Rebecca Sear and Frank W. Marlowe How universal are human mate choices? Size doesn t matter when Hadza foragers are choosing a mate

Similar documents
It is advisable to refer to the publisher s version if you intend to cite from the work.

Preferred waist-to-hip ratio and ecology

Conference Item (paper)

Test Bank. Chapter 2. Abrams, Sexuality and Its Disorders SAGE Publishing, 2017

Building a Solid Foundation for Your Study Based on Past Research

Sexual behavior and jealousy: An evolutionary perspective

Title:Spousal diabetes as a diabetes risk factor: A systematic review and meta-analysis

It s Not All about Her: Men s Mate Value and Mate Retention. Emily J. Miner. Florida Atlantic University. Valerie G. Starratt

AQA A Level Psychology. Topic Companion. Joseph Sparks & Helen Lakin

HUMAN SEXUAL DIMORPHISM IN SIZE MAY BE TRIGGERED BY ENVIRONMENTAL CUES

3.2A Least-Squares Regression

Reproductive Decision-Making in Transitional Contexts. Kristin Snopkowski Boise State University

Issues in the study of sex differences

MARK SCHEME for the May/June 2006 question paper 0495 SOCIOLOGY

Methodology A. Converging Evidence

Height among Women is Curvilinearly Related to Life History Strategy

Individual Differences

PERCEPTIONS OF PARTNER FEMININITY PREDICT INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN MEN S SENSITIVITY TO FACIAL CUES OF MALE DOMINANCE

LJMU Research Online

Middlesex University Research Repository: an open access repository of Middlesex University research

04/12/2014. Research Methods in Psychology. Chapter 6: Independent Groups Designs. What is your ideas? Testing

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Sensation seeking and men's face preferences

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Journal of Political Economy, Vol. 93, No. 2 (Apr., 1985)

THE EFFECT OF THE SHAPE OF EYEGLASSES ON JUDGEMENTS TOWARD WEARERS OCCUPATIONS -REPLICATION AND EXTENSION OF GUÉGUEN (2015)-

Peer review on manuscript "Multiple cues favor... female preference in..." by Peer 407

LJMU Research Online

PERFORMANCE OPERATING CHARACTERISTICS FOR SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DISCRIMINATIONS: COMMON OR SEPARATE CAPACITIES?

Ten recommendations for Osteoarthritis and Cartilage (OAC) manuscript preparation, common for all types of studies.

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing?

draw and interpret pedigree charts from data on human single allele and multiple allele inheritance patterns; e.g., hemophilia, blood types

CHAPTER-5. Family Disorganization & Woman Desertion by Socioeconomic Background

Psychology 305A: Lecture 7. Wrap up Biological Approach Begin Cognitive Perspective

The relative contribution of profile body shape and weight to judgements of women s physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia

MULTIPLE LINEAR REGRESSION 24.1 INTRODUCTION AND OBJECTIVES OBJECTIVES

Robert Farr Attitudes, social representations and social attitudes

Walking upright Specific changes in chewing design: teeth, jaws and skull. Homonoidea, Hominidae, Hominininae, Hominini, Hominina, Homo

LIMITATIONS OF FAMILY PLANNING PRACTICES AMONG WOMEN OF REPRODUCTIVE AGE IN OWAN WEST LOCAL GOVERNMENT AREA OF EDO STATE

Sheila Barron Statistics Outreach Center 2/8/2011

DUAL PROTECTION DILEMMA

Assortative mating for perceived facial personality traits

Relationships. Between Measurements Variables. Chapter 10. Copyright 2005 Brooks/Cole, a division of Thomson Learning, Inc.

