Tibor Pólya. Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary

Similar documents
International Conference on Humanities and Social Science (HSS 2016)

PERCEIVED TRUSTWORTHINESS OF KNOWLEDGE SOURCES: THE MODERATING IMPACT OF RELATIONSHIP LENGTH

MEASUREMENT, SCALING AND SAMPLING. Variables

A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan

HOW TO IDENTIFY A RESEARCH QUESTION? How to Extract a Question from a Topic that Interests You?

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Cohesion in online groups

2 Psychological Processes : An Introduction

The observation that the mere activation

SENTENCE COMPLETION TEST FOR DEPRESSION. LONG FORM Version 3.1 SCD-48

Reliability and Validity

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

Intro to SPSS. Using SPSS through WebFAS

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

COPING STRATEGIES AND SOCIAL PROBLEM SOLVING IN ADOLESCENCE

The detection and management of pain in patients with dementia in acute care settings: development of a decision tool: Research protocol.

Statistical analysis DIANA SAPLACAN 2017 * SLIDES ADAPTED BASED ON LECTURE NOTES BY ALMA LEORA CULEN

THE POWER OF STORYTELLING

Closed Coding. Analyzing Qualitative Data VIS17. Melanie Tory

Signing High School Science

QUALITATIVE RESEARCH. Not everything that counts can be counted, and not everything that can be counted counts (Albert Einstein)

Work Personality Index Factorial Similarity Across 4 Countries

VIEWS AND EXPERIENCES OF DEAF TEACHERS ON INCLUSIVE EDUCATION AND DISABILITY

CHAPTER 3 DATA ANALYSIS: DESCRIBING DATA

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Principles of Sociology

Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population

Doing Quantitative Research 26E02900, 6 ECTS Lecture 6: Structural Equations Modeling. Olli-Pekka Kauppila Daria Kautto

Still important ideas

Evaluation of Forensic Psychology Reports: The Opinion of Partners and Stakeholders on the Quality of Forensic Psychology Reports

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

Guidelines for Incorporating & Strengthening Perspective-Taking & Self-Authorship into Division of Student Life Programs

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHOD. method the major components include: Research Design, Research Site and

Basic SPSS for Postgraduate

Hearing young children s perspectives on life with a parent with bipolar disorder using the In My Shoes interview

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance

Moral Personality Exemplified. Lawrence J. Walker. University of British Columbia

Introduction. Chapter 1

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies throughout the world

The Meta on Meta-Analysis. Presented by Endia J. Lindo, Ph.D. University of North Texas

Validity and Reliability. PDF Created with deskpdf PDF Writer - Trial ::

Statistical Methods and Reasoning for the Clinical Sciences

Learning Objectives. Learning Objectives 17/03/2016. Chapter 4 Perspectives on Consumer Behavior

Research Methods in Human Computer Interaction by J. Lazar, J.H. Feng and H. Hochheiser (2010)

Introduction. Activities undertaken 13/10/2015. Qualitative Data Analysis. CSHG Hotel Management School of Galicia

Quantitative Research Methods FSEHS-ARC

Narrative social psychology 1. János László and Bea Ehmann. 2

Applied Social Psychology Msc.

Lesson Plan: Japanese Internment a Challenge to Democracy

School of Nursing, University of British Columbia Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

A Cross-Cultural Study of Psychological Well-being Among British and Malaysian Fire Fighters

Likert Scaling: A how to do it guide As quoted from

Carrying out an Empirical Project

Altruism & the New (non- American) Social Psychology. Prosocial behaviour is. Altruism is. Week 10

Personal Growth Interpretation of Goal Attainment as a New Construct Relevant to Well-being

University of Warwick institutional repository:

Introduction to Meta-Analysis

The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence

EMPATHY AND COMMUNICATION A MODEL OF EMPATHY DEVELOPMENT

The significance of deaf identity for psychological well-being

Story Shifters DR. BARBARA WARD

RESEARCH METHODOLOGY-NET/JRF EXAMINATION DECEMBER 2013 prepared by Lakshmanan.MP, Asst Professor, Govt College Chittur

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

The longer you ve been here I think the more they get moulded : Identity regulation, identification and resistance

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Doctoral Dissertation Boot Camp Quantitative Methods Kamiar Kouzekanani, PhD January 27, The Scientific Method of Problem Solving

