Right lateral prefrontal cortex Specificity for inhibition or strategy use?

Similar documents
Prefrontal cortex. Executive functions. Models of prefrontal cortex function. Overview of Lecture. Executive Functions. Prefrontal cortex (PFC)

Accepted Manuscript. When does a strategy intervention overcome a failure of inhibition? Evidence from two left frontal brain tumour cases

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

Components of working memory and somatic markers in decision making

Overt vs. Covert Responding. Prior to conduct of the fmri experiment, a separate

Decision neuroscience seeks neural models for how we identify, evaluate and choose

Brain Regions Associated with the Cambridge Brain Sciences Tests

Functional Neuroanatomy and Traumatic Brain Injury The Frontal Lobes

Mapping task switching in frontal cortex through neuropsychological group studies

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

correlates with social context behavioral adaptation.

The differing roles of the frontal cortex in fluency tests

Social and Emotional Decision-making Following Frontal Lobe Injury

How Clinical Neuropsychological assessment can inform research. Professor Lisa Cipolotti

Problemas conceptuales de la memoria del trabajo

Attentional Control 1. Identifying the neural systems of top-down attentional control: A meta-analytic approach

Neuropsy. of the frontal lobes: functions and disfunctions

How to Build a Cognitive Task

Laboratoire sur le Langage, le Cerveau et la Cognition (L2C2), Institut des Sciences

Executive function and behaviour

Biological Risk Factors

The Brain on ADHD. Ms. Komas. Introduction to Healthcare Careers

BINGES, BLUNTS AND BRAIN DEVELOPMENT

Importance of Deficits

The two sides of human thought. Human thinking: Lessons from Neuroscience. Patient studies. Kalina Christoff Vancouver, BC May 29, 2007

The Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes. Anatomy of the Frontal Lobes 3/2/2011. Portrait: Losing Frontal-Lobe Functions. Readings: KW Ch.

Task Switching. Higher-Level Cognition Oct 7, 2008

Regional Grey Matter Atrophy is associated with Social Cognition impairment in Progressive Multiple Sclerosis (MS).

Joseph T. McGuire. Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences Boston University 677 Beacon St. Boston, MA

Mindfulness and compassion as the foundations of well-being Saturday 7 October am

Dynamic functional integration of distinct neural empathy systems

Cognitive Neuroscience Attention

Executive function in progressive and nonprogressive behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Agustin Ibañez, PhD Facundo Manes, MD, MS. VIEWS & REVIEWS Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia ABSTRACT

Effects of acute ketamine infusion on visual working memory encoding: a study using ERPs

PEG TAPPING. Description of the Instrument:

Supplemental information online for

Early identification of neurobiological markers of remission. Michael Bodnar, PhD Ashok K. Malla, MD Martin Lepage, PhD

Neuropsychologia 49 (2011) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Study of Verbal Working Memory in Patients with Parkinson s Disease

HAL author manuscript

New Research on ECT and development of neuromodulation for treatment of depression

A CONVERSATION ABOUT NEURODEVELOPMENT: LOST IN TRANSLATION

Dementia UK & Admiral Nurses

Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. February 27, 2006

The relationship between executive functions and fluid intelligence in Parkinson s disease

Main points. Neuropsychology. Neuropsychological methods OCD. The neuropsychology of obsessivecompulsive. Neuropsychology OCD

Exploration of social rule violation in patients with focal prefrontal neurosurgical lesions

Neurobiological Foundations of Reward and Risk

Auditory Processing Of Schizophrenia

Clinical Applications of fmri

Neurobiological and neuropsychological pathways into substance abuse and addictive behaviour

NeuroImage 54 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

The Measurement of Everyday Life Executive Functioning in Adults with FASD: A Pilot Study

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

Supplementary Online Content

SPECIAL ISSUE: ORIGINAL ARTICLE PROACTIVE INTERFERENCE IN A SEMANTIC SHORT-TERM MEMORY DEFICIT: ROLE OF SEMANTIC AND PHONOLOGICAL RELATEDNESS

Executive function and the dysexecutive syndrome

Ask Dr. Judy: Strengthening the Brain s Executive Functions: The Real Higher Order Processing

Pharmacologyonline 3: (2010)

EXPLORATION OF THE NEURAL SUBSTRATES OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING BY FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING

IQ Tests, IQ Augmenting Technologies & How To Join Mensa

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Inception, Total Recall, & The Brain: An Introduction to Neuroscience Part 2. Neal G. Simon, Ph.D. Professor Dept. of Biological Sciences

Often Forgetting to do Things: What, Why and How not to?

Empathy is a complex psychological response in which observation, memory,

590,000 deaths can be attributed to an addictive substance in some way

Frontotemporal Dementia: Towards better diagnosis. Frontotemporal Dementia. John Hodges, NeuRA & University of New South Wales, Sydney.