CHAPTER - 6 STATISTICAL ANALYSIS. This chapter discusses inferential statistics, which use sample data to

UNIVERSITY OF THE FREE STATE DEPARTMENT OF COMPUTER SCIENCE AND INFORMATICS CSIS6813 MODULE TEST 2

1/19/2011. The New Science of Evolutionary Psychology. Three theories of origins of complex adaptive mechanisms

HW 3.2: page 193 #35-51 odd, 55, odd, 69, 71-78

King s Research Portal

Factors Related to Zimbabwe Women s Educational Needs in Agriculture. Anna E. Mudukuti, Ph.D. Zayed University

Bangor University Laboratory Exercise 1, June 2008

University of Huddersfield Repository

Social Norms Diagnostic Tool

This version was downloaded from Northumbria Research Link:

University of Huddersfield Repository

Economic Development Rural Women s Empowerment & Reproductive Health

Genetic basis of inheritance and variation. Dr. Amjad Mahasneh. Jordan University of Science and Technology

Proposed new target height equations for use in Australian growth clinics

Silent Partners: The Wives of Sleep Apneic Patients

Women's body morphology and preferences for sexual partners' characteristics

Selection at one locus with many alleles, fertility selection, and sexual selection

Mate Value of Romantic Partners Predicts Men s Partner-Directed Verbal Insults. Emily J. Miner and Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

Nature = biology - your genetic make up - determines the range of an individual s potential

Running Head: INSULTS AND MATE RETENTION

Evolutionary Psychology. by Elizabeth Anderson

Does Body Mass Index Adequately Capture the Relation of Body Composition and Body Size to Health Outcomes?

STA 3024 Spring 2013 EXAM 3 Test Form Code A UF ID #

[In press, Personality and Individual Differences, February 2008] Not all Men are Sexually Coercive:

Karri Silventoinen University of Helsinki and Osaka University

Repeated Measures ANOVA and Mixed Model ANOVA. Comparing more than two measurements of the same or matched participants

The Basic Cognition of Jealousy: An Evolutionary Perspective. Jon K. Maner. Florida State University. Todd K. Shackelford. Florida Atlantic University

The Relationship between Objective Sperm Competition Risk and Men s Copulatory Interest is. Moderated by Partner s Time Spent with Other Men

Hungry Men Prefer More Mature Women: A Field Test of the Environmental Security Hypothesis

International Journal of Health Sciences and Research ISSN:

BODY COMPOSITION: AN ANALYSIS BETWEEN THE FOOTBALLER AND THANG-TA PRACTITIONER OF MANIPUR

The Making of the Fittest: Natural Selection in Humans

UNCORRECTED PROOF ARTICLE IN PRESS

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

Social Structure and Interaction in Daily Life. Chapter 4

Infertility in Ethiopia: prevalence and associated risk factors

Mind the gap How inequality facilitates poorer social and health outcomes

41% HOUSEHOLD DECISIONMAKING AND CONTRACEPTIVE USE IN ZAMBIA. Research Brief. Despite Available Family Planning Services, Unmet Need Is High

University of Dundee. Statistical packages and clinical psychology research Peck, Dave; Dow, Mike; Goodall, William

The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs. Researching from Within: Moral and Ethical Issues and Dilemmas

National Culture Dimensions and Consumer Digital Piracy: A European Perspective

Test Validity. What is validity? Types of validity IOP 301-T. Content validity. Content-description Criterion-description Construct-identification

Intersexual Competition

EFFORT AND SCIENCE TO WIN

Day 11: Measures of Association and ANOVA

Evolutionary Psychology (Psych 459) Midterm Exam Winter 2010

A to Z OF RESEARCH METHODS AND TERMS APPLICABLE WITHIN SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH

Marriage Matters: Spousal Similarity in Life Satisfaction

STATISTICS 8 CHAPTERS 1 TO 6, SAMPLE MULTIPLE CHOICE QUESTIONS

DATA GATHERING METHOD

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli

Demography. Zimbabwe:

Lighten-up your Child s Growth

EPDs and Heterosis - What is the Difference?

King s Research Portal

Assessing Measurement Invariance in the Attitude to Marriage Scale across East Asian Societies. Xiaowen Zhu. Xi an Jiaotong University.