Understanding Social Norms, Enjoyment, and the Moderating Effect of Gender on E-Commerce Adoption

Unit Options and Core Texts

CURRENT RESEARCH IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY

CHAPTER 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS 1.1 MOTIVATION AND STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS 1 SYNOPSIS OF THE THESIS MOTIVATION OF THE THESIS

CHAPTER V. Summary and Recommendations. policies, including uniforms (Behling, 1994). The purpose of this study was to

Business Statistics Probability

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

UNDERSTANDING QUANTITATIVE RESEARCH

Incorporating Experimental Research Designs in Business Communication Research

The role of narrative perspective in the elaboration of individual and historical traumas

Understanding Your Coding Feedback

Addressing issues of power and equality using. narrative and reflexive research. Professor Kim Etherington University of Bristol

(CORRELATIONAL DESIGN AND COMPARATIVE DESIGN)

CONSTRUCTION OF EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE RATING SCALE

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Construction of an Attitude Scale towards Teaching Profession: A Study among Secondary School Teachers in Mizoram

Identity Theory: Reconstruction and Application

Exploring rare patient behaviour with sequential analysis: an illustration

Relationship, Correlation, & Causation DR. MIKE MARRAPODI

Phil 490: Consciousness and the Self Handout [16] Jesse Prinz: Mental Pointing Phenomenal Knowledge Without Concepts

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE A GATEWAY TO SUCCESS FOR MANAGEMENT STUDENTS

Visual Imagery and Public Speaking Apprehension in South Korea

Health Sciences Department or equivalent Division of Health Services Research and Management UK credits 15 ECTS 7.5 Level 7

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Garmon, Lance C., Egerton, Nichole, & Kinder, Alexandria Salisbury University Salisbury, Maryland

Sense-making Approach in Determining Health Situation, Information Seeking and Usage

REPORT FOR THE ROYAL SOCIETY FOR THE PROTECTION OF BIRDS (RSPB): CONNECTION TO NATURE QUESTIONNAIRE ON THE NORTHERN IRELAND KIDS LIFE AND TIMES SURVEY

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHOD

From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: Linking behaviour change techniques to their mechanisms of action.

Transcription:

Psychology Research, August 2017, Vol. 7, No. 8, 458-462 doi:10.17265/2159-5542/2017.08.006 D DAVID PUBLISHING National History Contributes to the Definition of National Identity Tibor Pólya Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary The study empirically tests the claim that national history contributes to the definition of national identity. Definition of national identity was assessed by the analysis of narratives of historical events gained from 260 participants. The results show that national history plays a part in the definition of national identity. Keywords: national identity, national history, self-categorization theory, narrative construction Recently, increasing theoretical interest has been shown to how national history contributes to the definition of national identity (Finell & Liebkind, 2010; László, 2013; Liu & Hilton, 2005; Liu & László, 2007; Smeekes, 2014). History provides resources to define national identity (Wertsch, 2002). History contains the ontogeny (Liu & Hilton, 2005) and other defining moments of national history which shows up a critical moment in which the nation was truly itself (Reicher & Hopkins, 2001, p. 139). History also encompasses the exemplary behaviours of key figures or heroes. Generalizations over history, such as charters (Liu, & Hilton, 2005) and templates (Wertsch, 2002) also add to the definition of national identity. However, empirical evidence is constrained regarding the defining role of history. There are studies showing that when persons are asked about the definition of national identity they most frequently refer to tradition and heritage (e.g., Cinnirella, 1997). Correspondingly, Smeekes (2014) found that persons views on national history are associated with inter-group attitudes. The aim of this study is to gather empirical evidence of the claim that history contributes to the definition of national identity. According to the Self-categorisation theory (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987), when a person identifies with a social identity category, that identity category becomes activated and defines the current self. If history contributes to the definition of national identity, we may expect that the level of identification with the national identity is related to a cognitive activity whose subject is representations of history such as constructing a historical narrative. More specifically, it could be expected that high identifiers construct a more complex historical narrative than low identifiers since representations of history are activated in their current self. The expected relationship is likely to be affected by the valence of the narrated event. There is considerable research evidence (e.g., Semin, 2000) showing that the valence of the event is systematically Acknowledgements: The study was funded by National Research, Development and Innovation Office, grant No. 109009. The study was conducted within the framework of COST Action IS1205 Social psychological dynamics of historical representations in the enlarged European Union. Tibor Pólya, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences.

