is, they tend to have a more pro-animal attitude (Herzog et al., 1991; Matthews & Herzog, 1997). Given the lack of agreement regarding the effect of variables other than gender and that many studies have been conducted with limited samples, there remains a need to investigate these issues further. Moreover, there is a need to establish the effect of such variables within a more normative population (larger samples and general community members) to provide a benchmark against which research with narrower populations can be gauged (violent offenders). Current Study As part of a larger study that aimed to assess links between empathy, aggression, and attitudes to animals within a large community sample, questionnaires were administered by telephone to a random sample of approximately 600 adults throughout Australia. This resulted in a total of 598 responses from 205 males and 393 females. Due to missing data, the number of actual participants within each variable is outlined throughout the results. The questionnaire included the following demographic questions: (a) gender, (b) age, 148 Tania D. Signal and Nicola Taylor

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 149 (c) education level, (d) income, (e) current occupation, (f) presence of a companion animal in the home (now and as a child), and (g) presence of children within current dwelling. In addition, respondents also were asked to complete a range of psychometric scales, including the AAS. Data were analyzed using SPSS v.12.0 both within each variable and then with gender as a concomitant factor. The AAS assesses individual differences in attitudes toward the treatment of animals (Herzog et al., 1991). It is composed of 20 items and has high internal consistency (Cronbach s Alpha = 0.91). Scores on this scale can range from 20 to 100, with higher scores indicating a more pro-animal attitude. Examples of these items include: It is morally wrong to hunt wild animals just for sport The use of animals in rodeos and circuses is cruel. (This scale is available from http://wcuvax1.wcu.edu/%7eherzog/research. html.) Results Age Data from 581 valid responses were analyzed, with ages ranging from 18 to 88 (M=48.4, SD=15.8). A minimal, non-significant, negative correlation between Age and AAS scores was found (r=-0.069, p=0.109). Gender and Age No significant relationship emerged between AAS, gender, and age (ANOVA, F(1,534)=1.051, p=0.382) beyond the expected gender bias (female participants across age groups scored significantly higher on the AAS (M=69.5, SD=9.1) than males (M=63.8, SD=8.7) (t[548]= -7.166, p=0.000). Attitudes to Animals 149

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 150 Education Level Data from 550 valid responses were analyzed with highest level of education completed, percentage of cases and related average AAS score presented in Table 1. Table 1. AAS Scores as a Function of Education Level HIGHEST EDUCATION FREQUENCY PERCENTAGE AVERAGE AAS LEVEL OF CASES SCORE Primary School 31 5.6 65.1 High School 246 44.7 67.9 Tertiary University* 106 19.3 68.4 Tertiary Other** 164 29.8 67.2 Other 3 0.5 64.3 * includes undergraduate and graduate level study ** includes TAFE and Industry Qualifications As can be seen in Table 1, those who had attended University had the highest average AAS score. However, following further analyses (ANOVA), no significant difference was found in AAS scores across educational levels (F(1,542)=1.024; p=0.394). Gender and Education Level Graphical analyses of AAS scores within each education level, further separated by gender, supported the expected gender bias with the exception of those who indicated that Primary School was the highest level of education attained. For these participants, there was no apparent gender difference in AAS scores. Presence of Children Analyses of 550 valid responses indicated that the majority of respondents did not currently live with children (65.6%). Although there was a trend toward higher AAS scores for those who did have children within their current dwelling, this trend was not found to be significant (t=1.681, p=0.093). 150 Tania D. Signal and Nicola Taylor

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 151 Gender and Children Present An ANOVA was conducted to investigate the interaction between gender and having a child within the current dwelling on AAS score. Although the interaction approached significance no differential effect on AAS was found (F(1,546)=3.258, p=0.072) Companion Animal Present Currently Data from 552 valid responses was analysed, the majority of respondents (77%) reported that they were currently living with at least one companion animal. Participants were asked to specify the species of their current companion animal(s), with the most common companion animal identified as the dog (77.4%), followed by the cat (45.3%). Thirty-five percent of respondents reported living with two or more species, the most common combination being dog plus cat. Overall no significant differences in AAS scores were found between those currently with or without a companion animal (t=-0.059; p=0.953); by type of animal (F(8,411)=0.712; p=0.681) or between those who currently live with one or multiple companion animal species (t=1.310; p=0.191). Gender and Companion Animal Currently An ANOVA was conducted to investigate the interaction between gender and the current presence of a companion animal on AAS score. No differential effect on AAS was found (F(1,546)=0.198, p=0.657). Companion Animal in Childhood Data from 552 valid responses were analysed and, as for the current presence of a companion animal 93% of respondents reported that they lived with at least one companion animal during their childhood. Again, when participants were asked to specify the species of their childhood companion animal(s), the most common companion animal identified was the dog. Overall no significant differences in AAS scores were found between those who lived with or without a companion animal in childhood (t=-0.526; p=0.599) or by the type of companion animal(s) lived with (ANOVA; F(8,411)=1.006; p=0.681). Attitudes to Animals 151