Transcription:

Rebecca Sear and Frank W. Marlowe How universal are human mate choices? Size doesn t matter when Hadza foragers are choosing a mate Article (Accepted version) (Refereed) Original citation: Sear, Rebecca and Marlowe, Frank W. (2009) How universal are human mate choices? Size doesn t matter when Hadza foragers are choosing a mate. Biology letters, 5 (5). pp. 606-609. DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0342 2009 Royal Society Publishing Ltd This version available at: http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/24375/ Available in LSE Research Online: October 2010 LSE has developed LSE Research Online so that users may access research output of the School. Copyright and Moral Rights for the papers on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Users may download and/or print one copy of any article(s) in LSE Research Online to facilitate their private study or for non-commercial research. You may not engage in further distribution of the material or use it for any profit-making activities or any commercial gain. You may freely distribute the URL (http://eprints.lse.ac.uk) of the LSE Research Online website. This document is the author s final manuscript accepted version of the journal article, incorporating any revisions agreed during the peer review process. Some differences between this version and the published version may remain. You are advised to consult the publisher s version if you wish to cite from it.

How universal are human mate choices? Size doesn t matter when Hadza foragers are choosing a mate Rebecca Sear Department of Social Policy, London School of Economics Houghton St, London WC2A 2AE, UK Corresponding author, email: r.sear@lse.ac.uk & Frank W. Marlowe Department of Anthropology, Florida State University 1847 West Tennessee St., Tallahassee, Florida 32306-7772, USA 1

Summary It has been argued that size matters on the human mate market: both stated preferences and mate choices have been found to be non-random with respect to height and weight. But how universal are these patterns? Most of the literature on human mating patterns is based on post-industrial societies. Much less is known about mating behaviour in more traditional societies. Here we investigate mate choice by analysing whether there is any evidence for non-random mating with respect to size and strength in a forager community, the Hadza of Tanzania. We test whether couples assort for height, weight, BMI, percent fat and grip strength. We test whether there is a male-taller norm. Finally, we test for an association between anthropometric variables and number of marriages. Our results show no evidence for assortative mating for height, weight, BMI or percent fat; no evidence for a male-taller norm; and no evidence that number of marriages is associated with our size variables. Hadza couples may assort positively for grip strength, but grip strength does not affect the number of marriages. Overall we conclude that, in contrast to post-industrial societies, mating appears to be random with respect to size in the Hadza. Keywords Mate choice, Hadza, height, strength, assortative mating, hunter-gatherers 2

Introduction Choosing the right mate is an important component of fitness, particularly so in a species like our own, where long-term relationships between partners are common. There is now a large body of research investigating mating patterns within evolutionary studies of human behaviour. The great majority of this research, however, has been conducted in a single cultural setting, the post-industrial West. This narrow focus becomes problematic when researchers generalise from post-industrial populations in order to draw conclusions about the significance of particular mating patterns in our evolutionary history, since it ignores the very real possibility that mating patterns will show ecological variation. For example, the importance of size as a cue has been emphasised in a number of recent publications. In post-industrial populations, size appears to matter for both mate preferences and mate choices. Laboratory studies of preferences suggest individuals take height into consideration when weighing up potential mates (Shepperd & Strathman1989; Higgins et al 2002). In the real world situations of mate advertising (in lonely hearts ads) and actual mate choice, height also appears to matter (Pawlowski & Koziel 2002). Similarly, weight is a factor in both stated preferences and observed choices (Silventoinen et al 2003; Kurzban & Weeden 2005). Despite these relationships having been demonstrated almost entirely in post-industrial societies, they have been used to bolster stories about the evolution of sexual dimorphism (Nettle 2002), and to explain intrasexual competition (Buunk et al 2008) and female character perception (Chu & Geary 2005). But if such mate preferences are contingent on ecology, they cannot explain the evolution of traits common to the entire species, nor can they be used to make generalised statements about the behaviour of all humans. 3