Classification of Inhibitory Function

Selective Tuning of the Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Response during Simple Target Detection Dissociates Human Frontoparietal Subregions

Psychology 320: Topics in Physiological Psychology Lecture Exam 2: March 19th, 2003

BIOMED 509. Executive Control UNM SOM. Primate Research Inst. Kyoto,Japan. Cambridge University. JL Brigman

Co-ac&va&on mapping and Parcella&on

Intelligence moderates reinforcement learning: a mini-review of the neural evidence

Title:Atypical language organization in temporal lobe epilepsy revealed by a passive semantic paradigm

SHARED COMPONENT PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND LONG-TERM MEMORY: INSIGHTS FROM FUNCTIONAL BRAIN IMAGING. Petter Marklund

Cognition and Psychopathology Fall 2009

FAILURES OF OBJECT RECOGNITION. Dr. Walter S. Marcantoni

Goal-directed Thought

the degree of dissociation between theory of mind and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage

Scams: Influencing the Aging Brain

Working Memory: Critical Constructs and Some Current Issues. Outline. Starting Points. Starting Points

Human Thought and the Lateral Prefrontal Cortex

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

Geography of the Forehead

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions

Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory in Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia

Cognitive and Motivational Functions of the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Jared X. Van Snellenberg. Tor D. Wager

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing

Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Fronto-executive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates

Childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative help:

Interactive Effects of Three Core Goal Pursuit Processes on Brain Control Systems: Goal. Maintenance, Performance Monitoring, and Response Inhibition

Brain Based Change Management

Transcription:

Right lateral prefrontal cortex Specificity for inhibition or strategy use? M. Hornberger 1 & M. Bertoux 1 1 Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

The specific functions of prefrontal cortex (PFC) subregions remain a matter of controversy. There is an urgent need to resolve this debate, in particular to improve outcomes for patients with behavioural problems caused by PFC dysfunction. One of the most prevalent behavioural symptoms is disinhibition, i.e., the inability to suppress a response to a prepotent stimulus, which can cause great distress to patients and their families. Functional imaging and animal lesion models have been long been used to study disinhibition but there is still no consensus as to how different PFC subregions contribute to this deficit. At the same time, there are few human lesion studies on disinhibition and few attempts have been made to translate imaging and animal findings back to patients. In the current issue of Brain, Robinson and colleagues address this issue by investigating the specificity of PFC subregions for verbal initiation, suppression and strategy use in a large cohort (n=90) of lesion patients (Robinson et al. 2015). More specifically, the authors contrasted the performance of frontal (n=60) and posterior (n=30) patients on the Hayling Sentence Completion Test (Burgess & Shallice, 1996). The Hayling test consists of two parts, with the first part of the test asking participants to complete sentences with appropriate words (for example: He posted the letter without a could be completed by saying stamp ) measuring initiation. By contrast, the second part of the test requires participants to complete a similar set of sentences by providing words that are unrelated to the sentence frame (for example: The captain wanted to stay with the sinking should be completed by a word that has no nautical reference, such as banana or desk ) measuring inhibition/suppression. The second part of the test also allows establishment of strategy use, as most participants adopt strategies to complete the sentences with unrelated words. For example, participants complete the sentences by naming objects that are present in the testing environment, such as a desk or a shelf. Robinson and colleagues show that initiation deficits and failure of suppression of prepotent word completion are specific to patients with frontal

lesions. More importantly, there is a specific right frontal lesion effect for strategy use, whereby patients with lesions in this region fail to suppress words because they can not adopt an appropriate strategy to successfully complete the task. Previous studies investigating the inhibition lesion correlates of the Hayling test have obtained similar findings in smaller lesion samples. For example, Roca and colleagues (2010) reported that right rostral prefrontal cortex lesions are directly linked to suppression deficits. More recently, Volle and colleagues (2012) reported that suppression deficits are related to frontal lesions, with right inferior frontal lesions associated with significantly longer reaction times. This nicely dovetails with the current findings, which also show significant slowness for the suppression condition in the case of right lateralised lesions. However, the current study is the first to link those deficits to the failure of strategy use instead of inhibition/suppression effects per se. The inferior frontal cortex has been known for a long time to be related to response inhibition. In particular, the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) has been implicated in action cancellation tasks, such as stop-signal or go/no-go tasks (e.g., Aron et al. 2003), which usually require participants to respond to one stimulus while inhibiting a response to another. However, a clear discrepancy emerges when contrasting the IFG findings for action cancellation with the current Hayling findings, as little strategy use is needed to perform a stop signal or go/no-go task since the response options are extremely limited and no generation of alternative responses is needed. Does this mean that the strategy use findings of Robinson and colleagues are not in line with previous lateralised frontal and particularly IFG findings? Not necessarily, as previous studies have also highlighted that right prefrontal lesions can be related to creativity and problem-solving deficits and not only inhibitory function per se. For example, using a non-verbal problem-solving task, Miller and Tippett (1996) reported that patients with right frontal lesions are impaired on measures of strategy shift and