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 152 Gender and Childhood Companion Animal An ANOVA was conducted to investigate the interaction between gender and the presence of a childhood companion animal in AAS score. No differential effect on AAS was found (F(1,542)=0.068, p=0.795). Occupation Participants current occupation was coded using the major job categories listed within an online job search engine (www.seek.com.au). Further recoding resulted in eight categories (including other) as can be seen in Table 2. Table 2. Percentage of Each Occupation Category and Occupation-related AAS Score CURRENT FREQUENCY PERCENTAGE AVERAGE AAS OCCUPATION OF CASES SCORE Primary Industry 32 5.5 57.7 Education 53 9.1 68.8 Health 56 9.6 69.6 White Collar 1 132 22.6 68.1 Blue Collar 2 47 8.1 62.8 Science 14 2.4 64.4 Welfare 3 230 39.5 69.2 Other 4 19 3.3 63.6 1 Administration, management, customer service etc. 2 Construction, transport, mechanic, laborer etc. 3 At home parent, pensioner, student, unemployed etc. 4 Police, religious official, artist etc. Those currently employed within the Healthcare category had the highest average AAS scores followed by those within the Welfare category. In contrast, those with the lowest average AAS scores were currently employed within the Primary Industry sector. Analyses (ANOVA) showed significant differences in AAS scores across occupation categories (F(7,532)=9.490, p=0.000). Post-hoc analyses (Tukey s HSD) showed that both Blue Collar and Primary Industry workers scored significantly lower than did those in Healthcare, Education, White Collar, and Welfare categories. In addition, those within 152 Tania D. Signal and Nicola Taylor

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 153 the Primary Industry sector scored significantly lower than did Blue Collar workers. Given that the Welfare category contained a number of sub-categories, was the most frequent, and that those within this category scored second highest on the AAS, further analyses were conducted. The Welfare category broke down into four discrete groups, with those currently unemployed scoring highest, and pensioners scoring lowest, on the AAS. An ANOVA showed that there was a significant difference in average AAS scores between the four Welfare sub-categories (F(3,209)=4.123, p=0.007). Post-hoc analyses (Tukey s HSD) indicated that the only significant difference was between the AAS scores of pensioners and at home parents. Gender and Occupation Although no significant difference on the AAS scores was found by the interaction of gender and current occupation (F(7,356)=1.639, p=0.123), it is of interest that no gender difference was apparent within either the Primary Industry or Education categories. Income Data from 454 valid responses were analyzed, with the largest cohort falling in the <AUD$15,000 bracket. A small, but significant, negative correlation between Income and AAS was found (r=-0.186, p=0.01). Those in the highest income category (>AUD$60,000) had the lowest overall AAS score while those in the lowest income bracket had the highest AAS scores. This difference in AAS scores was found to be significant (ANOVA, F(4,415)=4.626, p=0.001), with post-hoc analyses showing that those in the highest income category had significantly lower average AAS scores than did those in the <AUD$15,000 category (using Tukey s HSD). Apparent differences between the AAS scores of the other income categories were not significant. Given the potential for a sampling bias (the majority of respondents were in the lowest income category) a Chi 2 Test of Independence was conducted to ascertain whether the variables of income and job category were dependent. The results indicated that these categories were not independent (X 2 =211.676, p=0.000). That is, the majority of those currently earning less than AUD$15,000 Attitudes to Animals 153

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 154 fell within the Welfare occupation category, while those earning more than AUD$60,000 fell predominantly in the White Collar occupation category. Therefore, given the current data, it is not possible to differentiate the effect of income level from current occupation. Gender and Income An ANOVA was conducted to investigate the interaction between Gender and Income category on AAS score. This interaction was found to be significant (F(4,356)=2.586, p=0.037). As can be seen in Figure 1, females generally scored higher than males on the AAS in all income categories, with the exception of those earning more than AUD$60,000 where the gender difference effectively disappears. AAS Scores Estimated Marginal Means 75 70 65 60 55 Gender male female < 15 15-30 30-45 45-60 > 60 Income Category Figure 1. Estimated Marginal Means for AAS scores as a Function of Income Category Presented by Gender. 154 Tania D. Signal and Nicola Taylor