NATIONAL HISTORY CONTRIBUTES TO THE DEFINITION OF NATIONAL IDENTITY 459 related to the use of linguistic features. Since the social identification process is partly motivated by striving for positive self-esteem, it could be expected that when the historical event is positive, the narrator is more motivated to become active in the narrative construction, resulting in a narrative with a more complex structure. However, if the historical event is negative, a negative relationship is expected: the narrator is less motivated in narrative construction that results in a less complex structure. Regarding the complexity of the historical narrative, the study focuses on the feature of perspective. Perspective includes the mental states of either the character or the narrator presented in the narrative. Accordingly, two types of perspectives (character and narrator perspective) will be analysed. Inclusion of either of them makes the narrative more complex (e.g., László et al., 2013). Based on this, it is hypothesised that the level of identification and narrative complexity reflected by perspective are positively related when telling about a positive historical event, and they are negatively related when telling about a negative historical event. Study Participants The study involved 260 Hungarian undergraduate students (64 men, 196 women). Their mean age was 22.07 years (SD = 3.74). Participants were recruited from psychology classes and their participation was voluntary. Instrument The level of identification with the Hungarian national identity category was measured by 7 Likert-type items regarding the subjective experiences related to national identity. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agreed with each statement on a 7-point scale ranging from (1) Completely disagree to (7) Strongly agree. Examples of items are I feel strongly attached to Hungarians and Being Hungarian is not an important part of who I am (reversed item). The internal consistency of the measurement was good (α = 0.75). Procedure The study was conducted in small groups. The participants task was to recount the most important event from the history of Hungary in written form. They also rated the items measuring their identification with the national group. The order of presentation of the narrative writing task and identification measurement was counterbalanced. Completion of the tasks took about 20 minutes. Analysis of Narratives Short answers were omitted since they had no narrative structure. The cut point was at 18 words. For this reason, 26 cases were not included in the analysis. The valence of the events was coded as positive, negative or neutral by two independent coders. Many of the narratives contained both positive and negative information and the coders had to decide whether the historical event was dominantly positive or negative (κ = 0.63). Disagreements were resolved by the inclusion of a third coder. Neutral narratives were also omitted from the analysis (N = 14). The final sample included 220 narratives, 137 accounts of positive events and 83 accounts of negative events. Perspective was analysed with an automated content analytical toolkit of Narrative Categorical Content Analysis (László et al., 2013). The character s perspective is revealed by words which describe characters cognitions (e.g., think, make a decision), perceptions (e.g., see, hear) or emotions (e.g., sad, happy). The narrator s perspective was identified by the use of the first person singular pronoun. For statistical data analysis,

460 NATIONAL HISTORY CONTRIBUTES TO THE DEFINITION OF NATIONAL IDENTITY relative measures were computed, that is, raw frequencies of the coded words were divided by the total word count of the narrative. Results In sum, 14 different historical events were mentioned. The three most frequently selected events covered almost two thirds of the narratives. These events were the Founding of the state (25.8%), the Treaty of Trianon (24.6%), and the Revolution of 1956 (13.1%). Mean identification was 4.17 (SD = 1.05). In order to test whether the presentation order of the tasks (narrative writing and identification measurement) had any effect on the level of identification, the two conditions were compared by independent-samples t-test. No difference in identification was found (t < 1). It was also assessed whether the level of identification was related to the choice of historical event and its valence. The sample was divided into two groups of high and low identifiers at the sample mean. Chi-square tests showed no difference between high and low identifiers in the choice of historical event (χ 2 (13) = 14.57, p = 0.335). However, there was a marginal difference in the valence of the selected historical events (χ 2 (1) = 3.22, p = 0.073). High identifiers chose more than twice as many positive historical events (N = 83) as negative ones (N = 40). The average length of the narratives was 60.7 words (SD = 32.61). The relative frequency of character s and narrator s perspective was 1.26 (SD = 1.81) and 1.28 percent (SD = 1.59), respectively. Pearson correlation analysis was conducted including the level of identification and the relative frequencies of the two perspective measures. The results are reported in Table 1 where data are broken down according to the valence of the historical event. Table 1 Correlation Coefficients Among the Variables of Level of Identification and Perspective Perspective Negative event N = 83 r Level of identification Positive event N = 137 r Character s perspective -0.15 + 0.15 * Cognition -0.20 * 0.08 Emotion -0.01 0.18 * Narrator s perspective -0.24 * 0.14 + Notes. * p < 0.05, + p < 0.10. Results on character s perspective are in line with the hypothesis. When narrating a positive historical event, identification correlates positively with the frequencies of character s perspective and reference to characters emotions whereas in the case of negative events, there is a negative correlation between identification and references to characters cognition in the narrative. Results on the narrator s perspective are also in line with the hypothesis: The correlation is positive in the case of positive historical events, and it is negative in the case of negative historical events. Discussion This study aimed to empirically test the idea whether national history contributes to the definition of national identity. More specifically, we attempted to reveal whether the level of identification with the national

