Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key

Similar documents
Effects of Response Elaboration Training on Increased Length and Complexity of Utterances with Two Participants with Fluent Aphasia

The effect of word familiarity and treatment approach on word retrieval skills in aphasia

Clinical Aphasiology Submission 2006

World Languages American Sign Language (ASL) Subject Matter Requirements

Measuring Changes in Quality of Life in Persons with Aphasia: Is Communication Confidence a Good Measure?

English and Persian Apposition Markers in Written Discourse: A Case of Iranian EFL learners

How difficult is it? How well Adults with Aphasia Perceive Task Demands

Methods. Participants

INTRODUCTION Use of images in multiple-choice tasks is common in linguistic comprehension

Exploring the relationship between high level anomia, attention and cognitive processing deficits: a retrospective data analysis

2.1 Participants 122 stroke patients with aphasia and 25 healthy controls (Table 1). <insert Table 1>

Critical Review: The Efficacy of Animal-Assisted Therapy for Improving Communication Skills in Adults with Aphasia or Apraxia

California Subject Examinations for Teachers

Critical Review: The Effectiveness of Constraint-Induced Language Therapy in a Distributive Format

FORENSIC HYPNOSIS WITH THE DEAF AND HEARING IMPAIRED

Changes in Chronic Global Aphasia at Impairment and Functional Communication Levels following SGD Practice and Use. Richard. D. Steele Lingraphica

Information for goal setting: use of a passive digital camera (SenseCam)

Pragmatic language impairments

MANUAL FOR THE LIDCOMBE PROGRAM OF EARLY STUTTERING INTERVENTION 2008 CONTENTS

Improvements in Chronic Broca s Aphasia at Impairment and Functional Communication Levels following SGD Practice and Use

Brooke DePoorter M.Cl.Sc. (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Arts and Entertainment. Ecology. Technology. History and Deaf Culture

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Handheld AAC Program for People with Aphasia. Marjorie Nicholas, PhD Krystal Peralta, MS

California Subject Examinations for Teachers

TITLE: Acquisition and generalization responses in aphasia treatment: Evidence from sentence-production treatment

Who Exactly Is This Book For?

FOURTH EDITION. NorthStar ALIGNMENT WITH THE GLOBAL SCALE OF ENGLISH AND THE COMMON EUROPEAN FRAMEWORK OF REFERENCE

OF NUMBERS AND NARRATIVE Paper prepared for presentation at the Law & Society Annual Meeting, Amsterdam, June 1991.

Disclosures. Background. Equipment 2/4/2015

Interviews IAEA. International Atomic Energy Agency

(SAT). d) inhibiting automatized responses.

Impact of Personalization on Acquisition and Generalization of Script Training: A Preliminary Analysis. Abstract

There are two types of activities: Think about it! And apply it! Each activity will be marked by an icon and a specified color as you can see below:

Listening to life stories

Critical Review: Do mind-body therapies improve language outcomes in persons with aphasia?*

INTRODUCTION METHODS

PECS and Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Andy Bondy, Ph.D. Lori Frost, MS, CCC-SLP

ASL ONE CURRICULUM UNIT ONE: Lesson 1.1 Fingerspelling I Rule #1 of Deaf Culture Assessment: Assignment 1.1

The Things They Carried

AUDITORY VOCAL HALLUCINATION RATING SCALE (AVHRS) 1,2 (Jenner and Van de Willige, 2002)

Effort Invested in Cognitive Tasks by Adults with Aphasia: A Pilot Study

Aphasia And Associated Disorders: Taxonomy, Localization And Recovery By Andrew Kertesz

This is an edited transcript of a telephone interview recorded in March 2010.

How to Recognize and Reduce Challenges to Children s Comprehension of Books

Putting Autism in Perspective

The Emergence of Grammar: Systematic Structure in a New Language. Article Summary

Faculty Advisor: Dr. Sandra Gillam. Abstract. 1. Introduction

Understanding Your Coding Feedback

We are doing some research. It is about practising standing up early. after stroke. Research helps us learn. We need to know more about how to

H.O.T. Theory, Concepts, and Synesthesia: A Reply to Adams and Shreve

Running Head: NARRATIVE COHERENCE AND FIDELITY 1

Characteristics of the Text Genre Nonfi ction Text Structure Three to eight lines of text in the same position on each page