Here, we investigate mate choice in a forager society, the Hadza of Tanzania, to determine whether there is evidence that mating is non-random for anthropometric characteristics. Research in post-industrial societies suggests that positive assortative mating is common for height and weight (Silventoinen et al 2003), and that there is a male-taller norm, whereby marriages in which the female partner is taller than the male are much rarer than would be expected by chance (Gillis & Avis 1980; Pierce 1996). Taller men also appear to contract more marriages (Pawlowski et al 2000), though the reverse may be true for women (Nettle 2002). We investigate here whether there is any evidence for assortative mating, a male-taller norm, or that size affects the number of marriages contracted in the Hadza. Material & Methods Population The Hadza are hunter-gatherers who live in Tanzania in a savanna/woodland habitat. They number about 1000 and live in mobile camps that average 30 people. Women forage for plant foods and men hunt with bows and arrows. Women get married at around 17-18 years and men at age 20. There is no wedding ceremony but after a liaison a couple will begin sleeping at the same hearth and are considered married. Polyandrous marriages do not exist but about 4% of men have two concurrent wives. Because divorce is common, serial monogamy is the best way to characterize the mating system. Marriage is not arranged: the Hadza are extremely egalitarian so individuals are not coerced into marriage. Female choice seems to be the main factor leading to marriage, because young single men appear willing to marry a wide range of women. Previous research on the Hadza suggests their mate preferences resemble that of Western mate preferences in some ways but not others. For example, Hadza men have significantly different preferences for women s waist-to-hip ratio compared to 4

Western men (Marlowe et al 2005) and place much more emphasis on women being hard-working and fecund (Marlowe 2004). Though preferences for size have not been systematically tested in the Hadza, when asked which characteristics are attractive in a mate, very few individuals of either sex mention size: only 1 woman out of 46 questioned (2%) cited big as desirable; 1 of 46 women (2%) and 1 of 55 men (2%) preferred thinness; and only 2 men out of 55 (4%) mentioned that the woman should be shorter (Marlowe 2004). Data have been collected on the Hadza by FM since 1995. Our sample is close to the entire population of the Hadza, especially all those who are full-time foragers, since all individuals in each camp were measured during anthropometric surveys. Methods We analysed the evidence for non-random mating for the following anthropometric characteristics: height, weight, body mass index (BMI), percent fat and handgrip strength. Height and weight indicate absolute size; BMI is a measure of weight for height; percent fat, measured with a bioelectrical impedence scale (Tanita), is an indicator of body composition. We also examine grip strength, measured using a Smedley 3 Dynometer, as an indicator of overall strength and work capacity (Jones & Marlowe 2002). We performed the following analyses: Assortative mating: Linear regression analyses were performed to determine if there is any evidence for assortative mating for each anthropometric characteristic. The wife s characteristic was included as the dependent variable, and the husband s characteristic as an independent variable. Controls were included for age of both wife and husband and region of residence. 5

Male-taller norm: We investigated whether there is a male-taller norm by comparing the observed proportion of female-taller marriages (those in which the female partner is taller than the male) with the proportion of female-taller marriages expected from random mating. To determine the latter, we randomly coupled up women and men, and calculated the proportion of marriages in which the wife was taller than the husband in these randomly-mated pairs. We then performed a chi-squared test to determine whether the observed number of female-taller marriages was different from that expected from random mating. Number of marriages: Finally, we investigated whether anthropometric status is correlated with the total number of marriages contracted for each sex (during the 6 year period covered by the anthropometry data collection). The majority of individuals only have one spouse recorded (~80% of women and 70% of men), and few record more than two spouses (5% of women and 7% of men). So we performed logistic regressions to determine whether each anthropometric variable affected the probability of having multiple (>1) spouses, as opposed to one spouse. Each regression analysis controlled for the age and region of the individual; regressions were performed separately for men and women and separately for each anthopometric variable. Given we have performed multiple statistical tests for each sex for both assortative mating and the probability of multiple marriage (one for each anthropometric measure), for all analyses we assume a conservative alpha level of <0.01 to indicate statistical significance, based on a Bonferroni correction. 6

