Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal

Similar documents
Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal

Emotions of Living Creatures

Computational models of emotion

The Importance of the Mind for Understanding How Emotions Are

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

The Detection of Fear-Relevant Stimuli: Are Guns Noticed as Quickly as Snakes?

Emotion Theory. Dr. Vijay Kumar

Myers Psychology for AP*

PSYC 222 Motivation and Emotions

Psychological Science

WP3 Theories and Models of emotion

Address for correspondence: School of Psychological Sciences, Zochonis Building, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, M139PL, Manchester, UK

GfK Verein. Detecting Emotions from Voice

EMOTIONS S E N I O R S P E C I A L I S T I N P S Y C H I A T R Y A N D S E X T H E R A P Y

Human Emotion. Psychology 3131 Professor June Gruber

Aversive picture processing: Effects of a concurrent task on sustained defensive system engagement

Introduction to Psychology. Lecture no: 27 EMOTIONS

Emotion and Motivation. Chapter 8

THE PHYSIOLOGICAL UNDERPINNINGS OF AFFECTIVE REACTIONS TO PICTURES AND MUSIC. Matthew Schafer The College of William and Mary SREBCS, USC

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry

Temporal dynamics of amygdala and orbitofrontal responses to emotional prosody using intracerebral local field potentials in humans

Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions

Characterization of the Affective Norms for English Words by discrete emotional categories

The effects of emotional arousal on item and associative memory

Competition Elicits Arousal and Affect

See no evil: Directing visual attention within unpleasant images modulates the electrocortical response

Motivation represents the reasons for people's actions, desires, and needs. Typically, this unit is described as a goal

MPEG-4 Facial Expression Synthesis based on Appraisal Theory

Chapter Eight: Emotion and Motivation

Chapter 8. What Is Emotion? What Do Our Emotions Do For Us? Emotion and Motivation

MODULE 41: THEORIES AND PHYSIOLOGY OF EMOTION

Emotion Lecture 26 1

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

EMOTION. Joy, Sorrow, Anger, Fear, Love, Hate, Happiness

Artificial Emotions to Assist Social Coordination in HRI

Attentional Deployment Is Not Necessary for Successful Emotion Regulation via Cognitive Reappraisal or Expressive Suppression

Introduction to Emotion

The Geneva affective picture database (GAPED): a new 730-picture database focusing on valence and normative significance

The challenge of representing emotional colouring. Roddy Cowie

BRIEF REPORT. Memory for novel positive information in major depressive disorder

THE INFLUENCE OF NEGATIVE AFFECT ON SELF-REFERENT SENTENCE PROCESSING AND MEMORY PERFORMANCE. Austin Greg Fitts

Deborah Talmi, Division of neuroscience and experimental psychology, School of Biological Sciences,

Emotional Agents need Formal Models of Emotion

Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

the remaining half of the arrays, a single target image of a different type from the remaining

Look Before You Regulate: Differential Perceptual Strategies Underlying Expressive Suppression and Cognitive Reappraisal

References. Note: Image credits are in the slide notes

Elise K. Kalokerinos, Katharine H. Greenaway, and Thomas F. Denson Online First Publication, October 6,

General Psych Thinking & Feeling

Emotions and Motivation

Emotion October 16th, 2009 : Lecture 11

Person Perception. Forming Impressions of Others. Mar 5, 2012, Banu Cingöz Ulu

Chapter 12,13 &14. Motivation, Emotion, Stress & Health

Emotional representation in facial expression and script A comparison between normal and autistic children

drive-reduction theory

Learning Outcome: To what extent do cognitive and biological factors interact in emotion?

Neural Response to Emotional Pictures Is Unaffected by Concurrent Task Difficulty: An Event-Related Potential Study

Myers Psychology for AP, 2e

The Fearful-Face Advantage Is Modulated by Task Demands: Evidence From the Attentional Blink

Suppression of Emotional and Nonemotional Content in Memory

Supplementary Study A: Do the exemplars that represent a category influence IAT effects?

Course syllabus. University of Warsaw, Faculty of Psychology. their prominent role in human functioning. In the very

Behavioral and Brain Sciences Once more with feeling: On the explanatory limits of the GANE model, and the missing role of subjective experience.

Influences of emotion on context memory while viewing film clips

Outline. Emotion. Emotions According to Darwin. Emotions: Information Processing 10/8/2012

Affective Science Fall, 2014 (Psyc )

Memory Enhancing Effects of Emotional Arousal are Automatic: A Divided Attention Study

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Conceptual, Theoretical, and Methodological Issues in Inferring Subjective Emotion Experience

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

Modeling the Mechanisms of Emotion Effects on Cognition

Alternative Explanations for Changes in Similarity Judgments and MDS Structure

Understanding Affective Experiences: Towards a Practical Framework in the VALIT-Project

Unmasking emotion: Exposure duration and emotional engagement

Emoacoustics: a study of the physical and psychological dimensions of emotional sound design

Response to Commentaries on Multiple Arousal. Theory and Daily-Life Electrodermal Activity Asymmetry

The World of Emotions Is Not Two-Dimensional Johnny R.J. Fontaine, 1 Klaus R. Scherer, 2 Etienne B. Roesch, 2 and Phoebe C.