CLINICAL SURVEY ON 300 APHASIC PATIENTS IN. Shinichi WATABE, Shunichi SASAO and Itaru KIMURA. (Miyagi Byoin National Sanatorium, Miyagi)

Syllabus. ASL Level 3. Instructor: Deanne Bray-Kotsur (Video Relay Service) Class Meets: In Room I-7

BLACK RESIDENTS VIEWS ON HIV/AIDS IN THE DISTRICT OF COLUMBIA

Children s Understanding of Time in Picture Books. Undergraduate Honors Research thesis

Using Virtual Clinicians to Promote Functional Communication Skills in Aphasia Method. Participants. Experimental design

Analysis A step in the research process that involves describing and then making inferences based on a set of data.

Search: Finding Cultural Models in Everyday Discourse. Analysis: Write it up. A procedure for finding cultural models

Political Science 15, Winter 2014 Final Review

Strategies for Building ASL Literacy

Does Wernicke's Aphasia necessitate pure word deafness? Or the other way around? Or can they be independent? Or is that completely uncertain yet?

A corpus study of the construction of evaluative stance in Introduction in Psychology and Radiology journals

A Probability Puzzler. Statistics, Data and Statistical Thinking. A Probability Puzzler. A Probability Puzzler. Statistics.

Finding Evidence of Deductive and Abductive Reasoning Abilities: An Experiment with Lateral-Brain-Damage Patients

Chapter Three: Sampling Methods

observed. In spite of the fact that D.H. demonstrated minimal definitive changes on the conversational measures selected, by the end of both

Good Communication Starts at Home

Social Organisation of Remembering and Reconciliation: A Case of Former British POW s

The architect s perspective on the tour and map perspective

CLASSROOM & PLAYGROUND

Problems and Solutions in Teaching ESL Students to Write Concisely Jim Hu

Chapter 3 Tools for Practical Theorizing: Theoretical Maps and Ecosystem Maps

COLLEGE OF THE DESERT

Narrative: Its Importance in Modern Behavior Analysis and Therapy. Yvonne Barnes-Holmes, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, and Ciara McEnteggart

by Marge Blanc, M.A., CCC-SLP (appeared in Autism Asperger s Digest, March/April 2013)

Realidades A 2011 Correlated to: (Grades 4-8)

UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN-MADISON Research Participant Information and Consent Form

Unit III Verbal and Non-verbal Communication

An investigation of oral narratives in children with High Functioning Autism: aspects of microstructure and macrostructure

Signing Naturally, Units is one of the book in the series of curricular materials for the instruction of American Sign Language (ASL) as a

AS and A Level English Language and Literature EXEMPLAR RESPONSES

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups

American Sign Language II Topic Outline Course Description and Philosophy

Poetry in South African Sign Language: What is different?

REFERENCES Blank, M., & Franklin, E. (1980). Dialogue with preschoolers: A cognitively-based system of assessment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1,

Quick guide to autism

Pragmatics and Disordered Speech. Mackenzie Salt, M.A., H.B.A., D-TEIL

Hearing young children s perspectives on life with a parent with bipolar disorder using the In My Shoes interview

FOURTH EDITION. NorthStar ALIGNMENT WITH THE GLOBAL SCALE OF ENGLISH AND THE COMMON EUROPEAN FRAMEWORK OF REFERENCE

Working with an Interpreter QUICK GUIDE. your Clients. What is Interpreta on? Common Terms Arranging for an interpreter Fee schedule

Autism and Communication

Quick Read Series. Information for people with seizure disorders

Demonstrating Highlights from NAPSA s Core Competency Professional Communication Training Module

Communication. Jess Walsh

3/25/2016. Ashley Dittmar. What s Wrong Here? Learning Assessment Question 1

Note to the interviewer: Before starting the interview, ensure that a signed consent form is on file.

Plot. What is the story s central conflict? Who is the protagonist? What does he or she want? What is at stake for the protagonist in the conflict?