Results Assortative mating: Table 1 shows sample sizes and statistical output from the 5 linear regression models. The parameter estimates, SEs and significance values shown relate to the husband s characteristic (statistical output for control variables is not shown). These regression coefficients show no evidence for assortative mating for height, weight, BMI or percent fat. The grip strength of husbands and wives is correlated at the p<0.05 level (p=0.024). However, given that we are applying a correction for multiple tests, we would need a lower p value to be convinced that this is a real effect. Male-taller norm: We also find no evidence for a male-taller norm in this society. In 8.2% of all marriages the wife was taller than the husband (17 of 207 marriages). This is no different from the proportion of female-taller marriages expected by chance (8.8%: χ 2 = 0.089, df=1, p=0.765). Figure 1 shows the proportion of female-taller marriages in this Hadza population. RS has previously investigated this phenomenon in both a postindustrial society (UK) and a pre-industrial agricultural community in the Gambia, and the results of these analyses are also presented in Figure 1 for comparison (Sear 2006). The proportion of female-taller marriages in the UK is significantly lower than expected by chance, but the two traditional populations show a frequency of female-taller relationships no different from that expected from mating which is random with respect to height. Number of marriages: logistic regression analyses demonstrated no significant effect on the probability of multiple marriages for any anthropometric variable, for either sex (see Table 2). 7

Discussion These results suggest that mating is random with respect to size in this population. Such a conclusion is based on accepting that our null hypotheses are correct, so in order to provide some confidence in our conclusion we determined the power of our statistical tests, using G*Power 3 (Faul et al 2007). Post-hoc tests suggest that the power of our tests ranges from 95-97%, assuming a medium effect size (precise power varies by test: analysis provided on request). We conclude that we would have been able to detect size-related mating patterns in the Hadza, were they similar to those in industrialised populations. Despite the lack of evidence that size is important for mate choice, there is some weak evidence that strength may matter, given that the correlation between the strength of husbands and wives approached significance. Assortative mating may be brought about if both men and women prefer strong partners, in which case the most attractive (strongest) male is likely to end up paired with the most attractive (strongest) female, leading to a positive correlation for strength. Strength has been shown to correlate with work capacity in this population (Jones & Marlowe 2002), so couples may be positively assorting on productivity. An alternative possibility is that couples converge for strength after they marry. But men and women forage separately and largely for different food items in this population, so shared work patterns seem unlikely to lead to a convergence in strength. Overall, however, our analysis suggests size and strength are not greatly important when Hadza are choosing a mate. This lack of size-related mating patterns might appear surprising, since size is usually assumed to be an indicator of health, productivity and overall quality. But health and productivity may be signalled in alternative ways in the 8

Hadza, who are a small, relatively homogeneous population. An individual s health history may be more important than size, for example, and this may be relatively well known in a small, mobile population. Additionally, there may be some disadvantages to large size in food-limited societies, where the costs of maintaining large size during periods of food shortage may be high. Such disadvantages will not be seen in foodabundant societies, so that large size may be a better indicator of quality in postindustrial populations. Finally, research on another African forager population found that height is negatively correlated with hunting returns (Lee 1979), suggesting that tall height may not be an indicator of productivity in such economies. It is also important to note that we have not systematically tested mate preferences in this population, only mate choice. The distinction between mate preferences and mate choice is important. While mate preferences can give useful information about which traits are valued in a prospective mate, observed mate choices are arguably more informative about the evolutionary implications of mating behaviour. Mate choice takes into account the costs and constraints involved in acquiring a partner, and results from a combination of mate preferences (which may span a number of domains, not just physical cues), mate availability and one s own value on the mate market. These factors may assume different importance in different environments, just as mate preferences may differ between environments. We suggest that neither human mate preferences nor mate choices are likely to be identical across all populations, and that efforts should be made to test the universality of both preferences and choices before speculation is made on their evolutionary implications. With more data from a diverse array of populations we can begin to test hypotheses about why mating patterns should vary between environments and why some patterns might indeed be universal. It is time to expand our 9

horizon to a truly cross-cultural view and begin to sort between highly variable and truly universal mate patterns. 10