NATIONAL HISTORY CONTRIBUTES TO THE DEFINITION OF NATIONAL IDENTITY 461 group is related to the construction of historical narratives. In consistence with our prediction, it was found that the level of identification is positively correlated with the complexity of the account of a positive historical event while the correlation between identification and complexity is negative in the case of negative historical events. The results can be explained by suggesting that the activation of national identity involves the activation of its representational contents related to national history that results in the construction of a complex historical narrative. However, due to the correlational design of the study, we could not infer to the direction of a possible causal relationship between identification and construction of a historical narrative. There is empirical evidence showing that the presentation of pictures of national historical events can be used to make that identity category salient (Finell & Liebkind, 2010). Alternatively, it is also plausible that narrative construction and identification interact. In line with this interpretation, the order of presentation of the narrative writing task and identification measurement had no effect either on narrative construction or on the level of identification. Regardless of the direction of the relationship, the study provides empirical evidence of the conclusion that history is a part of the definition of national identity. This conclusion is in line with earlier approaches which stressed that history provides events and characters to exemplify important features of the national identity (Cinnirella, 1997; László, 2013; Liu & Hilton, 2005; Reicher & Hopkins, 2001; Smeekes, 2014; Wertsch, 2002). As an extension of previous studies which relied on verbal reports to uncover the contents of national identity, this study used verbal reports to assess the activity of representations of national identity in the current self. This extension corroborates the position that national history serves to define national identity. This position is also confirmed by the results related to the motive for positive self-esteem (e.g., Turner et al., 1987). The finding that high identifiers prefer positive historical events to negative ones can be explained by the need for positive self-esteem. Furthermore, the study also found that the very construction of historical narratives is affected by this motive. These results reveal that national history contributes to the definition of national identity in such a way as to satisfy the positive self-esteem motive related to the activated national identity category. The study highlights the importance of history education to define national identity, however, it has got several limitations. It was conducted with a homogeneous sample of university students. The study had a correlational design and it included only one national identity category, namely, the Hungarian national identity. These limitations make us cautious about generalizing the results without further empirical evidence. References Cinnirella, M. (1997). Towards a European identity: Interactions between the national and European identities. British Journal of Social Psychology, 36(1), 19-31. Finell, E., & Liebkind, K. (2010). National symbols and distinctiveness. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 321-341. DOI:10.1348/014466609X455190 László, J. (2013). Historical tales and national identity: An introduction to narrative social psychology. London and New York: Routledge. László, J., Csertő, I., Fülöp, É., Ferenczhalmy, R., Hargitai, R., Lendvai, P., Péley, B., Pólya, T., Szalai, K., Vincze, O., & Ehmann, B. (2013). Narrative language as an expression of individual and group identity: The narrative categorical content analysis. SAGE Open, April-June, 1-12. DOI:10.1177/2158244013492084 Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weighs on the present: Social representations of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 1-21.

462 NATIONAL HISTORY CONTRIBUTES TO THE DEFINITION OF NATIONAL IDENTITY Liu, J. H., & László, J. (2007). A narrative theory of history and identity: Social identity, social representations, society and the individual. In G. Moloney, & I. Walker (Eds.), Social representations and history (pp. 85-107). New York, NY: Palgrave-Macmillan. Reicher, S., & Hopkins, N. (2001). Self and nation: Categorization, contestation and mobilization. London: Sage. Semin, G. R. (2000). Agenda 2000 Communication: Language as an implementational device for cognition. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30(5), 595-612. Smeekes, A. N. (2014). The presence of the past: Historical rooting of national identity and current group dynamics (Doctoral dissertation, Utrecht University). Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). (Eds.). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell. Wertsch, J. V. (2002). Voices of collective remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.