Chapter 4 - Listening and Relationship Development. Chapter Orientation. Learning Objectives. Rick Grieve, Ph.D. PSY 442 Western Kentucky University

Transcription:

Introduction Stories or narratives are a common discourse genre embedded in everyday conversation (Norrick, 2010). It has been proposed that a key function of narrative perhaps the very driving force behind its existence is to convey one s point of view, attitude, or opinion about the event being narrated (Labov, 1972; Polanyi, 1989). This transmission of significance, i.e., the expression of the author s stance on the so what of the narrated event, is achieved through a process of (narrative) evaluation, and the linguistic and paralinguistic means of evaluation are termed evaluative devices (Labov, 1972). Evaluative devices appear to work in concert to selectively highlight or add prominence to information in the narrative (Polanyi, 1989; Olness et al., 2010). Notably, an evaluative device is not inherently evaluative, but rather becomes evaluative when its frequency of use departs from the baseline frequency of use of that device in preceding utterances (Polanyi, 1989, p. 22). Clinical research on narrative evaluation may illuminate the mechanisms behind the paradoxical reality that speakers with aphasia are often better communicators than they are language users (Holland, 1977). Recent studies present evidence that the categories, distribution, and semantic coherence of evaluative devices may be comparable for speakers with and without aphasia, even when the referential content of the aphasic speakers narratives is not clear (Olness et al., 2010; Olness & Englebretson, In press). Clinical case data discussed by Nespoulous at al. (1998) similarly suggest that speakers with aphasia may be better at using language for the purpose of conveying the speaker s personal attitude, than they are at using language referentially. At the same time, the presence of aphasia in the narrator may place certain bounds on the use of evaluative devices. For instance, it has been found that certain evaluative devices are used by a lower proportion of speakers with aphasia, as compared to speakers without aphasia (Olness et al., 2010). Also, for narrators with relatively severe aphasia, evaluation may be all-pervasive throughout the discourse (Olness et al., 2010), a finding consistent with the observation of high frequency of use of single evaluative devices, such as direct speech, in the narratives of speakers with aphasia (Berko-Gleason et al., 1980). A too-pervasive use of evaluative devices would not allow the speaker to establish a baseline frequency of non-use, which ironically is the very baseline that allows the device(s) to fill their evaluative function in the first place (Polanyi, 1989, p. 22). The current study extends prior research on aphasic narrators use of evaluative devices (Olness et al. 2010; Olness in press). Unlike earlier studies, the current study samples across multiple narrative elicitation tasks, many of which have been traditionally used in clinical practice, and which may vary in their ability to elicit evaluative language. It also specifically examines evaluative devices which may differ in frequency of use by speakers with aphasia as compared to speakers without aphasia. Finally, participants were selected to be ethnically homogenous, to control for the potential effects of ethnicity on the use of evaluative devices. Method Participants/interviewees Participants were 39 English-speaking African-American (A) adults: Of these, twentyone had aphasia (APH) associated with a history of left-hemisphere stroke, and eighteen had no neurological disorder or injury (NBI). Age, gender, education, and socioeconomic status of the two clinical groups are comparable. (See Table 1.) A range of aphasia severity levels was represented. (See Table 2.) Narrative tasks

All participants were interviewed individually by a female interviewer, race-matched to the narrators. Tasks were selected based on their potential to elicit a narrative discourse genre. The five task types represent different points on a hypothesized continuum from those least likely to evoke an opinion, attitude, or stance from the narrator (and thus to elicit evaluative devices), to those most likely to do so, in order: retells of two stories presented verbally and in print; tellings of two stories based on a picture sequence; completion of a story for which only the setting and complicating action were provided; three stories told in response to single pictures; and two personal narratives. Analysis For each participant, responses to each task were categorized as either narrative or nonnarrative, and non-narrative response were excluded from further analysis to assure genre homogeneity of the analyzed samples. For each response, individual propositions were identified and counted. For each proposition, instances of each of seven evaluative devices occurring within that proposition were identified. These evaluative device categories were selected from a larger set of evaluative devices, based on prior evidence suggesting differences in the number of speakers with and without aphasia who use them. (See Appendix.) For each narrative, the percentage of propositions that contained each evaluative device type was calculated. Results Complete data from 3 participants have been analyzed to date: one with moderate aphasia (A-APH26); one with mild-moderate aphasia (A-APH17) and one non-brain-injured (A-NBI06). (See Table 3.) Effects of aphasia and task were examined for their potential effect on the presence and percentage use of the seven evaluative devices. Effects of aphasia The individual with moderate aphasia did not use four of the seven evaluative devices (modals, expressive, like/as and idioms), and the mild-moderate aphasic and non-aphasic participants used each evaluative device at least once. Percentage use of direct speech was highest in the individual with moderate aphasia. Effects of task Across the three participants, there is no consistent effect of task type on the number of different evaluative device types used. All task responses displayed at least one evaluative device. The number of different evaluative devices used per task ranged from one to seven, but those tasks with the highest and lowest number of different devices was not consistent across subjects. Interactions of task and aphasia There were only two participants for whom a given evaluative device was produced at least once on every task: direct speech was used in responses to all tasks by the individual with moderate aphasia, and attributives were used in responses to all tasks by the individual with no aphasia. Statistics For analysis of the full data set, statistics will be selected for their ability to account, both descriptively and inferentially, for the effects of aphasia presence, aphasia severity, task, and aphasia-by-task on the use of the seven categories of evaluative devices.