Emotional Development

Exposure to arousal-inducing sounds facilitates visual search

Investigating the role of emotion during the search process in free recall

Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions

Chapter 7 Motivation and Emotion

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

Emotion. Cannon-Brad Theory. Display Rules. Schacter s Two-Factor Theory. Cognitive Appraisal Theory. Lateralization of Emotion. James-Lange Theory

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Emotional influences on time perception

Visual Awareness and the Detection of Fearful Faces

Motivation Versus Aversive Processing During Perception

Fear Selectively Modulates Visual Mental Imagery and Visual Perception

The Timing of Appraisals

Unconscious Social Behavior. John F. Kihlstrom. University of California, Berkeley

Emotions in a repeated Cournot game

Psychosocial intervention to optimal treatment to patients with schizophrenia: neurocognitive perspectives

Defining emotion: a clinical perspective

Theories and Physiology of Emotion. AP Psychology Module 41

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

autonomic ne rvous system The autonomic ne rvous system The autonomic ne rvous system Pa rasympathetic Sympathetic

Trait Emotions and Affective Modulation of the Startle Eyeblink: On the Unique Relationship of Trait Anger

Affective Systems. Rotterdam, November 11, 2004

Transcription:

BBS-D-15-00893_ Mather_ Montagrin & Sander Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal Alison Montagrin 1,2,3 * & David Sander 1,2 1 Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, 2 Department of Psychology and 3 Department of Fundamental Neurosciences, University of Geneva *Corresponding author and first author University of Geneva, 40, Boulevard Pont d Arve, 1205 Geneva - Switzerland Alison Montagrin: +41 22 379 92 27 David Sander: +41 22 379 92 07 alison.montagrin@unige.ch david.sander@unige.ch Alison Montagrin: http://cms.unige.ch/fapse/emotionlab/members/alisonmontagrin/index.php David Sander: http://cms.unige.ch/fapse/emotionlab/members/david-sander/index.php Abstract: Arousal is typically conceived as a key component of emotional response. We describe here the psychological processes thought to elicit arousal, in particular the processes involved in the appraisal of affective relevance. The key role of relevance in attentional and memory processing, and its links with arousal, is discussed with respect to the GANE model presented by Mather et al. Mather et al. provide an innovative and integrative model aimed at explaining, at the neural level, how arousal can both enhance and impair cognitive processing, such as perception and memory. The Glutamate Amplifies Noradrenergic Effects (GANE) model proposed by the authors accounts for results indicating that increased norepinephrine under arousal affects prioritization of information processing, e.g., enhances memory for salient information at the expense of mundane information. Mather et al. consider arousal to be the critical factor that amplifies the perception of emotional stimuli while impairing the perception of other concurrent stimuli, as has been proposed in the Arousal-Biased Competition (ABC) theory (Mather & Sutherland, 2011). Mather et al. are particularly interested in analyzing the effect of selectivity in the perception and memory of emotional stimuli under arousal; however, the construct arousal is not conceptually

clear if one considers how it is used in the affective sciences literature, especially concerning theories of emotion. More generally, the relationship between emotion and arousal is far from being consensual, notably when it comes to modeling the psychological mechanism that elicits different types of arousal (see Sander, 2013). For instance, Frijda (1986) distinguished between three response systems that embrace the construct of arousal or activation: autonomic arousal, electrocortical arousal, and behavioral activation. Traditionally, theories of emotion refer to arousal mainly in terms of either (1) the consciously felt activation dimension of the experienced affect or (2) the bodily reaction during an emotional episode, specifically the sympathetic nervous system. Dimensional theories propose that any experienced affect can be described as a particular feeling on a continuum in terms of an activated (e.g., as in surprise) or deactivated (e.g., as in boredom) state (Feldman, 1995; Russell, 1989). On the other hand, the bodily reaction can be measured in terms of a psychophysiological state driven by the activation of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS). Emotion researchers typically use the construct of arousal to refer to the activation of the SNS, and physiological measures (e.g., skin conductance response or pupil dilation) are often used as measures of emotion intensity, as they are also considered to mirror subjective affective ratings (see Bradley, Miccoli, Escrig, & Lang, 2008; Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm, 1993). The effect of arousal on performance has been revealed in various research domains and is consistent with the model proposed by Mather et al., suggesting that under arousal neutral stimuli with a high priority are processed preferentially. The priority factor is essential to memory enhancement. Indeed, it has been proposed that arousal per se is not sufficient to explain memory improvement for neutral information (Reisberg & Heuer, 2004). For instance, a study that manipulated arousal by injection of adrenalin versus saline showed no difference in the results of memory performance for verbal descriptors (e.g., name, occupation) presented previously with neutral faces, although heart rate and skin conductance increased in the group with adrenalin injections (Christianson & Mjörndal, 1985). The information presented did not differ in terms of priority. This notion of priority, especially when related to the well-being or major