strategy use compared to controls, while patients with left-frontal or non-frontal lesions are not impaired. Similarly, the failure to develop an efficient strategy and to execute a predetermined plan, as measured by the Hotel task, has also been related to right inferior frontal cortex (Roca et al., 2010). Taken together, these deficits of strategy application associated with right frontal lesions support the results of Robinson et al. How can the inhibitory and strategy use findings for the right lateralised PFC and IFG be reconciled? One potential commonality between tasks as different as action cancellation (e.g., the stop-signal task) and strategy use (e.g., Hayling, real-life problem solving) might be the maintenance of task goals. Such a supervisory attentional system would be engaged by all tasks that entail monitoring of whether or not an adaption of behaviour is required. Indeed, failure to maintain the task goal in the Hayling would lead to inhibition/suppression deficits as a result of erratic responses, i.e., the failure to implement and maintain the appropriate strategy across trials. Similarly, failure to maintain the task goal would lead to deficits in stop-signal or go/no-go tasks, as patients might respond erratically even with limited responses available. Thus, sustaining task goals across trials might well be required for tasks as varied as action cancellation and strategy use in verbal suppression. A recent meta-analysis across functional neuroimaging executive tasks (though without the Hayling) appears to corroborate this notion (Cieslik et al. 2015). The study by Cieslik and colleagues shows that right IFG, as well as right anterior insula, are activated across many types of executive tasks requiring maintenance of task goals. Does this mean that the right lateralised PFC might not be as critical for inhibition as previously thought? The findings of Robinson and colleagues raise the question as to whether the right lateral PFC might be part of an inhibition network requiring the concerted interaction of various brain regions for inhibition/suppression to occur. This notion is supported by previous findings from Volle et al. (2012) and from

our lab (Hornberger et al, 2011) highlighting the fact that orbitofrontal cortex lesions/atrophy are also strongly related to inhibition/suppression deficits on the Hayling. These orbitofrontal changes might be related more to the prediction and evaluation of specific behavioural outcomes (Rudebeck & Murray, 2014), with patients unable to resist the prepotent response due to a failure to re-evaluate their responses. Unfortunately, the study by Robinson et al. did not include any patients with orbitofrontal lesions, which would have been an interesting contrast to the right lateralised lesions and might have allowed the dissociation of strategy use versus reevaluation of responses during Hayling performance. Clearly, future lesion studies addressing this gap could be of great value for delineating PFC subregion functionality further. Taken together, the novel findings of Robinson and colleagues highlight the specific role of the right prefrontal cortex in adopting appropriate strategies in a verbal suppression task. The findings further challenge the current notion of the right frontal cortex being related to inhibitory deficits per se. Instead, a more general task maintenance deficit resulting in a failure to adapt behaviour might reconcile existing inhibition findings. Finally, the current study highlights again the value of human lesion studies to corroborate and challenge functional neuroimaging and animal lesion findings. References Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. 2003. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci. 6:115--116

Burgess PW, Shallice T. Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1996 Apr;34(4):263-72. Cieslik EC, Mueller VI, Eickhoff CR, Langner R, Eickhoff SB. Three key regions for supervisory attentional control: Evidence from neuroimaging meta-analyses. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015 Jan;48C:22-34. Hornberger M, Geng J, Hodges JR. Convergent grey and white matter evidence of orbitofrontal cortex changes related to disinhibition in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011 Sep;134(Pt 9):2502-12. Miller LA, Tippett LJ. Effects of focal brain lesions on visual problem-solving. Neuropsychologia, 1996 May;34(5):387-98. Robinson G, Cipolotti L, Walker DG, Biggs V, Bozzali M, Shallice T. Verbal suppression and strategy use: A role for the right lateral prefrontal cortex? Brain, in press Roca M, Parr A, Thompson R, Woolgar A, Torralva T, Antoun N et al. Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain. 2010 Jan;133(Pt 1):234-47. Rudebeck PH, Murray EA. The Orbitofrontal Oracle: Cortical Mechanisms for the Prediction and Evaluation of Specific Behavioral Outcomes. Neuron. 2014 Dec 17;84(6):1143-1156. Volle E, de Lacy Costello A, Coates LM, McGuire C, Towgood K, Gilbert S et al. Dissociation between verbal response initiation and suppression after prefrontal lesions. Cereb Cortex. 2012 Oct;22(10):2428-40.

Corresponding author: Dr. Michael Hornberger, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK mh486@medschl.cam.ac.uk