Discussion Narrators have a variety of evaluative devices to choose from to selectively add prominence to information in their stories. The current study is designed to examine how the presence and severity of aphasia and the type of discourse elicitation task may affect the use of evaluative devices, which are the tools essential for the function of transmitting significance of narrative content. Findings hold implications for the assessment of narrative evaluation, namely, identification of those evaluative devices that are likely to be effectively used by individuals with aphasia, thus contributing to aphasic speakers communicative competence; those that are least likely to be used, and are thus unlikely to contribute substantially to narrative evaluation; and those that may be overused, such as direct speech, thus detracting from their use as evaluative devices proper. Findings may further suggest those tasks that are optimal for the assessment of narrative evaluation, although analysis of the larger data set will be necessary to draw any inferences about these task effects. References Berko-Gleason, J., Goodglass, H., Obler, L., Green, E., Hyde, M. R., & Weintraub, B. (1980). Narrative strategies of aphasic and normal speaking subjects. Journal of Speech and Hearing Resesarch, 30, 370 382. Featherman, D. L., & Stevens, G. A. (1980). A revised socioeconomic index of occupational status: Center for Demography and Ecology working paper 79-84. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin. Holland, A. (1977). Some practical considerations in aphasia rehabilitation. In M. Sullivan & M.S. Kommers (Eds.), Rationale for adult aphasia therapy (pp. 167-180). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Medical Center Print Shop. Kertesz, A. (1982). The Western Aphasia Battery. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city: Studies in the black English vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Nespoulous, J.-L., Code, C., Virbel, J., & Lecours, A. R. (1998). Hypotheses on the dissociation between referential and modalizing verbal behavior in aphasia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 311-331. Norrick, N. R. (2010). Conversational narrative: Storytelling in everyday talk (Paperback edition). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Olness, G. S., & Englebretson, E. (In press). On the coherence of information highlighted by narrators with aphasia. Aphasiology. Olness, G. S., Matteson, S. E., & Stewart, C. T. (2010). Let me tell you the point : How speakers with aphasia assign prominence to information in narratives. Aphasiology, 24, 697-708. Polanyi, L. (1989). Telling the American story: A structural and cultural analysis of conversational storytelling. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Table 1: Gender, age, highest education level attained, and socioeconomic status of participants (2 clinical groups of African American adults) Gender Age (in years) Highest education level attained Socioeconomic status (maximum = 7) Participant group n Male Female Median Range Median Range Median Range African-American adults 39 15 24 With aphasia 21 9 12 56 33-74 3 2-7 4 2-7 Without aphasia 18 6 12 54 44-71 4 1-7 4 2-7 Highest education level attained is specified ordinally by number; 1=less than 12th grade, 2=high school graduate, 3=community college or trade school, 4=some college, 5=four-year college graduate, 6=some graduate school, 7=graduate school graduate Socio-economic rating was adapted from Featherman & Stephens (1980); higher numbers reflect higher socioeconomic status.

Table 2: WAB-AQ scores, and corresponding aphasia severity of APH participants Participants WAB-AQ (max = 100) Aphasia severity A-APH04 59.5 Moderate A-APH21 53.8 Moderate A-APH22 50.1 Moderate A-APH26 50.4 Moderate A-APH27 52.4 Moderate A-APH08 77.2 Mild-Moderate A-APH10 80.8 Mild-Moderate A-APH17 74.8 Mild-Moderate A-APH23 80.4 Mild-Moderate A-APH03 92 Mild A-APH11 89.2 Mild A-APH14 90.5 Mild A-APH15 93.1 Mild A-APH18 n.a. Mild A-APH28 93.4 Mild A-APH30 87.1 Mild A-APH32 93.6 Mild A-APH33 90.2 Mild A-APH09 99.7 Very Mild A-APH25 98.8 Very Mild A-APH29 95.1 Very Mild A-NBI (n = 18) All > 98.2 ----- n.a.= Test scores not available. Aphasia severity judged from spontaneous speech.