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 155 Discussion The aim of the current study was to assess the demographic variables that may be related to attitudes toward the treatment of nonhuman animals. While Income and Occupation proved to have a statistical effect on AAS scores, other variables tested showed no significant effect, but several trends were observed. These trends and the main findings are discussed below. In contrast to Kellert (1980), no significant effect of age (on AAS scores) was found. Kellert reported that younger respondents were more likely to have pro-animal welfare attitudes; however, the assessment tool used differed from the AAS. This may account for the disparity in findings. Alternatively, it may be that the current study, which took place 25 years later, reflects changes in attitudes over time. While there appears to be a trend whereby those with higher, formal education levels show a more pro-animal welfare attitude, this trend is not significant. This suggests that university student populations, upon which many previous research findings rest, may not be significantly different from other populations in this respect. This clearly deserves further analysis; in particular, it would be interesting to determine whether the type of subject studied has an effect on attitudes toward the treatment of animals. Various authors have reported an association between childhood companion animal-keeping and increased concern about nonhuman animal welfare in adulthood (Paul & Serpell, 1993; Paul, 2000). Miura, Bradshaw, and Tanida (2002) also found a similar relation across two culturally diverse populations (British and Japanese). They predicted that the positive effect of companion animal ownership in childhood on adult attitudes to animals may well be a universal finding. In contrast to this, Taylor and Signal (2005) failed to find an effect of childhood companion animal ownership but did find an effect of current companion animal ownership on attitudes to the treatment of nonhuman animals within a university student cohort. The current study with a larger, more representative sample failed to find any effect of companion animal ownership whether now, in childhood, by (human) gender, or by (nonhuman animal) species. The current data show that occupation had a significant effect on attitudes to the treatment of animals; in particular, those employed in the Primary Attitudes to Animals 155

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 156 Industry sector have lower AAS scores. Although there were substantially fewer females than males employed in the Primary Industry sector, no effect was found due to the interaction of gender and occupation. This suggests that the over representation of males within this category is not the underlying reason for the overall, lower average AAS scores. That those within the Pensioner category scored lowest on the AAS may lend some support to Kellert s (1980) finding that younger persons tend toward more pro-animal attitudes, despite the fact that this current study found no overall age effect. However, it was beyond the scope of the current questionnaire to elicit information regarding previous occupation(s) of those currently in the welfare category. Therefore, it is unclear whether the lower AAS scores of Pensioners are related to their age or their previous occupation. Similarly, when considering those within the Unemployed category (who have substantially higher AAS scores), it is also unclear whether this is a function of their age, their previous occupation, or other variables. Of particular interest is the difference between male and female AAS scores in the lowest income category. It should be noted that 80% of those who earned less than AUD$15,000 were female. Within this current data set, females have been found to score significantly higher than males on the AAS overall. It is therefore possible that the earlier identified pattern of the lowest income category relating to the highest AAS score is, in fact, a function of gender differences in the AAS. In contrast, although females only made up 37.5% of those in the highest income bracket, their average AAS scores were not significantly different from males in the same income category. Conclusion While it is important to benchmark attitudes toward the treatment of animals within a normative community sample, the current study has several limitations. Most notably, the potential demographic bias that is inherent within telephone surveys (McBurney & White, 2004). The random sampling method used here supports the generalizability of the results. While the sample was drawn from those currently residing in Australia, the potential effects of geographical location are beyond the scope of this study but may present an area for future research. * Tania D. Signal and Nicola Taylor, Central Queensland University, Australia 156 Tania D. Signal and Nicola Taylor

S & A 14,2_f3_147-157 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 157 Note 1 Correspondence should be sent to Tania Signal, School of Psychology & Sociology, Central Queensland University, Rockhampton, QLD 4702, Australia. E-mail: t.signal@cqu.edu.au. A grant awarded by the Centre for Social Science Research, Central Queensland University enabled data collection for this paper. References Bowd, A. D., & Bowd, A. C. (1989). Attitudes toward the treatment of animals: A study of Christian groups in Australia. Anthrozoös, 3 (1):20-24. Galvin, S., & Herzog, H. (1992). Ethical ideology, animal rights activism and attitudes toward the treatment of animals. Ethics & Behavior, 2 (3), 141-149. Herzog, H., Betchart, N., & Pittman, R. (1991). Sex role identity and attitudes toward animals. Anthrozoös, 4, (3), 184-192. Kafer, R., Lago, D., Wamboldt, P., & Harrington, F. (1992). The pet relationship scale: replication of psychometric properties in random samples and association with attitude toward wild animals. Anthrozoös, 5 (2), 93-105. Kellert, S. (1980). American attitudes toward and knowledge of animals: An update. International Journal for the Study of Animal Problems, 1 (2), 87-119. Matthews, S., & Herzog, H. (1997). Personality and attitudes toward the treatment of animals. Society & Animals, 5 (2), 57-63. McBurney, D. H., & White, T. L. (2004). Research Methods (6th ed.). Stamford, CT: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Miura, A., Bradshaw, J. W. S., & Tanida, H. (2002). Childhood experiences and attitudes toward animal issues: A comparison of young adults in Japan and the UK. Animal Welfare, 1 (4), 437-448. Paul, E. S. (2000). Empathy with animals and with humans: Are they linked? Anthrozoös, 13 (4), 194-202. Paul, E. S., & Serpell, J. A. (1993). Childhood pet keeping and humane attitudes in young adulthood. Animal Welfare, 2 (4), 321-337. Taylor, N. & Signal, T. D. (2005). Empathy and attitudes toward animals. Anthrozoös, 18 (1), 18-27. Attitudes to Animals 157

S & A 14,2_f4_158-178 4/5/06 5:43 PM Page 158