concerns of the individual, is considered to be of key importance in explaining emotion elicitation. Indeed, appraisal theories of emotion suggest that stimuli that are appraised as relevant for an individual s concerns (e.g., that relate to goals, needs, and values) are typically those that have the competency to elicit an emotional response by driving changes in action tendencies, in expressions, in the peripheral nervous system, and in the conscious feeling (e.g., Sander, Grandjean, & Scherer, 2005). With respect to arousal, a conceptually interesting advantage of such models is that an explanation for the elicitation of arousal is proposed: only concern-relevant events elicit arousal. Evidence indicates that stimuli that are appraised as concern relevant not only elicit such emotional responses, but are also prioritized in attention (see Pool, Brosch, Delplanque, & Sander, in press). For instance, a recent meta-analysis revealed that the magnitude of the attentional bias for positive stimuli varies as a function of arousal, but also that this bias is significantly larger when the stimulus is relevant to specific concerns (e.g., hunger) of the participants compared with other positive stimuli that are less relevant to the participants concerns (Pool et al., in press). Interestingly, in this meta-analysis, when arousal and relevance moderators were tested by statistically controlling their respective variances, only relevance remained a significant predictor of the magnitude of this bias in emotional attention. Emotional attention is a process that has been suggested to strongly rely on the amygdala (Vuilleumier, 2005), a region that has also been suggested to be critical for the process of appraised relevance (e.g., Cunningham & Brosh, 2012; Sander, Grafman, & Zalla, 2003), and, of importance, to promote memory consolidation, as described by Mather et al. (Roozendaal & McGaugh, 2011, for review). Studies supporting the ABC theory (e.g., Sutherland & Mather, 2012) and GANE models are largely based on experimental manipulation showing that high-priority neutral information under external arousal is enhanced in memory (as compared with lowpriority neutral information). Showing that goal-relevant events, which are intrinsically neutral, are processed preferentially might extend the model proposed by Mather et al. For instance, the goal-relevance hypothesis of memory facilitation has been tested in a game-like study in which the goal of the participant was to win points. Participants showed better memory performance for initially neutral items that signaled a gain (i.e.,

goal-conducive items) than they did for initially neutral items that were goal irrelevant (Montagrin, Brosch, & Sander, 2013). Our suggestion is that events that are relevant for one s goals elicit an arousal response, capture attention, and facilitate memory. A fascinating research question, for which both the GANE model and the ABC theory can be particularly well articulated with appraisal theories of emotion, is to understand the causal relationships between relevance detection, arousal, attention, and memory. References Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., & Lang, P. J. (2008). The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology, 45, 602 607. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.x Christianson, S. Å., & Mjörndal, T. (1985). Adrenalin, emotional arousal and memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 26, 237 248. Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: Amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 54 59. doi:10.1177/0963721411430832 Feldman, L. A. (1995). Valence focus and arousal focus: Individual differences in the structure of affective experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 153 166. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.153.search Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261 273. Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 114 133. doi:10.1177/1745691611400234 Montagrin, A., Brosch, T., & Sander, D. (2013). Goal conduciveness as a key determinant of memory facilitation. Emotion, 13, 622 628. doi:10.1037/a0033066 Pool, E. R., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., & Sander, D. (in press). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin. Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 3 41). London, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Roozendaal, B., & McGaugh, J. L. (2011). Memory modulation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125, 797 824. doi:10.1037/a0026187 Russell, J. A. (1989). Measures of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. 83 111). Toronto, ON, Canada: Academic Press.

Sander, D. (2013). Models of emotion: The affective neuroscience approach. In J. L. Armony & P. Vuilleumier (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience (pp. 5 53). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. Sander, D., Grafman, J., & Zalla, T. (2003). The human amygdala: An evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 14, 303 316. Sander, D., Grandjean, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2005). A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion. Neural Networks, 18, 317 352. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2005.03.001 Sutherland, M. R., & Mather, M. (2012). Negative arousal amplifies the effects of saliency in short-term memory. Emotion, 12, 1367 1372. doi:10.1037/a0027860 Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Emotion and attention interactions in social cognition: Brain regions involved in processing anger prosody. NeuroImage, 28, 848 858. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.023