Table 3: Percentage of propositions containing evaluative devices, by task and device type, for three participants Task type Stimulus attributives modals Moderate aphasia: A-APH26 direct speech Evaluative Device Type predicate modifiers expressive nominals and verbals like/as (nonmetaphorical) idioms Number of propositions Retell F&S 24% 0% 57% 10% 0% 0% 0% 21 Retell Starfish 4% 0% 36% 0% 0% 0% 0% 25 Picture seq. B&A 0% 0% 63% 0% 0% 0% 0% 19 Picture seq. C/T 0% 0% 29% 0% 0% 0% 0% 21 Completion Mrs. W. 0% 0% 45% 0% 0% 0% 0% 11 Single pict. D/T/A 8% 0% 40% 0% 0% 0% 0% 25 Single pict. EM 16% 0% 12% 0% 0% 0% 0% 25 Single pict. FloodR 12% 0% 18% 0% 0% 0% 0% 17 Personal narr. FE 10% 0% 10% 5% 0% 0% 0% 20 Personal narr. SO 6% 0% 53% 6% 0% 0% 0% 32 Mild-moderate aphasia: A-APH17 Retell F&S 8% 31% 23% 23% 0% 8% 0% 13 Retell Starfish na na na na na na na na Picture seq. B&A 22% 0% 0% 33.% 0% 11% 0% 9 Picture seq. C/T 38% 0% 0% 8% 0% 0% 0% 13 Completion Mrs. W. 45% 0% 0% 36% 36% 0% 9% 11 Single pict. D/T/A 27% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 11 Single pict. EM 0% 14% 0% 14% 0% 14% 14% 7 Single pict. FloodR 57% 14% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 7 Personal narr. FE 0% 12% 25% 37% 0% 0% 0% 8 Personal narr. SO 0% 0% 19% 0% 0% 0% 0% 16 No aphasia: A-NBI06 Retell F&S 18% 6% 15% 6% 6% 0% 0% 34 Retell Starfish 50% 0% 27% 14% 4% 0% 4% 22 Picture seq. B&A 15% 0% 0% 4% 0% 0% 4% 27 Picture seq. C/T 22% 4% 11% 22% 0% 0% 0% 27 Completion Mrs. W. 13% 22% 22% 9% 17% 13% 0% 23 Single pict. D/T/A 21% 0% 0% 7% 0% 0% 0% 14 Single pict. EM 10% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 20 Single pict. FloodR 9% 0% 18% 18% 0% 0% 0% 11 Personal narr. FE 8% 0% 32% 5% 0% 0% 0% 37 Personal narr. SO 16% 3% 35% 6% 3% 3% 1% 103 na = data not available (participant not tested on this task)

Appendix This appendix contains examples of the seven narrative evaluative devices included in the analysis. These were selected from a larger set of evaluative devices (Olness et al., 2010), to represent those whose frequency of use may be different for speakers with aphasia, as compared to speakers without aphasia. Hypothesized frequency of use by speakers with aphasia, as compared to speakers without aphasia (Olness et al., 2010; Berko-Gleason et al., 1980) How this evaluative device is purported to function (Olness et al., 2010) Example Evaluative device type Attributives Less frequent By intensifying information; associated with adjectives Modals Less frequent Through use of irrealis; mention of unrealized events petrified; crazy; You re in a strange theatre and here sit 10, 12 boys They could ve killed her. And they had to come get me. Direct speech More frequent By slowing or suspending the narrative event line, and increasing vividness Predicate modifiers Expressives (nominal or verbal) Like/as (nonmetaphorical) Less frequent Less frequent Less frequent By intensifying information; associated with adverbials By intensifying information; associated with nouns and verbs Through comparison with other entities Idioms Less frequent Through comparison of metaphorical entities to literal situation He said, It s important. I go, Say man! John sit down! so calm; all along the street i. nominal: idiot ii. verbal: careened I knew that my son had not been as active as he had been before. Freeze on that. ( don t do that )