References Buunk, A. P., Park, J. H., Zurriaga, R., Klavina, L. and Massar, K. (2008) Height predicts jealousy differently for men and women. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29 (2): 133-139. Chu, S. and Geary, K. (2005) Physical stature influences character perception in women. Personal. Individ. Diff. 38 (8): 1927-1934. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G. & Buchner, A. (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Meth. 39 (2): 175-191. Gillis, J. S. and Avis, W. E. (1980) The male taller norm in mate selection. Personal. Soc. Psych. Bull. 6 (3): 396-401. Higgins L. T., Zheng M., Liu Y., Sun C. H. (2002) Attitudes to marriage and sexual behaviors: a survey of gender and culture differences in China and United Kingdom. Sex Roles 46 (3): 75-89. Jones, N. B., and F. W. Marlowe. (2002) Selection for delayed maturity: does it take 20 years to learn to hunt and gather? Hum. Nat. 13:199-238. Kurzban and Weeden (2005) HurryDate: Mate preferences in action. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26 (3): 227-244. Lee, R. B. 1979. The!Kung San: Men, Women, and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Marlowe, F. W. (2004) Mate preferences among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Hum. Nat. 15 (4): 365-376. Marlowe, F. W., C. L. Apicella, and D. Reed. (2005) Men's preferences for women's profile waist-hip-ratio in two societies. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26:458-468. 11

Nettle, D. (2002) Women's height, reproductive success and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in modern humans. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, Biol. Sci. 269 (1503): 1919-1923. Pawlowski, B., R. I. M. Dunbar, et al. (2000). Evolutionary fitness: tall men have more reproductive success. Nature 403(6766): 156. Pawlowski, B. and Koziel, S. (2002) The impact of traits offered in personal advertisements on response rates. Evol. Hum. Behav. 23 (2): 139-149. Pierce, C. A. (1996) Body height and romantic attraction: a meta-analytic test of the male-taller norm. Soc. Behav. Personal. 24 (2): 143-149. Sear, R. (2006) Height and reproductive success: how a Gambian population compares to the West. Hum. Nat. 17 (4): 405-418. Shepperd, J. A. and Strathman, A. J. (1989) Attractiveness and height: the role of stature in dating preference, frequency of dating, and perceptions of attractiveness. Personal. Soc. Psych. Bull. 15 (4): 617-627. Silventoinen, K., Kaprio, J., Lahelma, E., Viken, R. J. and Rose, R. J. (2003) Assortative mating by body height and BMI: Finnish twins and their spouses. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 15 (5): 620-627. 12

Table 1: descriptive data and statistical output for assortative mating analysis Mean (SD) for wives Mean (SD) for husbands Parameter estimate (SE) p N couples Age (yrs) 37.88 (15.80) 41.89 (14.86) 0.839 (0.033) <0.001 224 Height (cm) 149.66 (6.27) 162.24 (6.76) 0.103 (0.068) >0.05 206 Weight (kg) 45.51 (7.05) 53.20 (5.93) 0.138 (0.094) >0.05 206 BMI 20.25 (2.51) 20.14 (1.64) -0.027 (0.112) >0.05 206 Percent fat 18.99 (6.79) 11.17 (3.28) 0.143 (0.133) >0.05 192 Grip strength (kg) 19.92 (6.10) 31.69 (8.03) 0.137 (0.061) 0.024 185 13

Table 2: results of logistic regression analyses on probability of multiple marriages Men Women Parameter SE p N Parameter SE p N estimate estimate Height (cm) 0.011 0.026 p>0.05 167 0.021 0.029 p>0.05 236 Weight (kg) 0.036 0.032 p>0.05 167-0.018 0.026 p>0.05 236 BMI 0.080 0.105 p>0.05 167-0.096 0.072 p>0.05 236 Percent fat -0.012 0.054 p>0.05 158-0.021 0.027 p>0.05 222 Grip strength (kg) 0.007 0.026 p>0.05 158-0.004 0.033 p>0.05 219 14

Figure legend Figure 1: observed and expected proportion of female-taller marriages in the Hadza, compared with the Gambia and UK % marriages in which female is taller 15 12 9 6 3 0 Observed Expected NS NS p<0.001 Hadza Gambia UK 15